You are on page 1of 13

Article

Vision
20(1) 2436
An Investigation into Consumer 2016 MDI
SAGE Publications

Search and Evaluation Behaviour: sagepub.in/home.nav


DOI: 10.1177/0972262916628946

Effect of Brand Name http://vision.sagepub.com

and Price Perceptions

Kavita Sharma1
Shivani Garg2

Abstract
Past researchers extended the conceptualized model of perceived value to include the effects of multiple extrinsic cues used as
indicators of quality. The need for replication as well as extending the research findings has, however, been expressed unequivocally
by the researchers as it remains to be seen if the existing relationships would hold for consumers hailing from emerging nations.
The present study extends the theoretical and empirical evidences regarding the structural relationships of consumers perceptions
of marketing cues, namely, price and brand name, with the perceptions of quality, risk, sacrifice and value in the Indian context.
Using Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM) based analysis of questionnaire-based responses of young Indian
consumers, the study reports that high price is associated with the presence of better product features, assuming that a good quality
product always comes with a high price attached and there is a possible shifting from looking to lower price-product options to alter-
natives which boast of a favourable brand name indicated by a higher price level. The study holds implications for marketers in regard
to brand name positioning as well as planning communication and retail strategies.

Key Words
Brand Name Perceptions, Price Perceptions, Product Value, Positioning Strategy

Introduction to enable a generalization of research findings and also to


explore the intricacies of customers value perceptions.
Researchers in the past have developed models to investi- Against this background, this study has been extended
gate customers perception of value on the basis of extrin- to one durable and one semi-durable product category
sic cues used as indicators of quality (Akdeniz, Calantone, (i.e., mobile phones and athletic shoes) and a survey-based
& Voorhees, 2013; Dodds, 1991; Lee & Lou, 2011; Teas & design, as against an experimental design, has been used to
Aggarwal, 2000; Rao & Monroe, 1989; Steenkamp, 1988; explore the intricacies of the effect of marketing cues on
Sweeney & Johnson, 1999; Wheatley & Chiu, 1977; customers value perceptions in the Indian context. Until
Zeithaml, 1988), sacrifice (Kwun & Oh, 2004; Monroe & recently, the focus of consumer researches has shifted from
Krishnan, 1985; Olson, 1977; Zeithaml, 1988) and risk the matured markets of the United States to maturing markets
(Ofir & Bechtel, 1990; Oglethorpe & Monroe, 1987; Wood of Japan, China, Austria and the Netherlands (Olbrich &
& Scheer, 1996;). The conceptualized model of perceived Jansen, 2014). It remains to be seen if the existing relation-
value based on actual price though was extended taking ships would hold for consumers hailing from emerging
care of a multi-cue scenario involving price, brand name, nations since according to Anderson, De Palma and Thisse
store name, country-of-origin and packaging. Dodds (1992) the idiosyncratic nature of consumers decision-
(1991) emphasizes that research is needed to replicate as making delimit the applicability of existing researches.
well as to extend the original findings. It is felt that, at In a multi-cue scenario, the current research thus extends
large, researches were focused on low-priced products, and the theoretical and empirical evidences regarding the struc-
the shift in focus to a wider range of products, situations, tural relationships of consumers perceptions of marketing
settings and populations is required (Rao & Monroe, 1989) cues, namely, price and brand name, with the perceptions

1 Professor, Department of Commerce, Delhi School of Economics, University of Delhi, Delhi, India.
2 Assistant Professor, College of Vocational Studies, University Of Delhi, Delhi, India.

Corresponding author:
Kavita Sharma, Professor, Department of Commerce, Delhi School of Economics, University of Delhi, Delhi, India.
E-mail: ksharma.dse@gmail.com
Sharma and Garg 25

of quality, risk, sacrifice and value in the Indian context. quality, risk and value, these efforts according to Akdeniz
The purpose of the study is to know the way perceptions et al. (2013) are inadequate in terms of presentation of
of price and brand name effect consumers perceptions of isolated effects of the select marketing cues. Purohit and
quality and value as the differences in the effect of price Srivastava (2001) consider the results of past researches
and brand name perceptions on consumers value percep- as equivocal for the reason that consumers seldom assess
tions require special focus in emerging market contexts. these cues in isolation. Rao and Monroe (1989) also suggest
Due to the unassailable growing affluence of consumers that there is a need to investigate the interactive effect of
in emerging markets, particularly in India, there is now various marketing cues on consumers value perceptions. In
a clear shift in inclination from relying primarily on price this study, through the testing of a conceptualized model,
for their product choice. Further, the increasing influx we try to understand the way the price and brand name
of branded products warrants the noticeable change in the together influence value perceptions, that is, whether Indian
consumers search and evaluation behaviour. Marketers consumers impute a higher value, assigning better evalua-
therefore need to be particularly mindful when combining tions, when brand name works as a stimulus along with
price with brand name. A greater understanding of how price information provided for their cognitive decision
the combination of price and brand name is perceived in processes.
emerging market contexts would therefore help marketers Lastly, the degree of knowledge and familairity about
to leverage brand names in specific buying situations. This the product possessed by consumers, also varying across
is in fact one among very few studies pioneering empirical different geographies, may have an influence on the
attempts to assess consumers inference of marketing cues relative use of the information cues in assesing product
in forming value perceptions in an emerging market setting value (Dowling & Staelin, 1994; Garbarino & Strahilevitz,
such as India. 2004). The current research investigates the level of con-
Besides, given that consumers rely on extrinsic cues to sumers familiarity and knowledge about the product
assess perceptions of risk (Bearden & Shimp, 1982), it is to ascertain the situations when consumers tend to place
important to examine the way risk perceptions effect value higher reliance on marketing cues during their search for
perceptions. According to Agarwal and Teas (2001), when the product. There are certain segments of consumers
exposed to extrinsic product cues, consumers do not just who are unable or unwilling to conduct an exhaustive
make judgements about product quality and sacrifice, they search and evaluation and shift their focus from product
also make judgements about uncertainties that may pose characteristics to marketing cues (Dodds & Monroe,
potential long-term losses. Therefore, we extend the basic 1985; Richardson et al., 1994; Zeithaml, 1988). Given
conceptualization of brand name to include the dimensions that India is comparatively a novice in its exposure
of perceived risk and argue that by relying on a good and to branded products, it is imperative for managers to get
credible brand name as cue, consumers get certain assur- acquainted with differing degrees of the consumers famili-
ances against the various types of risks involved in choice arity and knowledge of products and brands to formulate
decisions which in turn effect perception about quality. better marketing executions for them.
Possibly, lowered risk perception brought about by good In the remaining sections, the conceptual model is
and credible brand name effect value perception through developed along with the hypothesized relationships. The
higher quality perceptions (Aghekyan-Simonian, Forsythe, descriptions of research method involving the details of
Kwon, & Chattaraman, 2012). testing of measurement models, model predictability and
Furthermore, unlike past studies where brand name testing of hypothesized relationships are given next. The
perceptions are controlled through the selection of specific last section contains a discussion of the managerial implica-
brands having a less or more favourable image (Erdem tions, in particular, to emerging market-contexts assisting
& Swait, 2004; Erdem, Swait, & Valenzuela, 2006), this in strategic development and redefinition of marketing,
study in its research design required the respondents to positioning and communication strategies based on the rela-
select the brand they are more familiar with in the specified tionship between perceptions of marketing cues and value.
product category to allow for testing the information inher-
ent in the brand name and not the familiarity with the
brand. Besides, the experimental research design used for The Conceptual Model
testing the theorized relationships according to Rao and Considerable theoretical and empirical evidence suggests
Monroe (1989) may be incorrect for measuring the effect that consumers impute value for a choice decision by infer-
of actual price on value perception. ring quality of the product/service on the basis of market-
Although existing research efforts like Kwun and Oh ing cues identified either as extrinsic and intrinsic cues
(2004), Teas and Agarwal (2000), Richardson, Dick and (Dodds, Monroe, & Grewal, 1991; Olson, 1977; Lichtenstein,
Jain (1994), Dodds and Grewal (1991), Rao and Monroe Ridgway, & Netemeyer, 1993; Olson & Jacoby, 1972).
(1989), Dodds and Monroe (1985) and Olson and Jacoby Intrinsic cues, such as memory and processing speed of
(1972) have explored the intricacies of the relationships a computer, are the internal physical attributes or opera-
between perceptions of marketing cues and perceptions of tional features in-built into the product and are utilitarian
26 Vision 20(1)

and specific to the product itself. Extrinsic cues are out- of quality, risk and sacrifice perception with (i) price and
side the product and are not a part of the physical product. brand name used as extrinsic cues and (ii) consumers
They are more general and applicable to a wider range value perceptions based on these cues.
of products, such as price, brand name, store name and
country of origin (Bearden & Shimp, 1982; Bilkey & Nes,
1982; Dodds & Grewal, 1991; Dodds & Monroe, 1985; Hypotheses
Han, 1989; Han & Terpstra, 1988; Janakiraman, Meyer, & Impact of price as marketing cue: Both, the actual price of
Morales, 2006; Monroe, 2002; Stokes, 1985; Vanhuele, a product as the objective numeral, and consumers per-
Laurent, & Dreze, 2006; Wheatley & Chiu, 1977; Zeithaml, ceptions of actual price which render meaning to consumers
1988). Both intrinsic and extrinsic cues are informative (Jacoby & Olson, 1977) as expensive, moderate and inex-
cues, acting as frames of reference, and perceived differ- pensive, derived from subjective internal representations,
ently by different consumers. Individual differences of affect the consumers evaluation of the product. In the
familiarity with or knowledge of the product category mod-
context of customer and service provider relationship,
erate consumers reliance on product attributes.
Singh and Sirdeshmukh (2000) have related price percep-
Researchers, however, believe that consumers rely
tions to evaluation of fairness of cost of service, i.e., whether
more on extrinsic cues during their search and evaluation
price extracted was fair. Consumers perceptions of objec-
of a product (Dodds & Grewal, 1991; Han & Terpstra,
tive price, hence, vary according to consumers background,
1988). Existing researches have considered consumers
situations and competitive choices (Monroe & Dodds,
perceptions of brand name, price and value and their link-
1988; Olbrich & Jansen, 2014;). The hypothesis here is:
ages, as pivotal determinants of their evaluation and choice
decision (Brucks et al., 2000; Degeratu, Rangaswamy,
& Wu, 2000; Dodds & Grewal, 1991; Zeithaml, 1988). H1:
Price perceptions positively influence perceived
There is also a particular emphasis on consumers use of value.
extrinsic cues as indicators of product quality and sacrifice.
The extant review of literature also suggests that linkage Traditional economic theory indicates that consumers
between extrinsic cues and consumers evaluation of a perceive price as an indicator of sacrifice, construed as
product may not be completely influenced by perceived present costs involved in acquiring a product (Agarwal
quality and sacrifice. Perceived risk (including perfor- & Teas, 2001). Higher price represents a monetary mea-
mance risk, functional risk, psychological risk, physical sure of what must be given to obtain the deal, that is, the
risk and social risk) may affect consumers evaluations in amount of monetary sacrifice involved in purchasing a
the presence of marketing cues (Bicen, 2015; Shimp & product (Dodds et al., 1991; Erickson & Johansson, 1985;
Bearden, 1982; Teas & Agarwal, 2000). Wood and Scheer Grewal et al., 1998; Kelley, 1958; Lichtenstein et al., 1993;
(1996), for example, also suggest that consumers evalua- Marian, Chrysochou, Krystallis, & Thgersen, 2014;
tions of a deal may be a function of its perceived benefits, Schmidt and Spreng, 1996; Teas & Agarwal, 2000; Wood
costs and risks. & Scheer, 1996; Zeithaml, 1988). Perceived sacrifice, thus,
The conceptualized model examined in the study is dia- refers to the feeling towards giving up something, that
grammed in Figure 1 and suggests the intricate relationships is, money (Dodds, 1995). In relation to price perceptions,
the hypothesis is:

H2:Price perceptions negatively affect perceptions for


sacrifice.

Though consumers consider price as less or more depend-


ing upon their financial situation, higher sacrifice percep-
tions have a negative impact on value perceptions, leading
to reduced willingness to pay (Dodds & Grewal, 1991).
Hence, perceived sacrifice is postulated to impact consum-
ers value perceptions negatively.

H3:
Perceived sacrifice is negatively related to per-
ceived value.

Consumers also use the extrinsic cue of price as a sugges-


tion of product quality (Brucks, Zeithaml, & Naylor, 2000;
Grewal, Krishnan, Baker, & Borin, 1998; Mastrobuoni,
Figure 1. Effect of Price and Brand Name Perceptions Peracchi, & Tetenov, 2014) under the belief that the forces
Source: Based on the conceptual framework. of supply and demand would lead to a natural ordering of
Sharma and Garg 27

products on a price scale, indicating a strong positive rela- Impact of brand name as a marketing cue: Priceperceived
tionship between price and quality of product (Scitovsky, value linkages explored in extant researches provide that
1945). Higher price may represent a belief that expensive when exposed to extrinsic product cues consumers do not
inputs are used, thus suggesting to consumers that they are just make judgements about product quality and sacrifice,
paying for superior quality and so have to pay more for a they also make judgements about uncertainties that may
product of high quality. However, subsequent studies con- pose potential long-term losses and there is significant
ducted by Kirchler, Fischer and Hlzl (2010), Steenkamp negative linkage between perceptions of risk and value
(1988), Bodell, Kerton and Schuster (1986) and Yamada (Agarwal & Teas, 2001). Consumers use a number of risk
and Ackerman (1984) have postulated a diminishing cor- reduction cues when purchasing products (Bearden &
relation coefficient for pricequality relationship in the Shimp, 1982; Cox, 1962; Locander & Hermann, 1979;
context of food products, asserting that high price might Roselius, 1971; Shimp & Bearden, 1982; Taylor, 1976).
reflect only enhanced competition and thus should not be According to Wood and Scheer (1996), brand name as an
construed by consumers as an indicator of quality. The extrinsic cue facilitates formation of consumers perceptions
studies have, however, confirmed that price effectively of riskthe uncertainty of consequenceswhich in turn
indicates higher product quality for non-food products. influence consumers perception of value. Brand name, a
The hypothesis, therefore, is: summary construct (Hart, 1989; Johansson, 1989) or a
shorthand cue is a statistically significant quality cue even
H4:Price perceptions positively affect perceptions for in the presence of a price and other extrinsic cues. Researches
quality. in the past operationalized favourability of brand name
and manipulated quality perceptions of select brands as
Consumers perception of quality is construed as their products of low or high quality and assessed its association
belief in the overall goodness of what is received i.e., with perceived risk (Dodds et al., 1991; Strizhakova, Coulter,
product or service (Dodds, 1995). It is the consumers & Price, 2011).
judgement about products excellence or superiority Consumers perceptions of risk increase with higher
(Zeithaml, 1988). Dodds (1995) empirically supports a levels of uncertainty and/or the chance of greater associ-
statistically significant and positive linkage for hypothe- ated negative consequences (Oglethorpe & Monroe, 1987).
sized perceived quality and value relationship for all cate- Sweeney, Soutar and Johnson (1999) specified two impor-
gories of products tested in the research. Based on extant tant dimensions of perceived risks, namely, performance
researches, the hypothesis is: and financial, and allowed for the measurement of their
role in affecting the linkage between extrinsic cues and
H5:Perceived quality is positively related to perceived perceived value. Performance risk is posited as the uncer-
value. tainty about whether the product will perform its intended
function, and financial risk is the uncertainty about how
Past researches further suggest that there is a dichotomy
much loss may have to be incurred for repair/maintenance
of information associated with price as a cue which influ-
of the product, and also an assessment of future costs
ences consumers product choice as consumers percep-
involved with acquiring a product (Agarwal & Teas, 2001).
tion of value involves a trade-off between perception of
Since there is strong support for direct negative linkage
quality and sacrifice (Agarwal & Teas, 2001; Dodds &
between dimensions of perceived risk and perceived value
Grewal, 1991; Erickson & Johansson, 1985; Lichtenstein
as indicated in past researches (Wood & Scheer,1996), we
et al., 1993; Monroe & Krishanan, 1985; Olson, 1977;
also hypothesize that:
Zeithaml, 1988). The trade-off, initially observed by
Scitovsky (1945), suggests a paradoxical situation in which
consumers view a low-priced product as affordable, but H8:Perceived risk in terms of perceived performance
less attractive because of its perceived inferior quality. risk is negatively related to perceived value.
Monroe and Krishnan (1985) extended this conceptualiza- H9:Perceived risk in terms of perceived financial risk
tion to provide a model relating price, perceived quality, is negatively related to perceived value.
perceived sacrifice and perceived value and suggest the
possible mediating effects of perceived quality and per- According to Bearden and Shimp (1982), a known brand
ceived sacrifice in priceperceived value linkages. The pro- name simplifies consumers product evaluations and miti-
posed hypotheses for testing these mediation effects are: gates uncertainty for the product by influencing percep-
tions of product quality. Given that there is a relationship
H6:Perceived sacrifice influences the effect of per- among the perceptions of brand name, quality, risk and
ceived price on perceived value. value, we nevertheless hold the opinion that the relation-
H7:Perceived quality influences the effect of perceived ships need further exploration to clearly establish the process
price on perceived value. by which brand name as a cue affects value perceptions.
28 Vision 20(1)

It appears logical to argue that the effect of uncertainties Research Methodology


towards future benefits and cost on value perception,
assessed on the basis of brand name as a cue, depends on A structured questionnaire was administered online to stu-
dents pursuing higher education in the metropolitan city
the assessment of present benefits and cost (Aghekyan-
New Delhi, largely belonging to the age group of 2025
Simonian et al., 2012; Beneke, Flynn, Greig, & Mukaiwa,
years. Respondents included 41.4 per cent males and 58.6
2013; Bicen, 2015). Thus, as against the theorized relation-
per cent females with 87.1 per cent residing in the city. The
ship of quality perceptions as a predictor of risk perception study was targeted particularly at the specified age group
(Sweeney et al., 1999), we seek empirical investigations to ensure that those surveyed are purchaser and users of
into the reversal of this relationship and hypothesize that: the selected product categories of mobile phones (durable
product category) and athletic shoes (semi-durable product
H10:Perceived performance risk negatively affects category). Since, respondents were asked to respond in the
perceptions of quality. context of both the products, care was taken to ensure that
H11:Perceived financial risk negatively affects percep- products are of use to both male and female populations,
tions of quality. which also helped us in avoiding biasness in their product
H12:Perceived quality influences the effect of per- evaluations. Also, the choice of products differing in terms
ceived financial risk on perceived value. of their technicality and complexities, allowed us the scope
H13:Perceived quality influences the effect of per- to ensure more variability in responses and thus permitted
ceived performance risk on perceived value. us to gauge perceptions in general and not specific to the
concerned product. The specific choice of product catego-
Based on hypothesized relationships, Figure 1 shows the ries also renders replication of previous studies focusing
conceptualized model where both the direct and indirect on athletic shoes.
effects of price and brand name perceptions are assessed to Using convenience sampling, a total of 300 question-
find out value perceptions on the part of Indian consumers. naires were distributed to the sample population and 116
Moreover, in a multi-cue scenario, with the presence of usable responses were received. The choice of sample
both price and brand name as extrinsic cues, consumers population in fact allowed us to relate this study with previ-
evaluations can be expected to differ significantly from a ous researches capturing consumers value perceptions
while working with the same age group. Table 1 reports the
situation where price is the only cue presented (Monroe &
related measures of various constructs based on the scales
Krishnan, 1985).
used by previous studies.
Impact of product familiarity and knowledge: During
PLS-SEM based analysis was done to test the hypo-
their search for a product within a specified category, con-
thesized relationshipsH1 to H13shown through the
sumers with different levels of familiarity1 and knowledge2
conceptualized model in Figure 1. The use of the PLS-
attend to marketing cues differently (Dowling & Staelin,
SEM framework was facilitated as sample size meets the
1994; Garbarino & Strahilevitz, 2004; Johnson & Kellaris,
minimum sample requirement for conducting an analysis
1988; Rao & Monroe, 1989). Lee and Lou (2011), in their in the PLS-SEM framework. Accordingly, sample size is
study, indicated that consumers with developed knowledge required to be 10 times the maximum number of paths
structures would depend differently in their usage of price aiming at any construct in the measurement model and the
as a cue. As consumers level of familiarity and/or know- structural model (Barclay, Higgins, & Thompson, 1995).
ledge increases, they acquire product memory associations, Respondents were also specifically asked about their
leading to the development of a better evaluative schema level of familiarity and knowledgeability for each of
(Rao & Monroe, 1988). They develop the ability to relate the product categories. Participants provided responses to
intrinsic cues with product value and later substitute these statements seeking to delineate their search behaviour with
intrinsic cues with surrogates such as information about respect to search characteristics looked for, search time
brand and price to indicate product value. It is, therefore, expended and search source referred to, in the case of
possible that if familiar consumers believe that price is both products. They were then asked to provide a brand
not a better indicator of product value, they would switch name for each product category with which they were
over to other cues to assess product value. comfortable in forming opinions about quality, sacrifice,
Further, the extent of effect of extrinsic cues would risk (performance risk, financial risk) and value. The idea
also depend upon consumers level of familiarity and of seeking a brand name from the participants was to
knowledge about the product class (Lee & Lou, 2011; Rao ensure that the name is working as a cue in the consumers
& Monroe, 1988). In the emerging market context, there- mind and they are able to assess products on the basis of
fore, our effort is to find out the instances of different specified brand name working as a stimulus in their cogni-
levels of familiarity and knowledge influencing consum- tive evaluations. It is assumed that when the brand name
ers search and so to identify the consumers who are is self-specified, participants have paid attention to it, con-
likely to rely more on extrinsic cues for making their sidered it in decision situations and/or purchased it some-
value perceptions. time in the past.
Sharma and Garg 29

Table 1. Various Constructs and the Scale Items

Constructs Statements Previous Research


Perceived value 1.This product is a: Very Good Value for MoneyVery Poor Dodds and Grewal (1991)
2.At the price shown, this product is: Very EconomicalVery Uneconomical
3.I consider this product to be a good buy: Strongly AgreeStrongly
Disagree*
4.5. This product appears to be a bargain: Strongly AgreeStrongly
Disagree*
Perceived quality 1.The likelihood that this product would be reliable is: Very HighVery Low Dodds and Grewal (1991)
2.The workmanship of this product is probably: Very HighVery Low*
3.The quality of this product is likely to be: Very HighVery Low
4.The likelihood that this product is dependable is: Very HighVery Low
5.This product is likely to be durable: Strongly AgreeStrongly Disagree*
Perceived sacrifice 1.If I purchased the product for the indicated price, I would not be able Teas and Agarwal (2000)
to purchase some other products I would like to purchase now.
Strongly AgreeStrongly Disagree
2.If I purchased this product for the indicated price, I would have to reduce
the amount of money I spend on other things for a while: Strongly
AgreeStrongly Disagree
Perceived 1.How confident are you that the product will perform as described? Dowling (1999); Teas and
performance risk Very ConfidentNot at all Confident Agarwal (2000)
2.How certain are you that the product will work satisfactorily?
CertainUncertain
Perceived financial 1.Considering the potential investment involved, for you to purchase the Dowling (1999); Teas and
risk product would be: Not Risky At AllVery Risky* Agarwal (2000)
2.I think that the purchase of the product would lead to financial risk for
me because of the possibility of such things as higher maintenance/repair
cost: ImprobableVery probable
3.Given the potential expenses associated with purchasing the product,
how much overall financial risk is associated with purchasing the product:
Very Little RiskSubstantial Risk
Perceived price 1.The price shown for this product is: Very AcceptableVery Unacceptable* Dodds and Grewal (1991)
Source: Dodds and Grewal (1991); Dowling (1999) & Teas and Agarwal (2000).
Notes: *Items dropped in model testing using SmartPLS version 2.0 to improve scales reliability and validity.

Results and Analysis amount of time during search, the less knowledgeable
consumers include brand in their search characteristics, and
At the outset, the analysis of consumers search tendencies advertisements are used as a search source by highly know-
with regard to select products shows that consumers with ledgeable consumers. This analysis is based on analysis
less familiarity and knowledge tend to spend the least of variance results found significant at p < 0.05 (Table 2).

Table 2. Search Behaviour and Its Relation with Product Familiarity and Knowledge: ANOVA Analysis

Level of Familiarity1 ANOVA Level of Knowledge2 ANOVA


Search Behaviour Mean F p-value Mean F p-value
Search source Advertisements 3.12 0.854 0.427 2.98 4.354 0.014
In store inspection 3.11 2.73
Word of mouth 2.94 2.44
Search Features 3.12 1.459 0.235 2.87 4.060 0.019
characteristics Appearance 2.82 2.43
Brand 3.01 2.55
Search Time Less than 2 hours 2.81 7.439 0.000 2.39 8.880 0.000
24 hours 3.06 2.78
46 hours 3.63 3.19
More than 6 hours 3.41 3.17
Source: Results generated after analysis of primary survey data collected and summarizes the results for hypotheses testing.
Notes: 1 Four-point scale ranging from Not at all familiar to Highly familiar.
2 Four-point scale ranging from Not at all knowledgeable to Highly knowledgeable.
30 Vision 20(1)

Figure 2. Path Model1


Source: Analysis of survey data using SmartPLS software.
Notes: Refer Table 1 for details of the items retained in the final model.

Model testing: Theorized relations proposed through All multi-item measurement scales are internally con-
H1 to H13 were systematically tested in a partial least sistent with their composite reliability scores of more than
squares structural equation model, using SmartPLS 0.70, and Average Variance Extracted (AVE) of more than
version 2.0. The proposed path model (Figure 2) built with 0.50, this suggests convergent validity of these scales
the help of SmartPLS software conformed to the criterion (Table 3). Discriminant validity of measurement scales is
laid down for evaluating the measurement model and also evident as cross loadings of the items constituting
the structural model. these scales are found less than their loading on the con-
To allow reliability and validity of the constructs as structs (see Appendix) for which they are used as measure.
a part of evaluating measurement models, one item of R2 measure provides the extent of explained variation
financial risk, two items of perceived value and two items for each of the endogenous constructs, namely, perceived
from perceived quality were removed due to their poor quality, perceived sacrifice and perceived value, as shown
loadings on the respective factors. Except one item of per- in Table 3; though as a rule of thumb the results suggest
ceived quality scale, and one item of perceived financial weak predictability of the model. Hair, Ringle and Sarstedt
risk scale, all other item loadings are more than 0.70 for (2011) study reports that R2 results of 0.20 are considered
the multi-item constructs, namely, perceived financial high in disciplines such as consumer behaviour. Since,
risk, perceived performance risk, perceived quality and value perceptions are measured as based on effect of price
perceived value. and brand name perceptions only, the inclusion of other

Table 3. Results of Constructs Reliability and Validity


No. of Composite Cronbachs
Constructs Items AVE Reliability R Square Alpha Communality Redundancy
Perceived financial risk 2 0.5697 0.7239 0 0.2508 0.5697 0
Perceived price* 1 1 1 0 1 1 0
Perceived quality 3 0.5679 0.7891 0.2576 0.6328 0.5679 0.0116
Perceived sacrifice 2 0.7689 0.8692 0.0003 0.7045 0.7689 0.0003
Perceived value 2 0.6949 0.8199 0.1194 0.5612 0.6949 0.0162
Perceived performance risk* 2 0.7267 0.8415 0 0.6278 0.7267 0
Source: Results generated after analysis of primary survey data collected and summarizes the results for hypotheses testing.
Notes: *Scores for scale items were recoded.
Sharma and Garg 31

marketing and non-marketing cues influencing value perceptions on value perceptions (b value of 0.2558),
perceptions may help in building the higher levels of the though found significant at p < 0.10, the total effect is
variation explained in the model. significant at p < 0.05 level. Clearly, this also suggests
Stone-Geissers Q2 (Geisser, 1974; Stone, 1974) measure the need to explore the effect of variables intervening in
was used to assess the predictive validity of the model, and the pricevalue relationship. Despite literature support sug-
blindfolding procedure of SmartPLS was used to obtain gesting hypothesized relationships H2, H3, H5, H8, H9 and
Q2 value for each endogenous construct. Blindfolding H11, this study failed to provide sufficient statistical support
procedure is only applied to exogenous constructs that to hold the relationships in the Indian context. Concern-
have a reflective measurement model and cross-validated ing perceived sacrifice, the lack of support to hypotheses
redundancy measure is used to obtain Q2 value, as it uses H2 and H3 seems to be due to the scale items used for the
PLS-SEM estimates of both the structural model and survey based research design. It is possible that the absence
measurement model for data prediction (Hair et al., 2011). of a specific price point for the reference of the respondents
At omission distancedchosen to be 8, as per the has resulted in their inadequate response to the scale items.
suggested range of 5 and 10 (Hair et al., 2011), constructs In the case of hypothesis H5 suggesting the effect of per-
cross-validity redundancy value is larger than zero (i.e., ceived quality on value perceptions, it could be argued
Q2 = 0.024) for the construct perceived value. According that the lack of support is in the light of the fact that
to Hair et al. (2011), if an endogenous constructs cross- quality perceptions could be formed independently of any
validated redundancy measure value (Q) for a certain cue whether intrinsic and/or extrinsic, whereas value
endogenous latent variable is larger than zero, its explana- perceptions has some reference point, and generally it is
tory latent constructs exhibit predictive relevance. The the price. Besides, in past studies manipulation of quality
explanatory latent constructs, namely, perceived price, levels was used to suit the needs of experimental research
perceived quality, perceived sacrifice, perceived financial design. Lack of support for hypothesis H8 can be viewed in
risk and perceived performance risk, thus have predictive the perspective of research findings supporting indirect
relevance in assessing consumers value perceptions. effect of perception of performance risk on value percep-
Hypotheses testing: Table 4 shows the individual path tions (H13). Regarding perceived financial risk, the scale
coefficients (b coefficients) and their significance testing items finally retained in the model, though, have a high
(t-test value) by means of bootstrapping. composite reliability score (0.7239), the Cronbach alpha
Study supports the hypothesized relationship H4 and value is low. Moreover, scale items do not seem to fit well
H10 at significance level of p < 0.05. Accordingly, price in the context of one of the selected products, namely,
perceptions positively affect perceptions for quality (H4); athletic shoes.
perceived performance risk negatively affects perceptions Testing of mediation effect: According to the Baron
for quality (H10). For hypothesis H1, the effect of price and Kenny (1986) approach, a variable M is said to have

Table 4. Path Coefficients

Path Coefficient (b) Total Effect


Hypotheses Path (t-statistic)1 (t-statistic)1
H1 Perceived price --> Perceived value 0.2558 0.2815
(1.8401)** (2.4117)*
H2 Perceived price --> Perceived sacrifice 0.0167 0.0167
(0.2561) (0.1462)
H3 Perceived sacrifice --> Perceived value 0.0394 0.0394
(0.3298) (0.1865)
H4 Perceived price - -> Perceived quality 0.4315 0.4315
(2.2749)* (1.6888)**
H5 Perceived quality --> Perceived value 0.0581 0.0581
(0.4714) (0.2979)
H8 Perceived performance risk --> Perceived value 0.0992 0.1134
(1.2142) (0.9512)
H9 Perceived fin risk --> Perceived value 0.098 0.1021
(1.1804) (0.984)
H10 Perceived performance risk --> Perceived quality 0.244 0.244
(2.2049)* (2.1606)*
H11 Perceived fin risk --> Perceived quality 0.0712 0.0712
(0.9951) (0.7798)
Source: Results generated after analysis of primary survey data collected and summarizes the results for hypotheses testing.
Notes: *Significance level p < 0.05; **Significant at p < 0.10.
1Bootstraaping procedure used with 116 case and 5,000 sample runs.
32 Vision 20(1)

Table 5. Test of Mediation Effect Using PLS and Sobel Test

Total Effect Direct and Indirect Effect Sobel Test


Path Coefficients1 Path Coefficient1b 2-tail
Hypotheses Path (t-statistics)2 (t-statistic)2 Test Statistic Prob.
H6: PPPS 0.0509 (0.7837) 0.4738 0.6356
PPPSPV PSPV 0.0835 (0.5951)
PPPV 0.3208 (2.0961)* 0.325 (2.9202)*
H7: PPPQ 0.4654 (2.581)* 0.54008 0.58914
PPPQPV PQPV 0.066 (0.5529)
PPPV 0.3130 (3.9069)* 0.2823 (2.0961)*
H12: PFRPQ 0.159 (1.7455)** 1.2869 0.19813
PFRPQPV PQPV 0.2213 (1.9055)**
PFRPV 0.1725 (1.4662) 0.1373 (1.367)
H13: PPRPQ 0.3392 (4.3945)* 2.27 0.02321*
PPRPQPV PQPV 0.2575 (2.6851)*
PPRPV 0.1345 (1.144) 0.0472 (0.58677)
Legend: PP, perceived price; PS, perceived sacrifice; PV, perceived nalue; PFR, perceived financial risk; PQ, perceived quality; PPR,
perceived performance risk.
Source: Results generated after analysis of primary survey data collected and summarizes the results for hypotheses testing.
Notes: *Significant at p < 0.05; **Significant at p < 0.10.
1Individual path models constructed using SmartPLS, version 2.0in the case of each of the hypothesis.
2Bootstraaping procedure used with 116 case and 5,000 sample runs.

mediating effect on X " Y when (i) X significantly affects mediation effect of perceived quality on the relation
Y, (ii) X significantly affects M and (iii) M significantly between consumers risk perceptions and value percep-
affects Y controlling for X. The results of testing of indirect tions. The strength of indirect effect as per Sobel test is,
effect (Table 5) do not confirm the fulfilling of first two however, found to be weak in the case of hypothesized
conditions for mediation effect in the case of H6. Perceived relationship (H12) and in this study we could accept H12
sacrifice, which does not interact with price perceptions partially only. The hypothesized relationship H13 which
and also not significantly related to either price or value suggests perceived quality influencing the effect of per-
perceptions, however, may be treated as a homologizer ceived performance risk on value perceptions is accepted
variable. It could be due to large error terms that the homol- on the basis of establishing its mediating effect. Quality
ogizer deems to be a moderator variable leading to differ- perceptions fully mediate the relation between brand per-
ent predictive validity coefficients between subgroups for ceptions and value perceptions, as the direct effect tends to
the predictor variable (Sharma, Durand, & Gur-Arie, 1981). reduce on the introduction of mediating termperceived
The use of subgroup analysis may further help in clearly quality. Sobel test further supports the full mediation effect
establishing the influence of consumers perception of as the interacting term is significant at p < 0.05 (Table 5).
sacrifice on the relation between perceptions of price and
value. For lack of statistical support suggesting perceived Discussions and Implications
sacrifice as a mediator or a moderator, the study rejects the
hypotheses H6, which states the influence of perceived sac- In emerging economies, generally, price is one of the
rifice on the effect of perceived price on perceived value major considerations affecting consumers decision making.
(Table 5). In the case of H7, although, price-perceptions Contrary to the notion of viewing consumers in emerging
significantly affect the quality perceptions and also the markets as highly price sensitive, the results indicate that
total effect of price on value perceptions as compared to price of a product is increasingly looked upon as an indica-
direct effect is higher, but for insignificant effect of per- tor of product quality. Research findings clearly show the
ceived quality on value perceptions, its influence cannot be positive relationship of price perceptions with quality per-
treated as mediation effect. Sobel test further provides ceptions and with value perceptions. Consumers associate
support as the indirect effect is not significantly different high price with the presence of better product features,
from zero at p < 0.05 (Table 5). According to Sharma et al. assuming that a good quality product always comes with a
(1981) framework, perceived quality being the specified high price attached. As the price level increases, simple heu-
variable either appears to be exogenous, or antecedent to ristic, you get what you pay for is highly likely to be used
the said relationship. In this research we, therefore, reject by the consumers. Thus, the developing notion around price
the hypothesis H7 that perceived quality influences the as economical or expensive is very important in affecting
effect of price perceptions on consumers value perceptions. both, the quality perceptions and value perceptions.
In the case of hypotheses H12 and H13, the results confirm Use of a favourable brand name affecting consumers
Baron and Kenny (1986) conditions for establishing the decision is also an important aspect of marketing strategy as
Sharma and Garg 33

consumers give more consideration to brand performance effect on their perceptions about the price. Targeting, for
as a pre-requisite to forming their quality perceptions. example, those who are highly familiar and knowledge-
In the backdrop of strong positive linkage between price able, it is therefore imperative for marketers to clearly
and quality perceptions, it seems possible for marketers establish product advantages in terms of its features, ben-
to build and position a brand name for its higher level of efits etc. so as to adequately contribute to establishing the
performance in emerging markets too. Here, word of favourable price perceptions. However, the information
mouth publicity of the brand name may further help mar- should not fall into the realm of too much information,
keters in favourable brand name positioning as research as information overload may cause dysfunctional impact
findings also suggest word of mouth as one of the search on consumers decision making (Bartlett, 1969; Kelly &
sources, particularly for those who are less familiar and Fiske, 1951; Newell and Simon, 1972; Payne et al., 1993).
knowledgeable about the product. On the other hand, those with low levels of knowledge
In a nutshell, contrary to the notion that Indian consum- look for appearance and brand name (i.e., deciding on
ers consider the price of a product only as an indicator of the basis of stimuli (cues) available in the immediate envi-
the sacrifice needed on their part, it is the high price high ronment). These consumers, who may be in general novice
quality notion supported by the branded product that in their decision making, are likely to show stronger ten-
would make the marketer to gain better as Akdeniz et al. dencies towards impulse buying and on their own seek
(2013) also concluded in their study that consumers did surrogates, like brand name, for quicker processing that
not perceive a higher product quality with a stronger war- reduces their search and purchase time. Furthermore, a
ranty (or higher price) when brand reputation was low. brand name carrying a favourable image allays consumers
Thus, it appears that emerging economies are possibly uncertainty regarding brand performance and promises
shifting from looking to lower price product options to good quality. Creating positive brand perceptions particu-
alternatives which boast of a favourable brand name indi- larly through encouraging a favorable word of mouth
cated by a higher price level. This would also imply that opinion could be suggested in the light of the findings
marketers can quickly adjust the positioning of a product of the current research. Offering product demonstrations
through price modifications instead of re-instatement of and self-trial of product may also assuage associated
brand name; however, consumers expectations of a highly product performance uncertainty and ease product trial.
priced product carrying superior features should not be In all, for marketers targeting consumers on the basis of
disappointed. The current research, thus, could be seen as their knowledge and familiarity levels or more specifically
extending Dodds and Grewals (1991) research which sug- those who tend to be more rational in their decision mak-
gests that price is a significant cue in affecting product ing as against those who are more impulsive or novice in
evaluations and marketers can adjust price to enhance their buying patterns, manipulation of marketing cues, like
value perceptions. price and brand name, offers a strong potential for design-
Furthermore, the investigation into consumers search ing marketing strategies in particular communication and
behaviour indicates that those who carry a higher level of retail strategies.
knowledge and familiarity focus more on product features The study paves the way for future studies. In view of
and spend a considerable time in searching for the product. the lack of support to many of the hypothesized relation-
Thus, what makes the price acceptable to consumers may ships (Table 6), future researches need to be cautious with
depend upon what do they search for and this will have an regard to the use of tested scale items as these were largely

Table 6. Summary of Tested Hypotheses

Serial No. Hypotheses Status


H1 Price perceptions positively influence perceived value Accepted
H2 Price perceptions negatively affect perceptions for sacrifice Rejected
H3 Perceived sacrifice is negatively related to perceived value Rejected
H4 Price perceptions positively affect perceptions for quality Accepted
H5 Perceived quality is positively related to perceived value Rejected
H6 Perceived sacrifice influences the effect of perceived price on perceived value Rejected
H7 Perceived quality influences the effect of perceived price on perceived value Rejected
H8 Perceived risk in terms of perceived performance risk is negatively related to perceived value Rejected
H9 Perceived risk in terms of perceived financial risk is negatively related to perceived value Rejected
H10 Perceived performance risk negatively affects perceptions of quality Accepted
H11 Perceived financial risk negatively affects perceptions of quality Rejected
H12 Perceived quality influences the effect of perceived financial risk on perceived value Partially accepted
H13 Perceived quality influences the effect of perceived performance risk on perceived value Accepted
Source: Results generated after analysis of primary survey data collected and summarizes the results for hypotheses testing.
34 Vision 20(1)

used in experimental research design. Future researches Further, only limited search aspects have been investigated
are needed to establish the robustness of the model when in this study as the purpose was to find out those who tend
cues other than brand name and price, such as store name, to rely more on marketing cues in their decision making.
warranties, advertising and packaging are presented to the The more extensive research into search behaviour, particu-
consumers. Along with marketing cues, the non-marketing- larly including contemporary search sources such as the
controlled cues as suggested by Akdeniz et al. (2013) also social media, and internet, may bring out interesting insights
have a role in assessing product quality. Extension of the for marketers with regard to designing of their communica-
model to include the interactions between marketing and tion strategies. While defining the major constructs under
non-marketing controlled cues could help in resolving intri- study, sacrifice is construed in a monetary sense and other
cacies involved in understanding consumers perception of dimensions such as time sacrificed, and efforts needed are
product value. The study has been confined to two catego- not tested. Furthermore, in addition to perceptions of finan-
ries of products only, namely, mobile phones and athletic cial risk and performance risk, other dimensions of per-
shoes; however, an attempt to explore the underlying con- ceived risks need to be examined for their role in affecting
ceptual model for a wider range of products is required. consumers product evaluations.

Appendix
Cross Loadings of Scale Items

Constructs
Perceived Fin Perceived Perceived Perceived Perceived Performance
Measures Risk Price Quality Sacrifice Value Risk
Maintenance cost 0.8273 0.1645 0.1407 0.3268 0.1463 0.015
Reduction in spending 0.1784 0.0634 0.2341 0.8408 0.0495 0.0536
Brand status 0.11 0.3153 0.1778 0.1641 0.8228 0.0132
Certainty 0.1103 0.0324 0.2133 0.0867 0.1283 0.8868
Confidence 0.0588 0.025 0.189 0.061 0.0663 0.8167
Dependability 0.1979 0.3887 0.8065 0.0645 0.0113 0.0253
Overall financial risk 0.6746 0.0837 0.1137 0.115 0.1047 0.1997
Price acceptability 0.1702 1 0.4353 0.0166 0.2951 0.034
Purchase inability 0.1412 0.0224 0.1009 0.9115 0.087 0.0956
Quality 0.1164 0.4408 0.8969 0.1792 0.2787 0.2491
Reliability 0.0821 0.0126 0.4987 0.1741 0.1444 0.2916
Value for money 0.1694 0.1807 0.1834 0.285 0.8442 0.1784

Notes Barclay, D. W., Higgins, C. A., & Thompson, R. (1995). The par-
1. Defined as how much a person thinks s/he knows about the tial least squares approach to causal modeling: Personal com-
product and the product category. puter adoption and use as illustration. Technology Studies,
2. Defined as how much a person knows about the intricacies of 2(2), 285309.
the product. Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderatormediator
variable distinction in social psychological research:
Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal
References of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173.
Agarwal, S., & Teas, R. K. (2001). Perceived value: Mediating Bartlett, C. J., & Green, C. G. (1969). Clinical prediction: Does
role of perceived risk. Journal of Marketing Theory and one sometimes know too much? Journal of Counseling
Practice, 9(4), 114. Psychology, 13(3), 267270.
Aghekyan-Simonian, M., Forsythe, S., Kwon, W. S., & Bauer R.A. (1960), Consumer behavior as risk taking, in
Chattaraman, V. (2012). The role of product brand image and R.S. Hancock (ed.), Dynamic marketing for a changing world,
online store image on perceived risks and online purchase Proceedings of the 43rd American Marketing Association
intentions for apparel. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Conference, Chicago, IL.
Services, 19(3), 325331. Bearden, W. O., & Shimp, T. A. (1982). The use of extrinsic
Akdeniz, B., Calantone, R. J., & Voorhees, C. M. (2013). Effective- cues to facilitate product adoption. Journal of Marketing
ness of marketing cues on consumer perceptions of quality: Research, 19(2), 229239.
The moderating roles of brand reputation and third-party Beneke, J., Flynn, R., Greig, T., & Mukaiwa, M. (2013). The
information. Psychology & Marketing, 30(1), 7689. influence of perceived product quality, relative price and risk
Anderson, S. P., De Palma, A., & Thisse, J. F. (1992). Discrete on customer value and willingness to buy: A study of private
choice theory of product differentiation. Cambridge, US: label merchandise. Journal of Product & Brand Management,
MIT Press. 22(3), 218228.
Sharma and Garg 35

Bicen, P. (2015). Consumer Perceptions of Quality, Risk, consumers evaluations and purchase intentions. Journal of
and Value: A Conceptual Framework, in Harlan E. Spotts Retailing, 74(3), 331352.
(ed.), In Revolution in Marketing: Market Driving Changes Hair, J. F., Ringle, C. M., & Sarstedt, M. (2011). PLS-SEM:
(pp. 119). USA: Springer International Publishing. Indeed a silver bullet. Journal of Marketing Theory and
Bilkey, W. J., & Nes, E. (1982). Country-of-origin effects on Practice, 19(2), 139152.
product evaluations. Journal of International Business Han, C. M. (1989). Country image: Halo or summary construct.
Studies, 13(1), 89100. Journal of Marketing Research, 26(2), 222229.
Bodell, R. W., Kerton, R. R., & Schuster, R. W. (1986). Price as a Han, C. M., & Terpstra, V. (1988). Country-of-origin effects for
signal of quality: Canada in the international context. Journal uni-national and bi-national products. Journal of International
of Consumer Policy, 9(4), 431444. Business Studies, 19(Summer), 235255.
Brucks, M., Zeithaml, V. A., & Naylor, G. (2000). Price and Jacoby, J. and Olson, J. C. (1977). Consumer response to price:
brand name as indicators of quality dimensions for consumer an attitudinal, information processing perspective, Moving
durables. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, ahead with attitude research, 39(1), 7397.
28(3), 359374. Janakiraman, N., Meyer, R. J., & Morales, A. C. (2006). Spillover
Cox, D. F. (1962). The Measurement of Information Value: A effects: How consumers respond to unexpected changes in
Study in Consumer Decision-Making, in William, S.D (Ed.), price and quality. Journal of Consumer Research, 33(3),
Emerging Concepts in Marketing (pp. 413421). Chicago: 361369.
American Marketing Association. Johansson, J. K. (1989). Determinants and effects of the use of
Degeratu, A. M., Rangaswamy, A., & Wu, J. (2000). Consumer made in labels. International Marketing Review, 6(1), 4758.
choice behavior in online and traditional supermarkets: The Johnson, R. L., & Kellaris, J. J. (1988). An exploratory study of
effects of brand name, price, and other search attributes. price/perceivedquality relationships among consumer ser-
International Journal of Research in Marketing, 17(1), 5578. vices. Advances in Consumer Research, 15(1), 316322.
Dodds, W. B. and Grewal, D. (1991). Effects of Price, Brand, and Kelley, E. J. (1958). The importance of convenience in consumer
purchasing. The Journal of Marketing, 23(1), 3238.
Store Information on Buyers Product Evaluation, Journal of
Kelly, E. L., & Fiske, D. W. (1951). The prediction of perfor-
Marketing Research, 28(3), 307319.
mance in clinical psychology. Ann Arbor, MI, US: The
Dodds, W. B. (1991). In search of value: how price and store
University of Michigan Press.
name information influence buyers product perceptions,
Kirchler, E., Fischer, F., & Hlzl, E. (2010). Price and its relation
Journal of Consumer Marketing, 8(2), 1524.
to objective and subjective product quality: Evidence from
Dodds, W. B. (1995). Market cues affect on consumers product
the Austrian market. Journal of Consumer Policy, 33(3),
evaluations. Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, 3(2),
275286.
5063.
Kwun, J. W., & Oh, H. (2004). Effects of brand, price, and risk
Dodds, W. B., & Monroe, K. B. (1985). The effect of brand
on customers value perceptions and behavioral intentions
and price information on subjective product evaluations. in the restaurant industry. Journal of Hospitality & Leisure
Advances in Consumer Research, 12(1), 8590. Marketing, 11(1), 3149.
Dodds, W. B., Monroe, K. B., & Grewal, D. (1991). Effects of Lee, M., & Lou, Y. C. (2011). Consumer reliance on intrinsic and
price, brand, and store information on buyers product evalu- extrinsic cues in product evaluations: A conjoint approach.
ations. Journal of Marketing Research, 307319. Journal of Applied Business Research (JABR), 12(1), 2129.
Dowling, G. R., & Staelin, R. (1994). A model of perceived risk Lichtenstein, D. R., Ridgway, N. M., & Netemeyer, R. G. (1993).
and intended risk-handling activity. Journal of Consumer Price perceptions and consumer shopping behavior: A field
Research, 21(1), 119134. study. Journal of Marketing Research, 30(2), 234245.
Dowling (1999). Perceived Risk. In Earl, P.E. & Kemp, S. (Eds.), Locander, W. B., & Hermann, P. W. (1979). The effect of self-
The Elgar Companion to Consumer Research and Economic confidence and anxiety on information seeking in con-
Psychology, Cheltenham, Edward Elgar, United Kingdom, sumer risk reduction. Journal of Marketing Research, 16(2),
UK. 268274.
Erdem, T., & Swait, J. (2004). Brand credibility, brand consid- Marian, L., Chrysochou, P., Krystallis, A., & Thgersen, J.
eration, and choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 31(1), (2014). The role of price as a product attribute in the organic
191198. food context: An exploration based on actual purchase data.
Erdem, T., Swait, J., & Valenzuela, A. (2006). Brands as sig- Food Quality and Preference, 37(October), 5260.
nals: A cross-country validation study. Journal of Marketing, Mastrobuoni, G., Peracchi, F., & Tetenov, A. (2014). Price as
70(1), 3449. a signal of product quality: Some experimental evidence.
Erickson, G. M., & Johansson, J. K. (1985). The role of price Journal of Wine Economics, 9(2), 135152.
in multi-attribute product evaluations. Journal of Consumer Monroe, K. B., & Dodds, W. B. (1988). A research program for
Research, 12(2), 195199. establishing the validity of the price-quality relationship.
Garbarino, E., & Strahilevitz, M. (2004). Gender differences in Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 16(1), 151168.
the perceived risk of buying online and the effects of receiv- Monroe, K.B. and Krishnan, R. (1985). The Effect of Price
ing a site recommendation. Journal of Business Research, on Subjective Product Evaluations, in Jacoby, J. and
57(7), 768775. Olson, J.C. (ed.), Perceived Quality: How Consumers View
Geisser, S. (1974). A predictive approach to the random effect Stores and Merchandise (pp. 209232), Lexington Books,
model. Biometrika, 61(1), 101107. Lexington, MA.
Grewal, D., Krishnan, R., Baker, J., & Borin, N. (1998). The Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving
effect of store name, brand name and price discounts on (Vol. 104, No. 9). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
36 Vision 20(1)

Oglethorpe, J. E., & Monroe, K. B. (1987). Risk perception and Sweeney, J. C., Soutar, G. N., & Johnson, L. W. (1999). The role
risk acceptability in consumer behavior: Conceptual issues of perceived risk in the quality-value relationship: A study
and an agenda for future research (pp. 255260). AMA in a retail environment. Journal of Retailing, 75(1), 77105.
Winter Marketers Educators Conference, Chicago, IL. Taylor, J.W (1976). The Role of Risk in Consumer Behavior,
Olbrich, R., & Christian Jansen, H. (2014). Price-quality rela- Journal of Marketing (Vol. 38, April, pp. 5450).
tionship in pricing strategies for private labels. Journal of Teas, R. K., & Agarwal, S. (2000). The effects of extrinsic prod-
Product & Brand Management, 23(6), 429438. uct cues on consumers perceptions of quality, sacrifice, and
Ofir, C., & Bechtel, G. G. (1990). Scaling and dimensionalizing value. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 28(2),
perceived risk from ratings data: Managers risk perception of 278290.
business computers. Marketing Letters, 1(2), 171179. Vanhuele, M., Laurent, G., & Dreze, X. (2006). Consumers
Olson, J. C. and Jacoby, J. (1972). Cue utilisation in the qual- immediate memory for prices. Journal of Consumer
ity perception process, in M. Venkatesan (Ed.), Advances Research, 33(2), 163172.
in Consumer Research (pp. 167179). Iowa City, Iowa: Wood, C. M., & Scheer, L. K. (1996). Incorporating perceived
Association for Consumer Research. risk into models of consumer deal assessment and purchase
Olson, J. C. (1977), Price as an Information Cue: Effects in intent. Advances in Consumer Research, 23(1), 399404.
Product Evaluations, in A. G. Woodside, J. N. Sheth, & Wheatley, J. J., & Chiu, J. S. (1977). The effects of price, store
P. D. Bennet (Eds.), Consumer and Industrial Buying image, and product and respondent characteristics on percep-
Behavior (pp. 267286), New York, NY: North Holland. tions of quality. Journal of Marketing Research, 181186.
Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., & Johnson, E. J. (1993). The adap- Yamada, Y., & Ackerman, N. (1984). Price-quality correlations
tive decision maker. New York, USA: Cambridge University in the Japanese market. Journal of Consumer Affairs, 18(2),
Press. 251265.
Purohit, D., & Srivastava, J. (2001). Effect of manufacturer repu- Zeithaml, V. A. (1988). Consumer perceptions of price, quality,
tation, retailer reputation, and product warranty on consumer and value: A means-end model and synthesis of evidence.
judgments of product quality: A cue diagnosticity framework. Journal of Marketing, July, 222.
Journal of Consumer Psychology, 10(3), 123134.
Rao, A. R., & Monroe, K. B. (1988). The moderating effect of
prior knowledge on cue utilization in product evaluations. Authors bio-sketch
Journal of Consumer Research, 15(2), 253264. Kavita Sharma is Professor of Marketing and Head,
(1989). The effect of price, brand name, and store name
Department of Commerce, Delhi School of Economics,
on buyers perceptions of product quality: An integrative
review. Journal of Marketing Research, 26(3), 351357.
University of Delhi. She has teaching and research experi-
Richardson, P. S., Dick, A. S., & Jain, A. K. (1994). Extrinsic ence of three decades. She has authored three books and has
and intrinsic cue effects on perceptions of store brand quality. her more than two dozen research papers published in
Journal of Marketing, 58(October), 2836. national and international journals and edited book volumes.
Roselius, T. (1971). Consumer rankings of risk reduction meth- She was the part of fifteen faculties selected from all over
ods. Journal of Marketing, 35(January), 5661. the world for participation in Faculty Consortium organized
Schmidt, J. B. and Spreng, R. A. (1996). A proposed model of by Academy of Marketing Science in Australia. She is on
external consumer information search, Journal of the acad- the reviewer board for international and national journals
emy of Marketing Science, 24(3), 246256. and has reviewed the papers for Industrial Marketing Man-
Scitovsky, Tibor (1945). Some Consequences of the Habit agement; Qualitative Market Research-An International
of Judging Quality by Price, Review of Economic Studies
Journal, Journal of Business Research. She was the founder
(Vol. 12, No. 32, pp. 100105).
Sharma, S., Durand, R. M., & Gur-Arie, O. (1981). Identification
editor of Journal of Commerce & Business Studies. She has
and analysis of moderator variables. Journal of Marketing presented paper in more than two dozen conferences and
Research, 18(3), 291300. has been invited as keynote speaker and panel speaker at
Shimp, T. A., & Bearden, W. O. (1982). Warranty and other international conferences and seminars. She is the research
extrinsic cue effects on consumers risk perceptions. Journal supervisor for Ph.D and M.Phil students.
of Consumer Research, 9(1), 3846.
Singh, J., & Sirdeshmukh, D. (2000). Agency and trust mech- Shivani Garg is pursuing Ph.D in Marketing from Depart-
anisms in consumer satisfaction and loyalty judgments. ment of Commerce, Delhi School of Economics, Delhi
Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 28(1), University. At present, she is an Assistant Professor in
150167. College of Vocational Studies, University of Delhi. She
Steenkamp, J. B. E. (1988). The relationship between price and has to her credit a book entitled Business Entrepreneur-
quality in the marketplace. De Economist, 136(4), 491507.
ship and Management. Her research interests focuses
Stone, M. (1974). Cross-validatory choice and assessment of
statistical predictions. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society.
on exploring branding and other cues effects in emerging
Series B (Methodological), 111147. markets and had presented research papers in IIM-L,
Strizhakova, Y., Coulter, R. A., & Price, L. L. (2011). Branding IP university and University of Delhi. She is amongst
in a global marketplace: The mediating effects of quality and top rank holders in her Post Graduation and Graduation
self-identity brand signals. International Journal of Research and had also supervised undergraduate students in research
in Marketing, 28(4), 342351. projects.

You might also like