You are on page 1of 7

THE ‘X’ REPORT

by Paul Henrickson, Ph.D. © 2004 tm. © 2007

In the Spring of 2004 I had the interesting experience of administering a set of


verbal and a set of no-verbal creativity tasks that I had devised while a research
assistant (1960-1961) to Dr. E. Paul Torrance (Bureau of Educational Research,
University of Minnesota) to approximately 160 elementary school children.

Over the past 40 years these creativity tasks had been used by myself and a very few
other experimental psychologists, in some cases as a means of enriching other
research programs. The University of Northern Iowa, The University of Radford in
Radford, Virginia, The University of Georgia were cases in point. Other, more
demanding, responsibilities in other places required me to place these informative
tasks to the side until now. It is probably just as well as the need for the information
to be gained from their administration is even greater now than it was earlier.

It is important to stress that the information to be gained from these creativity tasks
relates to modes of thinking that differ markedly from those generally supported in
most schools, that is, these tasks invite diverse responses to the creation of a
functional reality. To put that observation more graphically, it might be said that the
puddle I play in may not be the puddle you play in, but we both get wet. After we
finish playing and walk home together for lunch we can still talk about our
experiences and build on the process of awareness.

There were two creativity tasks involved. The Creativity Design Task (CDT) and
The Just Suppose Task (JST). The CDT is a non-verbal task and the JST is a verbal
task. My purpose in making this report is to disseminate as widely as possible the
results of administering these tasks to a group of elementary school children, boys
and girls, ranging in age from 5 years to eleven.

These subjects might be described further as children involved in a school


environment that stressed accomplishment in achieving results in predetermined
areas of social and academic behavior. The desire for adult approval among the
students was remarkable in that in few ways would they express a desire to or an
interest in following their own intuitions. Actually, this particular school is not
unique in that way as these forms of ethical behavior are stressed in this society at
all levels and in all ways more than in many other societies I’ve known. Significantly
enough, this sort of control over social behavior of all sorts and on all levels has had
an interesting effect upon the ways and manner of achieving private goals and
circumventing existing convention while maintaining the appearance of social
conformity.
Having said all that I must, in order to clarify a point, and to give credit where
credit is due, state that the social structure within which most activity is regulated
did not inhibit my decision-making process or exert controls over my interpretation.
Where there was evidence of cooperation there was as well a willingness to follow
through with accepted responsibilities and for this I owe my gratitude to the
Director of Education for the area and most especially to the headmistress of the
elementary school involved.

Both of these individuals as well as the teachers involved were unfamiliar with the
goals and the methods of achieving them and trusted to my good will, honesty and
professional acumen to prevail. What came to me as a surprise was the striking
unfamiliarity, which surrounded my statements and my procedures. In all other
instances of my having used these creativity tasks I had been in an environment,
which had been much, more familiar with the open-ended research approach. If, in
this present experiment, my approach had confused anyone they endured their
confusion stoically. I also wish to thank those who had helped me in the
administration of these tasks including Marita Mejlak, Patricia Prescott, Coronato
Vella and William Driscoll for their, truly, invaluable, contributions.

As indicated above the administration of the CDT and JST tasks was accomplished
with approximately 162 subjects. The subjects were elementary school boys and
girls between the ages of 5 and eleven years. The administration of the tasks took
place over a four-week period during the Friday afternoon “experimental” free time
period. This is one hour a week at the end of a week otherwise devoted to the
academic subjects of Religion, Maltese, English, Math, and Social Studies.

It will be of help, I believe, if I first describe the creativity tasks used in this study.

THE CREATIVITY DESIGN TASK is a non-verbal task which is comprised of a six


page task booklet and a standard number of pieces triangles, squares, circles and
long rectangles measuring approximately ¼” by 3” of colored paper in red, green
and gray. The subjects are encouraged/allowed to alter any of these shapes in
anyway they choose with whatever materials or tools are at hand, be they scissors,
crayons, pencils and the like. The subjects were instructed to make as many designs
as they wished in the thirty minutes allowed.

The CREATIVITY DESIGN TASK was scored along the following criteria: fluency,
flexibility, manipulation, hi-manipulation, elaboration and aesthetic appeal. The
first five measures are fairly well standardized with the application of scoring
guidelines. The measure of “aesthetic appeal” was in this instance and in earlier
experimental situations standardized to the extent that the judges have always been
of professional standing in the field of graphic arts. This writer is aware that this
concept (the concept of expert opinion) could be further explored on its own with
interesting results. The writer is also aware that the concept of standardizing
aesthetic judgments has its intellectual pitfalls but in this and similar experiments,
the parameters and limitations of a scientific approach have been accepted.
The JUST SUPPOSE TASK was a verbal task, which presented to the subjects the
challenge of responding to improbable situations. The improbable situations in this
instance were:

JUST SUPOSE: all the water around Gozo would stand still. What might be the
result?

you were to be left alone on Fifla island with only a dog, a cat, and a
lizard. What would be the consequences?

all the cars and trucks on Gozo would disappear. What might be the
result?

everything in the world, the people, trees, animals, books, turned


into numbers. What might be the consequences?

that the end of the rainbow could be found in Calypso’s cave. What
would be the consequences?

just suppose boys were red in color and girls were green . What
would be the result?

The subjects were allowed five minutes to respond to each of these improbable
situations with as many ideas as they might manage and were told that spelling was
not, in this case, important, and they could respond in either Maltese or English
which ever they preferred.

INTERNAL ANALYSES OF THE CREATIVITY TASKS

CDT analysis:
In addition to the factor of aesthetic judgment measure which was an external factor
applied to products of the CDT the five measures which were a part of the internal
structure of the CDT were: fluency, flexibility, manipulation, hi-manipulation, and
elaboration.

“Fluency” was the sheer number of designs produced; “flexibility” was the measure
of a shift in the character of the design; “manipulation” was the evidence of the
number of actions taken to modify the raw material (the colored geometric shapes);
“hi-manipulation” was the score of the one product with the highest number of
evidences; “elaboration” was the score on playing with variations of a graphic
concept.

JST analysis:
The Just Suppose Task (JST) was comprised of six pages in booklet form with one of
each of six improbable situations at the head of each page with the question
following which asked: “what might be the results, or consequences”. The subjects
were asked to write down as many ideas as they could within the five minutes
allotted for each of the situations.
The measures from this task which were recorded were: “fluency”, “negativity”,
“positive responses”, and “originality”.

“Fluency” was the simple measure of the number of responses recorded.

“Negativity” was the count of responses indicating fear, dislike, disapproval and the
like.

“Positive responses” were responses indicating approval, agreement, an ability to


deal with the situation and the like.

“Originality” was arrived at by listing the numbers of times a statement might


appear in the booklets and arranging those statements along a normal bell curve, or
something close to one and selecting from the group that group that represented
about 5% of the least frequently appearing responses. There would be one count for
one such response.

REPORT OF COEFICIENTS OF CORRELATION:

CDT: cc(N=81) JST * cc(N=72)


Fluency r. flexibility = 0.66 Fluency r. negativity = 0.60
Fluency r. manipulation = 0.39 Fluency r. positive = 0.82
Fluency r. Hi-manipulation = 0.37 Fluency r. originality = 0.32
Fluency r. elaboration = 0.31 Negativity r. positive = 0.03
Negativity r. originality =0.19
Flexibility r. manipulation = 0.27 Positive r. originality =0.26
Flexibility r. Hi-Man = 0.20
Flexibility r. elaboration = 0.31 JST** cc(N=26)
Manipulation r. Hi-Man = 0.49 Fluency r. negativity =0.70
Hi-Man r. elaboration = 0.09 Fluency r. positive =0.76
Fluency r. originality =0.64
Aesthetic r. fluency = 0.27 Negativity r. positive =0.21
Aesthetic r. flexibility = 0.22 Negativity r. originality =0.38
Aesthetic r. manipulation = -0.13 Positive r. originality =0.57
Aesthetic r. Hi-Man = 0.09
Aesthetic r. elaboration = 0.04

DISCUSSION
The above analysis follows, in general, the results obtained in earlier research
projects with one remarkable exception. To begin the discussion, let us first identify
the non-surprises.
In both the CDT and the JST tasks all measures depend on the “fluency” measure
which is a simple measure of response. No response, no measurement! It is,
therefore, reasonable to expect CDT measures such as flexibility, manipulation, Hi-
manipulation and elaboration to be positively related to fluency and JST measures
such as negativity, positive and originality also to be positively related.

It is consistent with earlier studies, as well, that the measures of originality in the
JST task are more highly and positively related to the positive measure than they
are to the negativity measure. This we see is consistent in both JST sets, one with an
N of 26, the other with an N of 72.

The reason why the two sets differ may be explained by the fact that the larger “N”
contained more of the younger children than did the “N” 26 set. It must be
remembered that the JST task is a verbal task and the educational system here
stresses verbal expression in at least two languages, Maltese and English as well as,
maybe, three, four or five languages, Italian, French and German, depending upon
the level and the educational focus.

The startling result of a negative, -0.13, correlation between the aesthetic measure
and manipulation came as a complete surprise. All other administrations of the CDT
task that I know about the correlation were positive and relatively high. Although
the relationship between manipulation and Hi-manipulation remains the same i.e.,
“Hi-manipulation” is dependant upon “manipulation” in that it identifies the one
work that has the highest “manipulation” score and that its correlation with the
“aesthetic” score is both higher and in the positive direction than the correlation
between “aesthetic” and “manipulation”. This suggests, of course, consistent with
earlier studies, that those works judged to have “aesthetic” appeal are works that
have been more highly manipulated. The implication of this finding should be of
interest to art critics if not art historians.

In this administration as in earlier administrations this relationship has remained


fairly constant suggesting that this group does not differ in character from earlier
groups but only in degree. However, it is an important degree.

In earlier groups there had been several judges of aesthetic value, in this group there
was only one. Our familiarity with other and available judges did not allow us to use
more than the one we did. In past administrations of the CDT judges of aesthetic
value would differ from each other but in the over all their perceptions were
relatively consistent. It should be noted, however, that these perceptions are the
results of education, exposure, and experience. The judge we used in this instance
was born near Boston, Massachusetts is a graduate of Harvard University, a
practicing artist and a linguist with commands of over 14 languages including
ancient and modern Greek, Latin and Sanscript, a more competent judge would be
difficult to locate.

Another possible explanation for this surprising negative relationship between


“manipulation” and “aesthetic” appeal comes to our attention with the fact of the
contemporary “art scene” here being characterized by perhaps more art activity
taking place in this community than in others of comparable size (30,000) the
quality of that activity is not well informed.

The most advanced expressions in the area of the graphic arts remains, by and
large, at the same level as the analytical cubists of nearly a century ago. This
statement is not meant to imply that change for change sake is an acceptable goal,
but to ignore the legitimate developments in the graphic arts seems somehow, to
miss the point of their existence.

As extension of this interpretation might be that the contemporary artists here have
failed to perform their function as exponents of the cultural “cutting edge” and
perceptive critics have failed to bridge the gap between the creative artist and the
consuming community. Consequently educational leaders have failed to be
informed. At the root of this description is an awareness of the possibility of the
influence of cultural institutions and historical experiences. For centuries this
population has been enslaved, feudalized and ruled by occupiers and have had little,
if any, experience in ruling themselves. The three main supports for social dignity
and self-respect are education (by rote), noble heritage (through illegitimate
associations), and wealth (by deception).

These sad comments are made for one purpose only, to stress the significant
difference in approach to social and personal respect which performances on the
CDT and the JST underscore. One cannot do well on these tasks and be dishonest to
one’s legitimate perceptions. Trust in one’s individuated decisions is essential as is
an openness to experience and a willingness to self-correct, advance, change,
progress.

One can hardly avoid remarking upon the intense conservatism buttressed by the
masks of social pretense, the sensitivity to disparagement, attachment to perceived
“correctness”, and the intolerance toward error. Guilt walks like some corporeal
being and defends perceived assaults with vain pride and paper-tigering.

This form of insularism has, perhaps, found its ultimate expression in physical
disorders such as diabetes, rheumatoid arthritis and serious depression which
strikes the young and adventurous who yearn to leave and those who have reached
their sixties and yearn to die; and in social disorders such as an illogical but distinct
hierarchy of professional, political, and educational importance. As some German
visitors remarked: volksverblodung in describing the functioning mindset; and one
American surprised a bank president with the question: “Why do so many here
seem so uncertain as to who they are and their worth as human beings that they
seek recourse in cheating?” Perhaps the school custom of not identifying students
by name, but by number, as a means of protecting the child from potential other-
family reprisals is an expression of this as well. This last custom had caused me
considerable difficulty since having experienced several normally unnecessary
delays amounting to nearly a year made the identification of the tasks somewhat
dicey.

The importance of being able to identify the creative minds in one’s environment
rests on the understanding that it is those minds that may be able to determine the
degree of success in surviving until tomorrow.

Considerable stress has been placed on the importance to the worlds of business and
economics of developing and maintaining a creative mindset yet, I know of no
assistance being offered those enterprises among the numerous programs available
through, for example, the internet, that points out the repressions creative minds
endure culturally, socially, politically, economically. Nor have any of these programs,
to my knowledge, offered assistance to business and government to identify the
hidden creative talents in their midst. The one exception to this statement is that
which is offered through The Creativity Packet (www.tcp.com.mt) which uses as its
foundation both the CDT and the JST and a few more tasks.

What these creativity tasks are designed to accomplish is to cultivate the mindset
better able to flexibly select or devise the most appropriate course of action in
meeting the challenges of future problems. Practice in creative thinking has not
characterized most school situations and this author realizes that it stands in direct
opposition to the general consensus that the best way to teach is by authority, that is
authoritative, rote, repetitive responses.

The results of this pilot study suggests that at least in one school in this community
of 30,000 the curriculum available and the methods of offering it should be radically
changed to give as much emphasis to the investigative creative approach as to the
traditional.

You might also like