You are on page 1of 11

TheOriginofSuperstition,MagicalThinking, andParanormalBeliefs (anintegrativemodel) byMarjaanaLindeman&KiaAarnio

In his always quirky but usually insightful look into the human condition, comedianWoodyAllenonceremarked:Thereisnoquestionthatthereisanunseen world.Theproblemishowfaritisfrommidtownandhowlateisitopen? In point of fact, the majority of people accept as a given that an unseen world of paranormal powers exists, and all that remains is for us to discover the details of its workings. Superstition and magical thinking are the core cognitions that drive belief in the paranormal. Over 40% of Americans, for example, believe in devils, ghosts, and spiritual healing. Most social scientists do not bother trying to understand why people believe in the paranormal, while many psychologists have characterized superstitious and magical thinking as a problem for which there is no ready explanation, or as a label for a residual category a garbage bin filled with variousoddsandendsthatwedonototherwiseknowwhattodowith. Skeptics, of course, have not ignored superstitions and magical beliefs, and thereexist today severalinternational magazines, regularconferences, and dozens of excellent books that attempt to both explain the paranormal as well as understand why people believe in it. Explanations have ranged from personality traits, psychological motivation, and flawed cognition, to emotional instability, demographics, and social influences. From the purely scientific perspective of experimental psychology, however, our overall understanding of this area has yet to be adequately described and explained. This paper presents a new and integrative modelthatexplainssuperstition,magicalthinking,andparanormalbeliefs. TheNeedforaConceptualModel One of the primary problems facing a scientist studying superstition, magical thinking, and paranormal beliefs is defining what precisely the field entails. There is little agreement on how these terms should be defined, outside of simply providing specific examples of each, and it is not clear how (or if ), the constructs of superstition, magical thinking, and paranormal (supernatural) beliefs differ from each other, or how they differ from obviously false beliefs (e.g., whales are fish). Consequently, there is a strong need for a conceptual model that clarifies the meaningofmagical,paranormal,andsuperstitiousbeliefs,andexplainswhyrational Western people still believe in things that seem so irrational. This study provides an initial step in this direction. Here we will offer new theoretical propositions, which willdefine theconstructsandoffercriteriafortheirapplication. Wewillalsoanalyze whetherourdefinitioncanbeempiricallyjustified. Among the most influential definitions of magical thinking are the laws of sympathetic magic outlined in the early days of anthropology. The law of contagion

:1:

holdsthatthingsthathaveoncebeenincontactwitheachothercontinueto actupon each other at a distance after the physical contact has been severed. The law of similarity holds that superficial resemblance indicates, or causes, deep resemblance. During the last two decades, researchers have conducted a series of pathbreaking studies that revealed how these laws manifest themselves among welleducated Western adults. However, the laws of sympathetic magic are neither intended nor sufficienttocoverallsuperstitious,magicalandparanormalbeliefs.Moreover,asthe researchers themselves note, the distinction between the laws of magical thinking and reality for example between magical contagion and microbial contamination andbetweenmagicalsimilarityandvaccinationcanbesubtleandambiguous. Other authors have defined superstitious and magical beliefs more widely as false cognitions for example as limitations in cognitive processing, beliefs that are barely articulated, tenets founded on ignorance, and as causal beliefs that by conventionalstandardsareinvalid. Simply defining superstitious beliefs as erroneous still leaves us with an important question: how do they differ from other unfounded beliefs? Today most scientists agree with Charles Broads definition of the paranormal as a phenomenon that violates the fundamental and scientifically founded principles of nature. Using this as our starting point, we propose that the key difference between superstitious, magical,orparanormalbeliefs,andotherkindsofunfoundedbeliefscanbefoundby applying a concept from studies on childrens cognitive development known as core knowledge. CoreKnowledgeandSuperstition According to developmental psychologists, three types of knowledge determine a childs understanding of the world: intuitive physics, intuitive psychology, and with certain reservations, intuitive biology. Part of this knowledge is characterized as core knowledge, that is, knowledge that children learn without instruction; for example, intuitive comprehension of physical, biological, and psychological entities as well as different forms of processes in which these entities engage. Core knowledge developed by preschool age provides the foundation for further development. It is based on what psychologists call domain specialized learning mechanisms, or modules, which evolved in response to our Paleolithic environment. Developmentalstudiesshowthatcoreknowledgeofphysicalentitiesincludes theunderstandingthattheworldiscomposedofmaterialobjectswhichhavevolume and an independent existence in space. Core knowledge of biological entities represents a speciestypical adaptation to the problem of food selection and illness avoidance. Even if cultures lack a scientific understanding of disease transmission they still possess an intuitive understanding of it through their core knowledge. Similarly, 4yearolds know that abnormal behaviors are not contagious, and they can discriminate between contaminated and safe substances despite a lack of visible evidence. Core knowledge of psychological entities includes the understanding that animate beings are intentional agents which have a mind. By the middle of the

:2:

second year children understand that animate beings can reciprocate actions and haveacapacitytomoveandinitiateactionswithoutexternalforce.Inaddition,small children understand that the contents of mind thoughts, beliefs, desires, and symbols are nonmaterial and mental, and that they do not contain the properties they stand for. For example, 3 and 4yearold children understand that the thought of a dog does not have the material properties of a dog, and that the roads in a map donotneedtobewideenoughforcars. How is it, then, that intuitively rational children grow up to become superstitiously irrational adults? One explanation for superstition is that a childs intuitive core knowledges of physical, biological, and psychological entities can become conflated with each other and applied across categories. As a consequence, ordinary entities and processes in the natural domain become extraordinary entities and processes in integrated supernatural domains. This fusion leads people to conclude that mental contents have the attributes of physical or animate entities, resulting in the possibility that a thought can touch objects (psychokinesis) or move by itself (telepathy). For example, when contagion and healing are attributed to psychological phenomena, we find that Hitlers personality can spread into his sweater, and that a healer can cure someone at a distance by force of his or her thoughts. In this other world, entities with good and bad minds such as angels and devils have independent existences and operate as animate objects by moving and initiatingactionswithoutexternalforce. In the magical world, mental qualities like intention are attributed to physical and biological events such as when rain dancers try to influence the atmosphere, or when Las Vegas craps shooters roll the dice gently to coax a low number or vigorously to encourage a high number, in the belief that they can will a change in the physical environment. We also find that the concept of force in lay physics can be seen by some as a living and intentional entity. For example, feng shui holds that incorrect arrangement of home furnishings can block a vital force which might make a household susceptible to crime or divorce; and astrologers suggest that planets emit living forces which push and pull human beings in a purposeful way that affects personality and wellbeing. In the magical world of superstitious thinking, biological and physical processes are no longer seen as nonintentional as they are in the core knowledge world of children. Instead they are seen as having a purpose that is directed toward specific goals goals that can be influenced by the nonphysicalassertionsofoutsideagents. DefiningSuperstitious,Magical,andParanormalBeliefs Based on this research on core knowledge, we define superstitious, magical, and paranormal beliefs as category mistakes where the core attributes of mental, physical, and biological entities and processes are confused with each other. Of course, not all superstitions are category mistakes. For example, many adults regard physical force as a material substance, which it is not. What differentiates other category mistakes from superstitions is that in superstitions the category mistakes always include a confusion of core knowledge. Also, category mistakes can be

:3:

perceived as superstitions only insofar as the statements are believed to be literally true. Thus, metaphorical and allegorical expressions that deliberately confuse the propertiesarenotsuperstitions(e.g.,Awellfunctioningmemoryisagoldmine). While preschool children understand a surprising amount about physical, biological, and psychological phenomena, at first they make the same category mistakes that can be found in superstitions. However, equating adults superstitions with small childrens misconceptions does not imply that superstitious individuals are cognitively at the level of small children. The definition should be understood through the basic tenet of dualprocess theories that people have two modes of processing information intuition and logic which rely on different databases and operate using different rules. According to the dualprocess theories, as children mature analytical processes and rational knowledge do not replace intuitive processes and contents. Rather, both types of processes and knowledge exist and develop throughout ones life, and therefore two conflicting beliefs can coexist in an adults mind, one rational and justifiable (e.g., Death is final), the other operating more automatically and being more resistant to logical arguments (e.g., The soul continuestoexistthoughthebodymaydie). TestingtheHypothesis To investigate whether our new conceptualization was warranted, we hypothesized that in comparison to skeptical individuals, superstitious individuals should more often assign mental attributes to physical and biological entities and material attributes to mental entities, and confuse intentional processes with non intentional processes. We also hypothesized that various types of superstitious, magical, and paranormal beliefs are similarly related to ontological confusions that are, in turn, positively related to a tendency to rely on intuitive thinking. Finally, we expected that ontological confusions and intuitive thinking are more important correlates of superstitions, magical and paranormal beliefs than the two other correlates that have repeatedly been suggested to account for the beliefs lower rationalthinkingandemotionalinstability. To test our hypothesis, 239 Finnish volunteers participated in the study (recruited from a larger population of 3,261 individuals who participated in a study onsuperstitiontheyearbefore):96femaleand27maleskepticswerecomparedto88 female and 28 male superstitious individuals. We sent an invitation to those whose overallsuperstitionscoreswereamongtheupperorlower10%fortheirgender.(The groups were balanced for gender because in the earlier study women (M = 2.16) had higher superstition scores than men (M = 1.94), a difference significant at the .001 level. Their age range was 1647 with a mean of 24.2 years. The great majority (94%) were fulltime students who represented a wide variety of disciplines, including the natural,behavioral,medical,andsocialsciences,andtechnology,business,trade,and services.) Totestourhypothesisaboutontologicalconfusions,weconstructedameasure based on one developed by Chi and her associates. First, we examined the participants conceptions of properties of different ontological entities by 34

:4:

statements in which attributes of one ontological category were assigned to another one. The participants were asked whether they understood the statements as metaphorically or literally true (1 = only metaphorically, 5 = only literally). (Figures 13.) In 16 of the statements, material entities (artifacts, liquids, solids, plants) held mental attributes (e.g., beliefs, desires, kindness). For example: Old furniture remembers things about the past and When summer is warm, flowers want to bloom. These items measured the tendency to mentalize matter. Ten of the statements assessed physicalizing the mental. They described mental phenomena such as a thought or human mind that was said to have the attributes of physical matter such as volume, or the ability to affect a material object as if by touch. For example:AhumanmindlivesonafterthepersonhasdiedSixstatementsassessed biologizing the mental. They described mental phenomena (a thought, a human mind) said to have the attributes of biological entities (e.g., is living, can be contaminated). For example: An evil thought may literally contaminate an entity. For comparison, there were also eight fully metaphorical statements. (e.g., A wailing wind is a flute) and four fully literal statements (e.g., Running water is fluid). Confusion between intentional and nonintentional events was analyzed by giving the participants descriptions of 18 nonintentional events that lead to personallyrelevantoutcomes,andbyaskingwhethertheysawpurposeinthatevent (1=theeventhadnopurpose,5=theeventclearlyhadapurpose).The18statements included three types of nonintentional events. Six of the events were random (e.g., dealing cards in a card game), six were artificial (e.g., a server failure), and six were natural events (e.g., fog). For each set of six events, positive, negative, and neutral outcomesweredescribed. The positive and negative outcomes were derived from the Life Experience Survey.Anexamplestatementofarandomeventwithanegativeoutcomeis:When you play cards you get only clubs and spades and therefore run up large debts. Did you get those cards for a purpose? An example statement of an artificial event with a positive outcome includes: Your brakes fail, causing damage to a strangers car. You end up marrying the stranger. Did the brakes fail for a purpose? An example statement of natural events with a neutral outcome is: A lightning strike topples a big tree in your garden, but causes no other harm. Did the lightning have a purpose? For comparison, there were also four statements, which described true intentional events (e.g., a kiss, running a race, bullying) with positive, neutral, and negative outcomes (e.g., a beginning of a romantic relationship, reduced working ability). The level of belief in the paranormal by participants had been measured in an earlier study through the Revised Paranormal Belief Scale (RPBS), which was a slightly revised version of the most commonly used measure of superstitious, magical, paranormal and religious beliefs. Because the items on the RPBS cover only some aspects of superstitious and magical beliefs, it was supplemented with a number of items to cover a wider spectrum of beliefs. The 55 items were measured

:5:

on a fivepoint rating scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree) and included: entities, such as witches, extraordinary life forms, and extraterrestrial life (e.g., Ghosts exist); psi power, such as telepathy, spiritualism, precognition, and psychokinesis (e.g., A persons thoughts can influence the movement of a physical object); religious beliefs (e.g., I believe in God); luck, rituals, and amulets (e.g., Amulets, for instance a specific piece of jewelry, bring good luck); astrology (e.g., Theposition of thestars atthe time of birth influencespersonality); feng shui (e.g., Furnishing according to the principles of feng shui balances your environment and thus affects your health and success in a positive way). Finally, to measure overall superstitionameanscoreofalltheitemswasused. Analytical and intuitive thinking were assessed by the RationalExperiential Inventory (REI), which consists of two 20item scales (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree). The Rationality subscale of the REI assesses the extent to which an individual employs rational, analytic, effortful, affectfree, and logical thinking (e.g., I usually have clear, explainable reasons for my decisions). The Experientiality subscale of the REI assesses the extent to which an individual employs automatic, preconscious, holistic, nonverbal, and associationistic thinking (e.g., I believe in trustingmyhunches). Emotional instability was measured by the Neuroticism subscale of the Finnishversion of the NEO FiveFactor Inventory, also known as the Big Five.The subscale consists of 48 fivepoint items (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree), which measure anxiety, depression, selfconsciousness, vulnerability, impulsiveness, andhostility. ResultsoftheStudy As would be expected based on our hypothesis, there are ontological differences in belief between the believers and the skeptics in our sample: believers mentalized matter more than skeptics (Figure 1), believers physicalized the mental more than skeptics (Figure 2), and also believers biologized the mental more than skeptics (Figure 3). By contrast, believers did not differ from skeptics when they assessedthetruthofpurelyliteralstatementsorthepurelymetaphoricalstatements. In addition, believers also assigned more purpose to natural events than skeptics (Figure 4), more purposeto artificial events than the skeptics (Figure 5), and more purpose to random events than the skeptics (Figure 6). By contrast, determining that intentional events had purpose was recognised to an equal degree byboththebelieversandtheskeptics. The results also showed that believers relied more on intuitive thinking than skeptics (Figure 7), but less on analytical thinking than skeptics (Figure 8). And believers wereemotionally lessstable than skeptics(Figure 9). There were no gender differencesbetweenthegroups. WhattheResultsMean In the past, scientists have treated beliefs in superstition, magic, and the paranormal as both distinct and interrelated phenomena, and they have not been

:6:

consistent in defining these concepts. Our goal in this study was to present a unified conceptualframeworkwheretheconceptsweredifferentiatedfromotherunfounded beliefs and defined identically as an ontological confusion between the core attributes of mental, physical, and biological entities and processes. In addition, we examined whether superstitious individuals confuse the attributes of ontological categories,asthedefinitionsuggests. The results supported our hypothesis. Compared with skeptics, superstitious believers assigned more physical and biological attributes to mental phenomena. Significantly more than skeptics, believers held such notions as a mind that can physically affect objects and an evil thought may literally contaminate an entity. Superstitious individuals also assigned more mental attributes to water, furniture, rocks, and other material things than skeptics did, and accepted that entities like thesemayliterally,notonlymetaphoricallyhavepsychologicalpropertiessuch as desires, knowledge, or a soul. In particular, superstitious individuals saw natural, random and artificial (i.e., nonintentional) events like fog or a computer failure as having a purpose when the processes that led to episodes ended up having a personally relevant outcome such as a marriage. The believers did not, however, differfromtheskepticswhentheyassessedtheactualtruthofpurelyliteralorpurely metaphorical statements, or the purposefulness of truly intentional acts like kissing. Theresultsalsoshowedthatvariousmanifestationsofthebeliefsinsuchphenomena as astrology, feng shui, and ESP were associated with ontological confusions and with higher intuitive thinking, and albeit only slightly to lower analytical thinking and emotional instability. These results support the argument that superstitions and other paranormal beliefs arise from the intuitive system and not from a failure of the analytical system, and are in line with the earlier findings that people who rely more on intuitive thinking hold more superstitions than others. In short, our results are in line with the theory that ontological confusions are defining propertiesofsuperstitions,magical,andparanormalbeliefs. In addition, our new definition of superstition as a category mistake of core knowledge enables us to identify how superstitions differ from other unfounded beliefs. Accordingly, many beliefs that have previously been regarded as paranormal, magical, or superstitious are simply unsubstantiated beliefs, not superstitions. These include, for example, belief in graphology or biorhythms. Likewise, beliefs obeying the laws of contagion and similarity are here regarded as superstitions only insofar as the idea of contagion is stretched beyond the biological domain and similarity is used to draw inferences about entities from different ontologicaldomains.Thus,disgusttowardsapieceofclothingwornbyasickperson is not a superstition, whereas disgust towards clothes worn by Hitler is. Similarly, reasoning that whalesare fish because of similar appearanceand habitats is simply a false belief, whereas a belief that needles stuck in a doll cause pain in the person the dollrepresentsisasuperstition. The confusion of core knowledge is the common denominator for a wide range of superstitions, magical and paranormal beliefs, ranging from primitive animism to modern feng shui, and from childrens beliefs that the moon is an

:7:

animate being to educated adults beliefs in astrology. It is our hope that this new conceptualization will enable researchers to make more elaborated theoretical statements regarding superstition. For example, one theory holds that superstitions are a form of peoples search for causal explanations and organizing the world in a meaningfulandconsistentfashiontoimposeorderandpredictabilityonit.However, because the same reasons also apply to numerous other endeavors, such as science, theexplanatorypowerofthesestatementsforsuperstitionhasbeenweak. The present study suggests that future studies on superstition and theories to explain them might benefit from addressing intuitive thinking and its knowledge base. Subsequent efforts may provide more powerful indications that superstitiousindividualsknowledgeabouttheworldisinaccurateinthattheirearly, as yet undeveloped intuitive conceptions about psychological, biological, and physical phenomena have retained their power and coexist with later acquired rationalknowledge. In addition, future research might attempt to analyze whether superstitions can be understood in terms of a common essence. For example, children classify members into categories based on their essences (e.g., dogs and cats are in the same category animals; toys and dogs are in different categories).48 Similarly, anthropologists have suggested that a central theme in the versatile magical beliefs found among the Hua people in New Guinea is the vital essence they call nu.49 This common essence parallels the view that in superstitions, there is typically an interconnected cosmos, a fundamental relation between a part and a whole where, for example, individual humans are connected to the universe, and a single event is linked to the future.50 Thus, we suggest that confusion between the core properties of ontological categories implies a notion of a common essence between the categories, and this leads to thinking in terms of connections and undivided totalities. It is this core confusion that we believe leads to the belief in superstition, magic,andtheparanormal. Acknowledgments: We thank Tarmo Toikkanen and Marieke Saher for assistance. ThisstudywassupportedbyagrantfromtheAcademyofFinland(200828).

:8:

:9:

: 10 :

: 11 :

You might also like