Discover millions of ebooks, audiobooks, and so much more with a free trial

Only $11.99/month after trial. Cancel anytime.

Riveted: The Science of Why Jokes Make Us Laugh, Movies Make Us Cry, and Religion Makes Us Feel One with the Universe
Riveted: The Science of Why Jokes Make Us Laugh, Movies Make Us Cry, and Religion Makes Us Feel One with the Universe
Riveted: The Science of Why Jokes Make Us Laugh, Movies Make Us Cry, and Religion Makes Us Feel One with the Universe
Ebook407 pages8 hours

Riveted: The Science of Why Jokes Make Us Laugh, Movies Make Us Cry, and Religion Makes Us Feel One with the Universe

Rating: 3 out of 5 stars

3/5

()

Read preview

About this ebook

Why do some things pass under the radar of our attention, but other things capture our interest? Why do some religions catch on and others fade away? What makes a story, a movie, or a book riveting? Why do some people keep watching the news even though it makes them anxious?
The past 20 years have seen a remarkable flourishing of scientific research into exactly these kinds of questions. Professor Jim Davies' fascinating and highly accessible book, Riveted, reveals the evolutionary underpinnings of why we find things compelling, from art to religion and from sports to superstition. Compelling things fit our minds like keys in the ignition, turning us on and keeping us running, and yet we are often unaware of what makes these "keys" fit. What we like and don't like is almost always determined by subconscious forces, and when we try to consciously predict our own preferences we're often wrong. In one study of speed dating, people were asked what kinds of partners they found attractive. When the results came back, the participants' answers before the exercise had no correlation with who they actually found attractive in person! We are beginning to understand just how much the brain makes our decisions for us: we are rewarded with a rush of pleasure when we detect patterns, as the brain thinks we've discovered something significant; the mind urges us to linger on the news channel or rubberneck an accident in case it might pick up important survival information; it even pushes us to pick up People magazine in order to find out about changes in the social structure.Drawing on work from philosophy, anthropology, religious studies, psychology, economics, computer science, and biology, Davies offers a comprehensive explanation to show that in spite of the differences between the many things that we find compelling, they have similar effects on our minds and brains.

LanguageEnglish
Release dateAug 5, 2014
ISBN9781137438140

Read more from Jim Davies

Related to Riveted

Related ebooks

Biology For You

View More

Related articles

Related categories

Reviews for Riveted

Rating: 2.92500001 out of 5 stars
3/5

40 ratings20 reviews

What did you think?

Tap to rate

Review must be at least 10 words

  • Rating: 2 out of 5 stars
    2/5
    I was really looking forward to reading this. The book is about how the 'new brain' and the 'old brain' work together to create memories and emotions However I have been unable to complete it. I've started several times and each time I get bogged down, find myself re-reading and missing the comprehension. Personally, I found the writing style choppy. It didn't flow well. The information might be accurate, but I feel the author was writing more for a professional reader and not a lay reader. That's a shame because it was a fascinating topic. I don't lay books down easily. This one didn't work for me.
  • Rating: 3 out of 5 stars
    3/5
    I liked this book, probably because I really wanted to like this book, but it has a few problems. It reads like somebody's dissertation research that has been watered down for a general audience. I would have rather read the dissertation, because the author continually makes claims that need citations and don't have them. The author also relies on a few neuroscience concepts that were once widely believed and have since been disproven - the idea that the right side of the brain controls the left side of our bodies and vice versa, for example. (It's actually much, much more complicated than that.) So the book felt outdated in that regard - for a scientific book about the brain, I expect a more updated use of the science.That said, I liked the author's tone - he made me laugh out loud a few times. If you disagree with his biases, I could understand how you might be put off, but overall I thought it was an okay book. If you're really interested in the science, there are much better books to read, and more accurate ones to boot.
  • Rating: 1 out of 5 stars
    1/5
    Riveted I was not. While the premise of the book was very interesting, the book itself was just plain boring. It read like a science journal for a required science class I had no interest in taking. Each chapter was its own topic and could be read in no particular order if you wanted to. I also felt like a lot of what he was trying to prove never actually got proven.
  • Rating: 3 out of 5 stars
    3/5
    I was not exactly riveted by Riveted. While Jim Davies offers many interesting facts that help explain why we are compelled as human beings to act/react in certain ways, the book seemed to lack a cohesiveness to me. While I can see why Michael Shermer compares Davis to Malcom Gladwell in so far as subject mateers goes, Davies just doesn't have that interesting storytelling capability to keep the readers interest that Gladwell posses. Overall an interesting book and interesting ideas just a bit of a drag to read.
  • Rating: 2 out of 5 stars
    2/5
    Interesting tidbits strung together in a haphazard way do not a riveting read make. I referred to the table of contents repeatedly to try to visualize a structure for Davies' thoughts, but what I read just didn't hold together. After about halfway I put it down for a few weeks, thinking it might be I wasn't in the proper mood. When I came back to it, I stopped blaming myself. The book's description and jacket blurbs sound like just the kind of reading I'd enjoy, but I had a hard time getting through this book. I can imagine telling friends about this or that interesting fact I learned from RIVETED, but I can't imagine a single person to whom I would recommend reading it all the way through. I received this book through Library Thing's Early Review program. I am grateful that this program exists, and that authors and publishers participate. I hope these comments do not work to the detriment of the program.
  • Rating: 3 out of 5 stars
    3/5
    Riveted is a readable book that describes current cognitive research aimed at understanding how people think. The topics range from discussions of people's bias to seeing patterns to learning that British prime ministers have higher IQs than US presidents.A significant portion of the book explains religion's place in human thought, and might be better titled 'tips to designing a religion your brain will accept.' In some places the author sprinkles in theories along with the research, blurring the scientific basis of the observation. Still, there are nuggets here for everyone interested in the brain and its functions.
  • Rating: 1 out of 5 stars
    1/5
    I found it ironic that a book called Riveted was so incredibly dry and boring. I tried to be interested - I really did. But the tiny font, lack of story or cohesive organization was too much. He could have easily added stories, jokes and anecdotes, taking a page from his own research. If a book on writing is poorly written, it hurts the author's credibility because they haven't applied what they teach. The same is true here, and it's difficult to ignore.I don't deny there may be some grains of knowledge here that are worth learning, but it's a sparse harvest.
  • Rating: 1 out of 5 stars
    1/5
    “Riveted” by Jim Davies, is supposed to be a popularization of a scientific theory. It may work for some people, but not for me. Its continual use of anecdotal “evidence”, post hoc proptor hoc arguments, and lists of probably unrelated studies takes the book out of the realm of scientific discourse. There are too many parts of his theory which need to be proved, lest it become the pseudo-science or myth-making he claims to oppose. He separates the brain in a two part division of the mind into the “old mind” [which he does not call the limbic system, although that is the accepted terminology] and the “new brain” [which he does not call the frontal lobes of the the neocortex, also accepted]. He ignores the rest of the brain and the known uses of the parts of the mind are nowhere near as clear-cut as he stipulates. From there we go to his “compellingness foundation” which appear to be totally anecdotal. I was actually offended by some of his arguments, and went to his sources, where possible. They did not always agree with him.When his writing is not confusing, it is simplistic and not convincing. His continual use of phrases such as “I created a concept” or “I conjecture that” show how baseless this book is. No straight hypothesis, just a bunch or random possibilities which might eventually add up to one. Or two or three hypotheses. No apparent testing. No proof. Not worth the trouble clawing through.
  • Rating: 5 out of 5 stars
    5/5
    When I pick up psychology/science books like “Riveted” that are intended for a broad audience, I always worry that I’m going to be disappointed by fluffy, dumbed down, generalizing text. I was happy to find that “Riveted” is actually pretty “dense” and specific—it depends heavily on scientific research, gives detailed explanations, has a ton of keywords/concepts like you’d get from a textbook, looks at each idea from different angles, and even includes discussion of a few studies that contradict points the author was making for comprehensiveness and transparency. The writing itself had a nice conversational tone with some occasional humor thrown in. Overall, a very informative, fascinating and fun read.The tagline mentions religion, jokes and movies, but a wide variety of things, like books, quotes, ideas, conspiracy theories, art, sports and music are also covered. The basic theme was that we find things compelling because of biological/evolutionary impulses and psychological biases. This is extremely useful to understand. When presented with ideas or situations, you can take these impulses and biases into account and evaluate your reactions and thoughts more objectively, which leads to better decision making. I only have one complaint. It seemed like a disproportionally large chunk of the book was devoted to analyzing religion. While I found the religion parts interesting, and I feel like I have a much better appreciation of why religions come into existence and why people are so dedicated to them, I wish there could have been more emphasis on other things. (Religion is a huge part of our culture, so maybe that’s why it was given so much space in the book?) Note: I received an advanced reading copy of this book in exchange for an honest review.
  • Rating: 3 out of 5 stars
    3/5
    So unfortunately I waited too long and forgot to take good notes on this book. But I'm going to do my best. The big idea of Riveted is to ask why some things are captivating and others are not. Why some get sucked into a song that leaves others cold. The basic foundational principals Jim Davies uses to construct his argument are the assumption that humans are interested in other humans, we give special interest to facts we hope or fear, humans are pattern seeking, which in turn means we are compelled by incongruity, the structure of our senses is fundamental to our reaction to the world, and there are core psychological structures built into the structure of our brain that shape what we are inclined to believe or disbelieve.The question of compellingness is itself rather compelling. Marketers spend a lot of time wondering about how to manipulate it. Each person spends a decent amount of time considering what they find compelling when making a large purchase, and ultimately we spend time trying to pick from the myriad of possibilities in our daily life what we think will ultimately be most compelling in the long term.At least based on what stood out to me I'd say the strongest case for what will be compelling is made for items that encourage us to find a pattern and that which disrupts an expected pattern. Repetition in games feeds the desire to observe regularity. But a misplaced object can cause a unquenchable disease even if a person doesn't quite understand what is causing it. The other dimensions all are given a good case too but these seem to touch on some of the most fundamental reasons why something would be compelling. There are some questionable claims that a reader would have to dive into to figure out if are true. But that is pretty much a given for pop science books. The thesis is also a bit all over the place as far as ultimate unifying principal but overall Jim Davies does a good job presenting his concept and it was an enjoyable read. If you are interested in this sort of area I'd say give it a read.

    1 person found this helpful

  • Rating: 4 out of 5 stars
    4/5
    This book is a study of what makes things interesting in art, sport and other subjects. Davies splits the book up into topics like patterns, biology and incongruity and then links them all up at some point to show relationships between topics and make sense of it all.
  • Rating: 3 out of 5 stars
    3/5
    Riveted isn't bad work, per se, but it's disappointing. If Davies' thesis is the irrationality of religious faith, then there's too much other material in the book, and that material is distracting (not to mention the book cover and blurbs are misleading). That thesis would be more than enough to carry the book, and there's enough material here on that topic to fill an entire book. But Davies either didn't think that was sufficient or felt the need to be deliberately misleading. If his thesis is meant to be broader--to actually support this "compellingness foundations theory" that he puts forth--then he was himself seduced by the anti-religion argument and simply lets it run away with him. (I suspect the latter is the case.) The book is also extremely broken up and lacking in transitions. I've read a lot of academic scholarly work as well as a lot of academics writing for a general audience, and Davies needed an editor (or at least an English grad student) to make his work more palatable for the general public as his writing style is choppy in places and his tone inconsistent. Short story: if you want to read an erudite argument against theism, read Christopher Hitchens; if you want to read fascinating cultural analysis that synthesizes research from a number of different fields, read Malcolm Gladwell (or James Glick). Davies is too much of both and not enough of either.
  • Rating: 3 out of 5 stars
    3/5
    The introduction starts by introducing the functions of the "old brain" and the "new brain", and what they bring to the table. He then introduces his "Compellingness Foundations Theory" (not a particularly riveting name), which is composed of 6 aspects. These are the subjects of the first 6 chapters: People, Hope & Fear, Patterns, Incongruity, Biological Nature, and Psychological nature. The final, 7th chapter, pulls them together. At the end are the extensive footnotes. I admit, I had hoped to be more compelled by the book. It feels like a good first cut at the subject, but it is not itself riveting.
  • Rating: 4 out of 5 stars
    4/5
    I suspect that there are a lot people don't like the author's voice in this book - it is a bit snarky, a bit condescending, especially towards religion. But once you get around the voice - you find an excellent book that explains how your brain uses information that it receives to make a view of the world. Also, there is a lot of discussion about how people are very much a thing of evolution, even down to how we create our landscaping. This can be quite disconcerting... even for somebody like me, who sees the hint of truth in it, but likes to believe that my choices are intentional. The author is skilled at taking complex ideas, and summing them up for a lay person. I think it helps to have a small understanding of the brain before going into this book. I like the idea of the old brain vs the new brain. All (as far as I can tell) sources are extensively cited, although he will add additional comments inside the source - which I found rather tedious - because I couldn't tell if reading a citation would add more to the story and because each chapter starts anew from citation number one, they were difficult to find quickly. It would have been better to put only citation in the back, and use the asterisk at the bottom of the page for additional comments.Overall - this is a book I will reread again - or maybe read later editions of the book if the author updates this book with newer information.To summarize - this book will make you question how you make decisions and what you believe. Even if you don't agree with everything in the book (and you won't) it will make you think.
  • Rating: 3 out of 5 stars
    3/5
    I received this book through LibraryThing's early reviewer program, and found it an interesting read. The author's premise of a "compellingness foundations theory" is an interesting one, and he explores different aspects of his theory throughout the chapters. Some are readily believable, others less so, but all in all he raises some interesting questions. Unfortunately, the questions sometimes undermine the premise of his theory. The author acknowledges the lack of scientific study in some aspects of his theory, so this feels more like a starting place than a fully realized, thoughtfully tested theory. Perhaps hypothesis is the better term for the author's ideas, although they certainly merit further thought and research.
  • Rating: 4 out of 5 stars
    4/5
    In Riveted, author and professor Jim Davies takes us on a journey through the mind, though not so much the chemical makeup of the mind, but rather seeing the mind manifest itself in every day human nature.
  • Rating: 4 out of 5 stars
    4/5
    In an effort to understand human behavior the author examines five principles from our subconscious or biology that compel humans to respond and suggests these "compellingness foundations" make something riveting. The information is well presented and researched, even including studies that do not support a given point. The title is a little misleading since a there is a disproportionate focus on arguing science is a truer body of knowledge than religion.I would agree but while the author does admit to some shortcomings of science, he was less rigorous in pointing out how often science is influenced by the same subconscious compellingness foundations as religion.
  • Rating: 4 out of 5 stars
    4/5
    Some books are annoying because the author writes too much out of his own experience and doesn’t draw on science. This book doesn’t have that problem; Davies does the research. Oh my, yes, Davies has done the research. He quotes at least two studies on every page of this book. So that’s not the problem.I was annoyed by the loose organization at the book. I loved all the fascinating research Davies shares, but I was never very clear where he was going with his findings. My take: this would have been a much stronger book, a more (bear with me) riveting book, if you will, had Davies spent more time editing it.
  • Rating: 4 out of 5 stars
    4/5
    This book made me very angry. That anger did not come from any of the particular points that were made, although there will be ideas that upset people with certain world views and mindsets. Rather, my anger was actually rooted in disappointment. I felt I was reading one of the best science/informational books I've read in long time. And everything – all the value, all the insights, all the really good stuff – was subverted by the occasional inclusion of content that did not match the high standards I saw everywhere else in the book. According to the book's cover, this is about "the science of why jokes make us laugh, movies make us cry, and religion makes us feel one with the universe." Davies fulfills that promise and more. The accomplishment of providing so much valuable information was why I wanted to like it so much. But throughout the book there were jarring moments where rigor seemed to take a backseat.And then there is the last chapter. But before explaining just what went wrong, let's talk about how good this book almost was.Riveted contains some of the most interesting information I have seen in a long time. Jim Davies has done an excellent job of pulling together an incredibly large and diverse group of sources to attempt an explanation for why humans react the way we do. All of this is in support of his "compellingness foundations theory". (And, if I may be allowed to foreshadow a little, that may be at the root of much of the problem – we seem to have a partially constructed theory here; one that is waiting for further proof.)He brings together the surprising commonalities that exist in areas and concepts such as art, religion, sports, superstitions – the plethora of life's aspects that we take for granted. However, by not taking them for granted – by questioning why it is we react the way we do – he provides insight into the basic makeup of our humanity. He is actually addressing a question as big as "what makes us human" and is able to put together answers that are understandable, insightful, and the type that should give every reader pause as he or she begins to look more closely at his or her own actions.It is easy to read, it is engaging, and it is revelatory. Which just made me that much madder. It is a book that deserves to be flawless or, at the very least, less prone to "flawfulness".I ran into my first issue with the book within the first chapter. And it continued throughout. I kept being brought up short as I stumbled across data, discussions, and conjectures that seemed to be made based on "non-rigorous" information. In some instances circumstantial evidence was used to support concepts. For example, to support the conjecture that people dominate artistic depiction, he reports the results from an art history student's review of the number of people depicted in each individual work of art found in a specific art history book. A wonderful start, but far from proof of anything, except that Davies either needed quick support for an idea or something easy for the reader to grasp. In another part of the book, he begins a description with the phrase "In an interesting but unscientific study..." How is the reader supposed to use this information?In some cases suppositions are made that I found questionable. As an example: "Science fiction, fairy tales, and fantasy genres tend to have less developed characters." Of course you can find lots of examples in these three different genres where character takes a back seat. But you can do the same for any genre. Sturgeon's Law states that 90% of anything stinks (paraphrasing). So it is not inconceivable that the majority of science fiction may come up short in the area of character. But, to indicate science fiction suffers from a disease not suffered elsewhere is to ignore the majority of all fiction that has similar limitations. Davies has made a broad-based statement that seems to be based on "feel" rather than rigorously vetted information.I also lost track of the number of times I read something to the effect of "My conjecture is..." or "My theory predicts that..." followed by the hope that, in the future, research will support what is about to follow. (Note that in some instances even this disclaimer was not included; instead, just stating that he thinks the following is true.) Again, what is the reader supposed to make of this? Is this a book about what is known or about what Davies hopes will be proven true?This book is well researched. Why does it have to be hamstrung by these lapses?I will note that Davies seems to acknowledge this particular issue. As he notes "the book is super lumpy", going on to indicate that he is describing "a possible research program". The result is a book that seems to be for scientists ("Hey, go look over here") and laymen ("Look at all this cool stuff") and, in the process, does not serve either particularly well.And now about that last chapter. Why is it that scientists seem to have this overpowering desire to speak up about religion? Throughout the book Davies includes discussions of religion and how we may be hardwire to accept it. I am fine with that; it is part of the entire discussion of why we appreciate art, etc. But then along comes the final chapter – "Why We Get Riveted" – which purports to be a summary of what has come before. Instead, after some warm up about celebrity worship (Davies' word choice) the chapter becomes devoted (an unintentionally ironic word choice on my part) to discussion on why we care about religion. Now, the chapter does not come right out and say that religion is a bad thing. But there is an underlying tone that implies that being a religious person is not necessarily the best choice.It is veiled, but it feels that there is a touch of proselytizing here. And it is a dirty thing to put at the end of a potentially excellent book.I have spent an inordinate amount of time, words, and pixels on this book. And a lot of it has been spent on lambasting it for the problems I saw. I am not a believer in criticizing just to throw more words out there. That kind of critical review is not worth the time. But, again, this book held so much promise that these shortcomings became even more glaring. Ultimately I am torn on whether this book should be recommended or not. I guess, because of the incredible value I found (it is a resource I know I will be returning to time and again), I would say it is worth having. Just know that such a recommendation comes with a caveat – that warning being all those words I've used above.
  • Rating: 3 out of 5 stars
    3/5
    I had high expectations when I read the blurb on the dust jacket … in which another author writes, "Jim Davies could be the next Malcolm Gladwell.” My first thought was that we didn’t need a “next” Malcolm Gladwell, as the current one is alive, well and still writing as far as I know. My final thought, after finishing Riveted, was, “Jim Davies is no Malcolm Gladwell."Whereas, Riveted seems to me like a thousand factoids in search of an idea, Mr. Gladwell’s books involve actual storytelling … and building a coherent case that non-expert readers can understand along with the author. I’m sure Mr. Davies is a brilliant person, and Riveted is for other brilliant people who share his vocabulary, not us mere mortals. I’m not saying Riveted is a terrible book because it gave me few aha moments along the way. I’m just saying that I was NOT riveted by Riveted.Review based on publisher-provided copy of Riveted.

Book preview

Riveted - Jim Davies

Riveted

Riveted

The Science of

Why Jokes Make Us Laugh,

Movies Make Us Cry, and

Religion Makes Us Feel

One with the Universe

Jim Davies

New_Logo_large_R--conv.tif

The author and publisher have provided this e-book to you for your personal use only. You may not make this e-book publicly available in any way. Copyright infringement is against the law. If you believe the copy of this e-book you are reading infringes on the author’s copyright, please notify the publisher at: us.macmillanusa.com/piracy.

Contents

Introduction

1: Hardwiring for Socializing

2: Wizard’s First Rule: Hope and Fear’s Anti–Sweet Spot

3: The Thrill of Discovering Patterns

4: Incongruity: Absurdism, Mystery, and Puzzle

5: Our Biological Nature

6: Our Psychological Biases

7: Why We Get Riveted

More Sources of Interestingness

Acknowledgments

Notes

Index

Introduction

In my first year of graduate school I worked in a condemned building. Sitting in my office was a woman I was trying to impress. We were talking about dance music. She liked club music and techno; I liked rap. I put on an acid jazz album in the compact disc player.

How can you dance to this? she asked.

"How can you not dance to this?" I replied, and then demonstrated the irresistibility of the track. For the most part, I only want to listen to music that makes me want to dance. Sorry, John Denver.

When I was young I was fascinated with psychic powers. I read every book in the libraries of both my elementary and high school on the subject, and was convinced that people had untapped mental abilities. All these books in the nonfiction section of my school’s library told me that people could move things with their minds, scry with crystal balls, and predict the future. We used only 10 percent of our brains, right? What else could that 90 percent possibly be for?

I was absolutely captivated by this idea and convinced of it, until I read Susan Blackmore’s sobering In Search of the Light: Adventures of a Parapsychologist in college.¹ It was the first skeptical book I’d encountered and it scorched and salted the lush landscape of my paranormal beliefs. First Santa Claus and now this? Ideas can be beautiful and we don’t want to let go of them even when we know they’re wrong.

There are things in this world that deeply resonate with us. We seek them out. They hold our attention. They feel right. I want to dance to hip-hop. I feel moved by sad, uplifting stories. I want to believe that people can move things just by willing it to happen.

You are struck by a beautiful view from a mountain cabin. You hear that everyone gets the afterlife that they imagined they’d have, and the idea is so beautiful, and feels so right, that you smile in spite of yourself. You hear a story of some terrible thing that happened to a child that gives you chills and haunts you for days. You find yourself glued to the screen, watching a close basketball game. You hear a great joke and can’t wait to tell it to your friends.

With the huge variety of things we find compelling, it seems natural that a huge variety of qualities would make them compelling. There can’t be anything similar about what’s good about a pop song on the radio and what’s moving when someone recounts their near-death experience, can there?

Yes, there can. Strange as it might seem, compelling things share many similarities. My purpose in this book is to tie together research from many fields. I’ll do something that has never been done before and show how all these phenomena can be explained with the foundations of compellingness. I will show you that, like art and other sensory experiences, beliefs and explanations have aesthetic qualities that make us more or less likely to believe them. The same qualities appear again and again in riveting things, be they jokes, paintings, quotations, paranormal beliefs, religions, sports, video games, news, music, or gossip. The qualities that are common to all these things fit like a key in a lock with our psychological proclivities. I call it the compellingness foundations theory.

* * *

Understanding compellingness and how it works requires some understanding of our brains and how they were shaped by evolution. Our brains are a mix of old and newer processes that evolved at different times. They sometimes disagree on the meaning, importance, and value of things, and often we are clueless as to how we got our opinions. Often we are attracted to something or repelled by it and don’t know why, and the reasons we dredge up are confabulations, mere guesses about our underlying psychologies.

The old brain is evolutionarily older. It’s located near the top of the brain stem and the back of the head. We share much of its anatomy with other animals. It’s a Rube Goldberg contraption, with special rules for this and not for that, all evolved, rather haphazardly, to help us survive and reproduce. It consists of a hodgepodge of specialized systems.

In the front of our head is the new brain, which is a general-purpose learning and reasoning machine and a system that tries to control the impulses of the older brain. It’s a slow, deliberate planner and imaginer. Jazz improvisers quiet this part of their brain before performing.² This part of the brain is not built to do specific jobs; rather it’s built to learn to do, well, just about anything. Where the old brain looks different depending on where you look, the new brain (particularly the cerebral cortex), looks remarkably similar no matter where you look. We have an old brain for the same reasons all animals do. We have the new brain because our ancestors got into an intelligence arms race with each other.

Because the old and new brains think with different rules, care about different things, and might even use different stores of knowledge, they often come up with different evaluations of the same situations. For example, there is a famous moral-reasoning experiment run by psychologist Joshua Greene that asks whether or not it is morally acceptable to pull a switch that will cause a train to kill one person rather than five (this is a version of a problem first proposed by Philippa Foot in 1967). Most people answer yes, such an action is acceptable, which indicates relatively high activation in the newer, more frontal areas. More emotionally salient problems, such as a version of the same problem that would require the pushing of a single person onto a track to save five people, show activation in the emotional, older parts of the brain. In this kind of scenario, where there is direct physical contact involved, people often report that doing so is morally unacceptable.³

When the new brain pulls in the opposite direction from the old, you can literally be of two minds about something. For example, your new brain can know that prepackaged cupcakes are unhealthful, but your old brain can be quite insistent that they should be devoured. Many of them. With a cold glass of milk, please. The old brain knows that sugar and fat are scarce and should always be eaten when the opportunity arises. Thousands of years of evolution taught it that. It doesn’t know that fat, sugar, and salt are now plentiful and contributing to an obesity problem in the industrialized world. In contrast, the new brain knows that too much sugar isn’t good for you. But who are you going to listen to? (While we’re asking, who are you?)

The new brain knows things because it learned them. In this case, each one of us has to learn things that run counter to what the old brain knows from evolution. Such is the source of many internal mental conflicts. The old and new have a tug of war. The new brain thinks more logically, in a step-by-step way. The old brain is not deliberative; it is intuitive. Sometimes we can get a strong, immediate sense that something is immoral. This is the old brain’s influence. Then, when asked to explain it, we must engage our new brains, which struggle, often unsuccessfully, to apply what we believe about morality to justify the feeling. This is what moral psychologist Jonathan Haidt called moral dumbfounding. Similarly, when choosing an immediate versus a delayed reward, the emotional part of the brain—the old brain—is active when thinking about the immediate reward, as are the frontal areas—the new brain—for the delayed reward. It could be that because we have some control over the activity of our frontal areas, we can use it to override the emotional areas when we need to. The everyday term for this would be resisting temptation.

Because the old brain is better at context and the new brain better at rules, there is evidence that people with low working memory (people who can’t remember long strings of numbers, for example) are actually better at some tasks that require old-brain function, particularly complex categorization tasks, as shown in a study by psychologist Maci DeCaro.⁴ The new brain uses its laser focus to attend to only a few attributes of a situation. The old brain, in contrast, has a more diffuse focus, giving lots of information more or less equal weight.

We often personally identify with the workings of our new brain. When we see a scary monster depicted in a movie, we say we don’t believe that the monster is there. However, as philosopher Tamar Szabó Gendler points out, some part of our brains must believe it’s there, or we wouldn’t get scared at all.

What does this have to do with compellingness? Often the evaluations by our old brain make us prefer certain ideas and experiences. In general, its processes are black boxes, inaccessible to consciousness.

Intuition can feel like a burst of insight that comes from nowhere. What is really happening is that unconscious processes generate judgments and feelings that bubble up to consciousness fully formed. For example, you might get a feeling of danger or trust. You are aware of their outputs, but you can’t look inside yourself to see how they work. If your mind is like an ocean, your conscious mind is just the surface of the water. Because you don’t know what causes these feelings in your own head, it can feel like divine intervention or psychic ability.

We call these unconscious convictions and feelings intuition.

Should you trust your intuition? These unconscious processes either evolved or were learned for some reason or another, and you often cannot tell in the moment whether those reasons apply to your current situation. If you justify the feeling with reasons, those reasons are a confabulation that your conscious mind has invented.

It is not clear what the relationship is between trusting your intuition and belief in the paranormal and in conspiracy theories. However, a study by psychologist Matthew Boden showed that people who trust their hunches were more likely to have ideas of reference, which are beliefs that things in the world relate directly to them—such as that someone got on the elevator because they were in it or that the raindrops are trying to send them a message.⁶ Ideas of reference are common symptoms of mental illnesses such as mania and schizophrenia.

* * *

People have an understandable desire to comprehend the world around them. At its best, news holds people, governments, and other groups accountable for trespasses, and it is a purveyor of important information. At its worst, news plays to our hopes and fears, terrifying us without justification. Psychologists Hank Davis and S. Lyndsay McLeod conducted a large study that asked people to sort news stories published between 1700 and 2001 into classifications of their own choosing. The 12 categories that emerged corresponded to things we have evolved to find important, such as reputation, treatment of offspring, good deeds, violence, and sexual assault.⁷ News has always been sensational. As terrorism expert Scott Atran says, Media and publicity are the oxygen of terrorism. Without them, it would die.⁸ Reports of terrorism increase fear and anger, and lessen feelings of forgiveness, found a study by psychologist Alice Healy. Watching nonfiction crime documentaries makes people think the national crime rate is increasing and makes them less confident in the criminal justice system. People who watch less TV are more accurate judges of risks.⁹ Unfortunately, we are most likely to remember the least likely events.

What can our desire to know things about the world, and about people, tell us about our interest in fiction?

Finding Nemo is an animated movie about a father clownfish trying to find his son who gets lost. I remember marveling at the fact that this film brought tears to my eyes within five minutes. Let’s examine the drastic differences between watching this film and witnessing a real-life tragic event. First, one is watching a film, not actual, physical people. Second, it’s about filmed fish, not filmed people. Third, the fish are animated by computer programs. That is, it is not a film recording of real fish. Fourth, it’s not even about real fish—the characters are completely fictional. Reflect for a moment on the absurdity of somebody (me) crying over a screen representing fictional fish spawned by computer graphics.

Long ago, our ancestors saw very few things that were not what they appeared to be. Some notable exceptions would be mirages, reflections of objects in liquid, dreams, echoes, and how a stick appears to bend when placed in water. But most of the time, if you saw a person, there was actually a person there. A sense’s proper domain is that which it was designed (by evolution) to understand. But the actual domain is anything that triggers it. In contrast, the modern world constantly exposes us to images and sounds depicting things that are not right in front of us. We are exposed to people, but also representations of people that we substitute for the real thing. When we see someone on television say she likes ice cream, we believe that the actual person represented likes ice cream, not merely her televised image. We hear voices of people who are not present on the radio, on podcasts, and on the phone. We see events and people animated on screens. We see photos in magazines and on billboards. Of course the photo is real, but the image of the person in the photo is not. It might be that fully seeing nonreal things as they truly are—for example, seeing a picture of a woman only as a piece of paper and ink—is, in the long run, evolutionarily adaptive, just as it might be worth a designer’s time to try to get the grocery store door not to open for dogs. The door was designed to open for people (the proper domain) but opens for dogs too (part of the actual domain). Environments, however, be they cultural, technological, or natural, can change so fast that evolution can’t keep up. Photography was developed so recently that there simply has not been enough time for evolution to make the older parts of our mind good at distinguishing, say, actual people from pictures of people.

This contrast between what you know is true and what you perceive has a similar effect in optical illusions. We experience one thing, but in some sense know that the experience isn’t real. Consider this effect for a moment, because its meaning is profound and its ramifications great. Although we all have experience with optical illusions, it’s important to appreciate that we have no control over their effects on us. We know that the lines are the same length, but we still perceive them to be different. Optical illusions are designed to put your brain in this befuddled state of affairs. Your early visual system is being tricked and no amount of convincing from your deliberate thinking processes can make the visual system interpret it otherwise.

A similar effect is occurring whenever you perceive representations in any art form. Your old brain (and your perceptual areas) believes that what you’re experiencing is real and often your new brain knows better at the same time.

Films are multimedia experiences that at least appear to depict real people, in that the light patterns that play on our retinas resemble the patterns that we’d get if we were looking at real people. But literature is even more mysterious in that we can become fully engaged when we are being exposed merely to words. Language-based narrative arts have an even greater chance to capture our attention than visual arts, because language is devastatingly effective at communicating the nuances of social changes, mental states, and relationships. Here are six words (sometimes attributed to Hemingway) that I’m sure will stir the emotions of a good percentage of readers: For sale. Baby shoes. Never worn. How can six words pack such a punch? Philosophers call our emotional response to fictional characters the paradox of fiction. Why would the part of our minds that wants to understand our actual peers get excited when reading a book or looking at what is obviously a static painting?

We’ll begin by looking at why people engage in oral storytelling, the likely precursor to literature. One of the fabulous things about language is that it allows us to communicate the lessons we’ve learned. Without communication, learning can only occur through direct personal experience or direct observation of others. Unfortunately, some important lessons, such as the dangers of wandering into enemy territory or the deadliness of getting caught in front of an avalanche, are costly to learn by trial and error, as the learner is likely to die during or shortly after the event they would be learning from. However, with language, a lesson learned by a single individual can be communicated to a great number of people. Not only can a lone surviving individual tell her tale, but others can tell it at second and third hand. Lessons can be communicated throughout an entire culture, sometimes lasting generations. It is likely that the communication of wisdom was the original function of storytelling. As philosopher of art Denis Dutton eloquently put it, stories provide a low-cost, low-risk surrogate experience.¹⁰

Modern humans can tell and appreciate fictional stories, be they crafted entertainment or outright lies. However, we still learn from and take to heart these made-up stories because we’ve evolved, culturally or genetically, to do so, often without even intending to. Psychologists Elizabeth Marsh and Lisa Fazio ran a study that found that people do not spontaneously keep an eye out for falsehoods.¹¹

If we don’t know whether a story we hear is truth or fiction, or some combination thereof, perhaps we will believe some part of it, just in case it contains some useful information. For example, a story about a bear attack might make us wary of approaching an actual bear, whether we know the story to be true or not. What about stories that we know are fiction, either because they are explicitly labeled as such, or, like The Wonderful Wizard of Oz, because they are simply too fantastic to believe? Although each of us feels as though our own mind were a single entity, under the hood of consciousness different parts of our brain fight for their points of view like argumentative parliament members. In the case of fiction, the old brain might believe the story even if the new brain doesn’t.

* * *

In this book I present a unified explanation of compellingness. Riveting things appeal to the foundations of compellingness. Those foundations are:

We are interested in people. We love to see people and learn about people, and we love explanations that involve their desires, loves, and conflicts. We prefer paintings of people and religious myths featuring personified gods and spirits.

We pay particular attention to things we hope or fear are true. Hope gives us pleasure, and fear, though we might not particularly enjoy it, demands our attention. This is why we are susceptible to believing in miracle cures, get-rich schemes, and attractive salespeople. It also explains the fear of hell and why we willingly expose ourselves to terrifying things like thrill rides, horror movies, and television news.

We delight in finding patterns. When we notice regularity in the world, we understand the world better and, thanks to evolution, we get rewarded with a rush of pleasure. Compelling things always have patterns, be they a repeated chorus in a song or a repeated religious ritual. When we notice the same pattern again and again, however, we can get bored. We are curious beings that seek out new things to understand and new patterns to discover.

We are attracted to incongruity, apparent contradictions, novelty, and puzzles. When there is something askew, something we don’t quite get, we are intrigued and want to figure it out. Art and religion both play on our love for incongruity and for pattern, pulling us in with something incongruous and then allowing us to feel pleasure when we discover an underlying meaning or pattern.

The nature of our bodies—the nature of our eyes and other sense organs, for example—affects what kinds of things draw us in.

We have certain psychological traits, many of which are evolved, that make us like and dislike, believe and disbelieve.

So how do these foundations explain what we find riveting?

* * *

First, the arts. I will mostly focus on folk and pop art, as I believe they better exemplify what people actually like, as opposed to fine arts, which are subject to intellectualizing by specialists and as such have the potential to be further removed from our natural and general cultural proclivities.

Why do people like to experience art (and things we experience in an artlike way) and how does art affect them?

Compelling art works because it reliably creates experiences in audience members. David Byrne reflects on creating music in this way: Making music is like constructing a machine whose function is to dredge up emotions in performer and listener. . . . I’m beginning to think of the artist as someone who is adept at making devices that tap into our shared psychological make-up and that trigger the deeply moving parts we have in common.¹² What’s important is the artistic experience, which happens in the mind of the audience. It’s a reaction.

Some might think that understanding why people like visual art is impossible because beauty is in the eye of the beholder. But as psychologist of art Ellen Winner points out, there is considerable agreement in aesthetic preferences, even among people of different sexes, intelligence, personality traits, and culture. There are indeed differences, but our similarities are undeniable.¹³

We tend to find beautiful images that show us a world that, were we actually in it, would be a great place to be. Even compelling nonrepresentational images are liked as a result of sharing patterns with real-world things that we prefer to be in the presence of. This covers more than just art. Seeing a beautiful vista or a sunrise can elicit the same kind of response, for the same reasons. The Dinka people, in east Africa, create almost no visual art, but they appreciate the markings on cattle.¹⁴

Our preferences for landscapes reflect our desires for safety, information, and resources. As we will see, certain colors produce primal, cross-cultural reactions. As social creatures we like to see other people in images. As sexual creatures, we like to see attractive people.

We find disturbing images compelling, since they tend to hint at dangerous, important information. Even if we are horrified, we can’t look away, much like rubbernecking drivers on the highway who must slow down to see what happened at an accident site. We either like to see images that show the world as we would like it to be, or as we strongly would prefer it not to be. It’s the middle area that’s boring—the anti–sweet spot between hope and fear.

When we see visual patterns, we are delighted, because seeing a pattern is noticing a predictable regularity in the world that we might be able to exploit. Patterns are breaks in the chaos. Visual rhymes, repeated colors, symmetry, and the repetition of symbols in a painting or a series of photographs attract us.

If we see something too often, or the patterns viewed are too familiar, we get bored. We need challenges to maintain our interest—the enigmatic Mona Lisa smile, an unexplored territory, or some other incongruity. There is a sweet spot between pattern and incongruity. An image is boring if it’s too simple or too complex.

The location of this sweet spot differs from person to person, and perhaps even from mood to mood. The more familiar one is with an art form or style, the more incongruity is allowed. Everyone can appreciate Norman Rockwell, but it takes some knowledge to appreciate Chinese calligraphy or Jackson Pollock’s drip paintings. Some paintings have surface patterns that are easy to appreciate, but further investigation reveals deeper incongruities for the audience to resolve. As people grow in expertise, they can detect more features and appreciate the art more.

* * *

Because they involve visuals as well as words, the performing arts and film are at the crossroads of literature and the visual arts. Even more than in painting and photography, they are about people. Nearly all films and live performances involve human or humanlike characters, ones that appeal to our desire to observe people in any situation.

Like paintings and photographs, films and performances include visual motifs that are played out over time as well as in space, appealing to the pattern compellingness foundation. For example, several dancers might be doing the same motions at the same time or similar movements at different times. Ballet has a vocabulary of moves. A film might use a limited palette of colors, thereby increasing the familiarity.

Our love for incongruity influences a film’s or a performance’s appeal as well. The works range from simple to incomprehensibly complex, all to be appreciated by different audiences, in different moods, at different times. We can stand extreme incongruities for only a short period of time, allowing music videos to be more avant-garde than feature-length films. This is why absurdist full-length theater is sometimes difficult for the general public to appreciate.

* * *

The narrative arts include any art form that uses a story element, such as theater and theatrical improvisation, novels, films, contemporary (urban) legends, tabletop role-playing games, and many computer games. People also find meaning in their lives by constructing them as narratives.¹⁵

Our obsession with people plays out most strongly in narrative, where conflicts between characters are ubiquitous. When we learn about the lives of characters, our old brains react as though the characters were real people, and we try to learn lessons from them. In fact, reading fiction has been found to correlate with empathy and unselfish behavior. White children who read stories with African American characters had improved attitudes toward African Americans—not only more than kids reading stories about white children, but more than children who interacted with real African American children on a shared task!

In one study, children who read stories without descriptions of mental states (e.g., Jill was happy) did better at a test of understanding mental states of other people than children who read stories with such descriptions. One explanation of this fascinating finding is that forcing the audience to make inferences themselves prepares them better for reasoning about other people than having conclusions handed to them.¹⁶

We learn from narratives—for better or worse—whether we want to or not. Unrealistic human interaction depicted in narratives can cause problems of understanding humanity. Readers of romance, for example, probably believe more strongly in the swept away by romance trope. Research by psychologists Marsh and Fazio found that they have negative ideas about using condoms, have used them less in the past, and plan to use them less in the future.¹⁷ Just as we take care of what food we put into our bodies, we should take care what narratives we put into our minds.

* * *

There is no human society without some musical tradition.¹⁸ Nearly all social religious rituals use music. Our reactions to music, though they differ by culture, appear to have some cross-cultural similarities, including the perception of emotional tone. The metaphors we attach to music (high pitches being happy, for example) might be cross-cultural.

Our desire for understanding, and our desire to detect patterns, is clearly manifested in music, where repetition is crucial. Repetition happens in music at multiple levels, from a constant drum beat to the recognizable elements that make up a musical style or genre. Where we often get a sense immediately if we like a painting or not, many of us have learned that music, although we sometimes get a strong reaction at first, often takes repeated listenings before we know for sure whether we like it or not. Music is meant to be heard again and again. One can acceptably love a novel that one has only read once, but we are surprised if we hear of someone speak of a song he or she likes, but has only chosen to listen to once.

* * *

Some sports—such as acrobatics, figure skating, cheerleading, synchronized swimming, martial arts, parkour, and competitive ballroom dancing—occupy a gray area between games and art. Though sports are not considered art, there can be no doubt that our enjoyment of sports involves aesthetic appreciation, and that some sports, such as synchronized swimming and figure skating, tread the art/sport border. But even prototypical sports, such as basketball and soccer, share similarities with an artistic performance: people take pleasure in watching it, skill is involved with the performance, critics analyze it afterward, and there is a strong emotional response.

Much like narratives, sports involve people in conflict. Sports in which two or more players are competing directly with one another (e.g., tennis and lacrosse) might be appreciated because they represent, deep in our minds, interpersonal conflict, where solitary sports (e.g., weightlifting and archery) are appreciated for the same reasons the man versus nature model of plotting are.

Team sports trigger our sense of loyalty, one of the foundations of moral psychology. People align themselves, in their minds, with a team, and feel joy when it succeeds and sorrow when it loses. Loyalty matters to men and to women, but men tend to be loyal to coalitions and women to two-person relationships.¹⁹ This would predict that men prefer team sports relatively more than women do.

Our love for repetition is satisfied with the ritual aspect of the rules, such as the

Enjoying the preview?
Page 1 of 1