You are on page 1of 153

Incubation

The Denitive Guide


to Science and Practice

The heart of the MacGyver Secret is incubation, plain and simple. It's the time that brings together
all of the other steps and taps into your inner MacGyver to solve any problem you ask. But, it's also
the most mysterious part of the system and the part most people understand the least.

So, to help you navigate, based on all the comments I received, I decided to put together an in depth
look at the science of incubation. This information is referenced in the book but has never before
been compiled in one place.

I want to warn you: a lot of this is dense science so be prepared for challenging reading. BUT, this is
top shelf material from the best minds in the eld.

Creativity - The Unconscious Foundations of the Incubation Period Page 1-10


Simone M. Ritter and Ap Dijksterhuis, Radboud University Nijmegen

Does Incubation Enhance Problem Solving? A Meta-Analytic Review Page 11-37


Ut Na Sio and Thomas C. Ormerod, Lancaster University

Dont Wait to Incubate: Immediate Versus Delayed Incubation in Divergent Page 38-47
Thinking Kenneth J. Gilhooly & George J. Georgiou & Jane Garrison & Jon D. Reston & Miroslav Sirota

Driven to Distraction: The Impact of Disaster Type on Unconscious Decision Making Page 48-63
Kibby McMahon, Betsy Sparrow, Ljubica Chatman, and Travis Riddle, Columbia University

Inspired by Distraction: Mind Wandering Facilitates Creative Incubation Page 64-70


Benjamin Baird, Jonathan Smallwood, Michael D. Mrazek, Julia W. Y. Kam, Michael S. Franklin and Jonathan W. Schooler

Neural Correlates of Subliminal Language Processing Page 71-80


Vadim Axelrod, Moshe Bar, Geraint Rees and Galit Yovel

On Making the Right Choice: The Deliberation-Without-Attention Eect Page 81-84


Ap Dijksterhuis

Probabilistic Word Pre-Activation During Language Comprehension Inferred Page 85-89


From Electrical Brain Activity
Katherine A DeLong, Thomas P Urbach and Marta Kutas

Some Experiments on the Recognition of Speech, with One and with Two Ears Page 90-94
E. Colin Cherry

The Automaticity of Everyday Life John A. Bargh, New York University Page 95-126

The Cognitive Unconscious John F. Kohlstrom

Think Dierent: The Merits of Unconscious Thought in Preference Development Page 127-135
and Decision Making Ap Dijksterhuis, University of Amsterdam

Thinking Fast and Slow Page 136-152


Daniel Kahneman, (New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux, 2011), 418 pp. Reviewed by Frank J. Babetski
REVIEW ARTICLE
published: 11 April 2014
HUMAN NEUROSCIENCE doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00215

Creativitythe unconscious foundations of the incubation


period
Simone M. Ritter* and Ap Dijksterhuis
Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, Netherlands

Edited by: Creativity is one of the most important assets we have to navigate through the fast
Matthijs Baas, University of changing world of the 21st century. Anecdotal accounts of creative individuals suggest
Amsterdam, Netherlands
that oftentimes, creative discoveries result from a process whereby initial conscious
Reviewed by:
thought is followed by a period during which one refrains from task-related conscious
Kirsten G. Volz, Werner Reichardt
Centre for Integrative Neuroscience, thought. For example, one may spend an embarrassing amount of time thinking about
Germany a problem when the solution suddenly pops into consciousness while taking a shower.
Ut Na Sio, Carnegie Mellon University, Not only creative individuals but also traditional theories of creativity have put a lot of
USA
Stephanie Lichtenfeld, University of
emphasis on this incubation stage in creative thinking. The aim of the present article
Munich, Germany is twofold. First, an overview of the domain of incubation and creativity is provided by
*Correspondence: reviewing and discussing studies on incubation, mind-wandering, and sleep. Second, the
Simone M. Ritter, Behavioural Science causes of incubation effects are discussed. Previously, little attention has been paid to
Institute, Radboud University the causes of incubation effects and most findings do not really speak to whether the
Nijmegen, Montessorilaan 3, P.O. Box
9104, 6500 HE Nijmegen, Netherlands
effects should be explained by unconscious processes or merely by consequences of a
e-mail: S.Ritter@psych.ru.nl period of distraction. In the latter case, there is no need to assume active unconscious
processes. The findings discussed in the current article support the idea that it is not
merely the absence of conscious thought that drives incubation effects, but that during an
incubation period unconscious processes contribute to creative thinking. Finally, practical
implications and directions for future research will be discussed.

Keywords: creativity, problem solving, incubation, mind wandering, sleep, unconscious processes

Important achievements in the arts and sciences depend on cre- Coutances, we entered an omnibus to go some place or other.
ativity (Feist and Gorman, 1998; Kaufman, 2002), and creativity is At the moment when I put my foot on the step, the idea came
associated with the development of new social institutions (Bass, to me, without anything in my former thoughts seeming to have
1990; Mumford, 2002) and economic growth (Amabile, 1997; paved the way for it, that the transformations I had used to define
Simonton, 1999). It is generally accepted that a creative idea or the Fuchsian functions were identical with those of non-Euclidian
geometry. I did not verify the idea; I should not have had the
a creative solution to a problem has to be novel (i.e., original)
time, as, upon taking my seat in the omnibus, I went on with a
and useful (Amabile, 1983; Runco and Pritzker, 1999). Creativity conversation already commenced, but I felt a perfect certainty. On
is not limited to the realms of greatness, but can also be found my return to Caen, for conscience sake, I verified the result at my
in daily life, for example, when one has to accomplish a task in a leisure. (Poincar quoted in Hadamard, 1945, p. 13).
new way (Cropley, 1990) or when one has to adapt to changes
(Runco, 2004). Todays world of continuous change thrives on In addition, several famous anecdotes suggest that sleep facil-
creative individuals. Creativity has been related to cognitive abili- itates creativity, ranging from musical compositions to scientific
ties, expertise, and practice (Patrick, 1986; Amabile, 1996; Runco, insights (Mazzarello, 2000). In speaking of the attainment of
2004; Ericsson, 2006; Sawyer, 2012), and one may expect that cre- solutions, Beatle Paul McCartney announced that he came up
ativity mainly thrives on extensive conscious thought. However, with the melody for Yesterday in a dream, and the Nobel Prize
creative individuals, in describing their work habits or the process winner Loewi woke up with the idea for how to experimentally
of creative problem solving, have suggested that oftentimes, cre- prove his theory of chemical neurotransmission. The idea that
ative ideas result from a period of incubationa process whereby a period of incubation might facilitate creativity has not only
initial conscious thought is followed by a period during which been suggested by creative minds, but has also been stressed
one refrains from task-related conscious thought (for anecdotal in creativity models. Wallas (1926) proposed that the creative
accounts, see Ghiselin, 1952). The most frequently cited anecdote process entails four stages: Preparation (acquisition of knowledge
is probably the one from the mathematician Poincar: to some task), Incubation (process that occurs when conscious
attention is diverted away from the task), Illumination (creative
[. . .] I left Caen, where I was living, to go on a geologic excursion idea flashes into sight), and Verification (creative idea is subjected
under the auspices of the School of Mines. The incidents of the to evaluation). Certainly a creative idea may be found before a
travel made me forget my mathematical work. Having reached decrease in conscious effort, that is, before the Incubation stage.

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org April 2014 | Volume 8 | Article 215 | 1

1
Ritter and Dijksterhuis Creativityunconscious foundations of incubation

Sometimes, however, a period of incubation seems to precede surrounding incubation effects, Dodds et al. (2003) conducted
creative breakthroughs as illustrated above for several scientific a review of experimental literature on incubation in problem
discoveries and artistic compositions. Sparked by the anecdotal solving and creativity, and revealed that 29 out of 39 experi-
accounts on incubation and creativity, various attempts have ments have found a significant effect of incubation. The authors
been made to investigate incubation effects. As demonstrated suggested that incubation length and preparatory activities can
by a Google Scholar search (Sio and Ormerod, 2009), with the increase incubation effects. Moreover, the authors demonstrated
search restricted to the years 19972007 and the subject areas to that presenting a clue during the incubation period can either
social sciences, arts, and humanities, the term incubation along have strong positive (if the clue is useful) or negative effects (if
with either creativity, insight, or problem, yielded more than 5000 the clue is misleading). For example, ocean or floor could
articles. Empirical research has shown that a period of incubation be a misleading clue when trying to find a fourth word that
indeed helps creativity (Dodds et al., 2003; Sio and Ormerod, functions as an associative link between the three items ship,
2009). outer, crawl, whereas space could be a useful clue. Sio and
However, it is not yet clear why incubation is helpful. The Ormerod (2009) conducted a statistical meta-analytic review
moderators discovered thus far do not really speak to whether the of empirical studies of incubation. In their meta-analysis 117
effects should be explained by unconscious processes or merely by independent studies were included, and the contributions of
other consequences of a period of distraction (e.g., relaxation, for- moderators such as problem type, presence of cues, and lengths
getting of fixating elements, mental set-shifting) without the need of preparation and incubation periods were investigated. Over-
to assume active unconscious processes (see also Orlet, 2008). The all, a positive incubation effect was found. In a recent study,
aim of the current article is to provide an overview of the domain Gilhooly et al. (2013) investigated interactions between the type
of incubation and creativity, and to review and discuss findings of creativity task (verbal or spatial) and the type of incubation
that speak to whether during an incubation period unconscious activity (verbal or spatial) on creative performance. Experimental
processes contribute to creative thinking, or whether it is merely groups, after 5 min of conscious work on a verbal creativity
the absence of conscious thought that drives creativity. Finally, task (Alternative Uses Task) or a spatial creativity task (Mental
practical implications and directions for future research will be Synthesis), had a 5-min incubation period that involved either
discussed. spatial (Mental Rotation) or verbal (Anagrams) tasks. Following
incubation, participants resumed their main task for a further
INCUBATION 5 min. Control groups undertook Alternative Uses or Mental
Many anecdotal accounts and traditional theories of creativity Synthesis for 10 min without any incubation periods. Significant
have put emphasis on incubation. The basic phenomenon is a incubation effects were found overall and there were interac-
familiar one: we are working on a problem, we cant solve the tions in that spatial incubation benefited verbal fluency and
task, we leave it aside for some period of timethe incubation verbal-rated creativity, and verbal incubation benefited spatial-
periodand when we return attention to the task we have some task fluency and spatial-rated creativity but not vice versa. These
new insight that helps us to solve the problem. In general, there are findings suggest that an interpolated incubation activity of a
two frequently used methods to conduct incubation experiments. dissimilar nature to the target task leads to stronger effects of
In the interpolated activity method, participants work on a task incubation as compared to an interpolated activity similar to the
or problem for a period of time, are then given an incubation target task.
period, and finally return to the task. Participants performance is Not only the task that is performed during an incubation
compared with that of a control group of people who worked on period, but also the time interval of an incubation period can
the same problem continuously. In the multiple trial or multiple vary. It can vary from a few moments or a night of sleep
item method, multiple tasks or problems are presented and, through days or weeks away from the problem. An example of
afterwards, items that have not been solved are re-administered. a relatively short incubation period is mind-wanderinga state
For example, participants work for one minute apiece on several of mind that occurs spontaneously, and largely autonomously,
problems. Then, the unsolved problems are re-administered for whenever an awake individual is not engaged in a cognitively
one minute apiece. It is presumed that the time between the first demanding task. Research on mind-wandering has a long his-
and second encounter with the tasks or problems allows incuba- tory, and was recently popularized by Smallwood et al. (2003)
tion to occur. Some researchers using this approach also insert who used thought sampling and questionnaires to investigate
an incubation period between the first and second encounter mind-wandering. In past and recent literature, alternative names
with the problems. For example, after the first encounter with to the term mind-wandering (Smallwood and Schooler, 2006;
the problems participants perform a distractor task and after this Mason et al., 2007) have been used, such as day dreaming
incubation interval return to work for a certain time on problems (Giambra, 1979), spontaneous thought (Christoff et al., 2011),
that they did not solve. Participants performance is compared task-unrelated thought (Giambra and Grodsky, 1989; Small-
with that of a control group, and if a within-participant design wood et al., 2003), and stimulus independent thought (Teas-
is used, the increase in number of problems is used. dale et al., 1995). In a recent study, Baird et al. (2012) exam-
Whereas some studies reported strong incubation effects (e.g., ined whether creative performance was facilitated differentially
Kaplan, 1989; Smith and Blankenship, 1989; Smith and Dodds, by engaging in mind-wandering (i.e., a 0-back task, an unde-
1999; Dodds et al., 2003), others have failed to find any effects at manding task without memory load that has been shown to
all (e.g., Olton and Johnson, 1976). To resolve the uncertainties elicit mind-wandering, Smallwood et al., 2009), a demanding

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org April 2014 | Volume 8 | Article 215 | 2

2
Ritter and Dijksterhuis Creativityunconscious foundations of incubation

task (i.e., a 1-back working memory task), a rest period, or no In a recent structural MRI study, Khn et al. (2013) provided
break between creativity problems. To measure creative perfor- further support for the involvement of the default mode network
mance, the Unusual Uses Task (a task that requires participants in creativity. Participants performed a well-established creativity
to generate as many unusual uses as possible for a common task by which a participants cognitive flexibility and the average
object) was used. All participants performed two Unusual Uses uniqueness and average creativity of a participants ideas were
Task problems (2 min per problem) to measure baseline creative assessed. For all psychometric measures of creativity a positive
performance. Subsequently, participants were assigned to one correlation was observed between inter-individual differences in
of the four between-subjects conditions. After the incubation creative performance and inter-individual differences in volume
interval (or following the baseline measure, in the case of the of the default mode network. Based on these findings, it can be
no-break condition), participants worked on the Unusual Uses assumed that greater volume in the default mode network (i.e., in
Task again. Four problems (2 min per problem) were presented the counterpart of the cognitive control network) provides more
in a random order: two problems that were identical to the neural resources for generating creative ideas. These findings
problems presented at baseline and two new problems. Engag- suggest that less controlled processes such as mind-wandering are
ing in an undemanding task during an incubation period led important in creativity. One relatively controversial finding is that
to significant increases in creative solutions to the target prob- periods of mind-wandering are associated with increased activa-
lems as compared to the demanding task, rest, and no break tion in both the default and executive system, a result that implies
conditions. This improvement was observed only for repeated- that mind-wandering may often be goal oriented (Smallwood
exposure problems, which demonstrates that it resulted from and Schooler, 2006; Smallwood et al., 2009). Apart from studies
an incubation process rather than a general increase in creative about the default mode network, there are several important other
problem solving. The unrelated thoughts that occur during mind studies on neuroimaging and creativity. For example, the research
wandering uniquely seem to facilitate incubation. According to from Reverberi et al. (2005) demonstrates that the lateral frontal
Baird et al., one possible explanation may be that mind wan- cortex impairs problem solving, and the research by Kounios
dering enhances creativity by increasing unconscious associative and Jung-Beeman (2009) on the cognitive neuroscience of insight
processing, as predicted by the spreading-activation account of suggests that insight is the culmination of a series of brain states
incubation (e.g., Yaniv and Meyer, 1987; Dijksterhuis and Meurs, and processes operating at different time scales. Recently, Dietrich
2006). and Kanso (2010) reviewed 72 neuroimaging studies on creativity
In recent years, functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging and insight and concluded that the neuroscientific literature on
(fMRI) research has been used to focus on understanding how creativity, thus far, is self-contradicting and that creative thinking
the brain generates the spontaneous and relatively unconstrained does not appear to critically depend on any single mental pro-
thoughts that are experienced when the mind wanders. One cess or brain region. The default mode network can, therefore,
candidate neural mechanism for mind-wandering is a network be considered one, but not the single neural underpinning of
of regions in the frontal and parietal cortex known as the default creativity.
mode network (Mason et al., 2007; Christoff et al., 2009). The Whereas mind-wandering can be considered a relatively short
default mode network, also called the default network, default incubation period, sleep can be considered an incubation period
state network, or task-negative network, is defined as a set of inter- that covers a longer period of time. Sleep is divided into two broad
connected brain regions including the medial prefrontal cortex types, rapid eye movement (REM) sleep and non-rapid eye move-
(MPFC), posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), and lateral and medial ment (NREM) sleep. Each type has a distinct set of associated
temporal lobes (Spreng et al., 2010). It is a brain system that is physiological and neurological features. REM sleep is a stage of
especially active when an individual is not focused on the outside sleep characterized by the rapid and random movement of the
world (Buckner et al., 2008) and when cognitive control is low eyes, and typically occupies 2025% of total sleep. During REM,
(Andreasen, 1995). Moreover, it has been related to complex, eval- the activity of the brains neurons is quite similar to that during
uative and unconscious forms of information processing (Vincent waking hours and subjects vividly recalled dreams mostly occur
et al., 2007; Yang et al., 2010), and it contrasts with the cognitive during REM sleep. Unlike REM sleep, during NREM sleep there
control network (Fox et al., 2005)a set of brain regions includ- is usually little or no eye movement and dreaming is rare. The
ing the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), dorsolateral prefrontal differences in the REM and NREM activity reported is believed
cortex (DLPFC), inferior frontal junction (IFJ), anterior insular to arise from differences in the memory stages that happen
cortex (AIC), dorsal pre-motor cortex (dPMC), and posterior during the two methods of sleep (Manni, 2005). For example,
parietal cortex (PPC; Cole and Schneider, 2007). Indeed, when Stickgold et al. (1999) have shown that cognition during REM
one network is activated, the other is deactivated (Fox et al., sleep is qualitatively different from that of waking and NREM
2005). In addition to these findings, structural MRI research has sleep, and may reflect a shift in associative memory systems. They
provided a first indication that the default mode network may suggest that this shift in cognitive processing is responsible, in
be involved in creativity. Jung et al. (2010) have linked cortical large part, for the bizarre nature of dreams and may serve to
thickness measures to psychometric measures of creativity and enhance the strength of associations between weakly associated
found a negative correlation between creative performance and memories, an important skill underlying creative thinking. The
activity in the lingual gyrus and a positive correlation between mental activity that takes place during NREM sleep is believed
creative performance and grey matter volume in the right PCC, to be thought-like, whereas REM sleep includes hallucinatory
a brain area that is part of the default mode network. and bizarre content (Manni, 2005). Thus far, sleep research has

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org April 2014 | Volume 8 | Article 215 | 3

3
Ritter and Dijksterhuis Creativityunconscious foundations of incubation

mainly been focused on memory performance. A prominent sixteen, heart: . . . . . . . . .; the answer to this item is sweet: cookies
finding is that sleep, and certain stages of sleep in particular, are are sweet, sweet sixteen, sweetheart). Reaching a solution requires
important in memory processing, resulting in delayed learning creative thought as the first, most probable associate to each of
without the need for further practice or task engagement (Stick- the items is often not correct, so the participant must think of
gold et al., 2001). These findings of sleep-dependent learning more remote associations (i.e., distantly related information) to
are now strongly supported by cellular and molecular evidence connect the three words. In the current study the RAT items
of sleep-dependent plasticity across a broad range of phylogeny varied in difficulty as a function of the strength of the stimuli
(Bennington and Frank, 2003). Yet memory consolidation is only answer associations. After a period of sleep, wake, or no delay,
one of many cognitive virtues possessed by the human brain, participants reattempted earlier unsolved problems. The sleep
another is creativity. group solved a greater number of difficult RAT items than did
The link between creativity and sleep, especially dreaming, the other groups, but no difference was found for easier RAT
has long been a topic of intense speculation (Stickgold and items. These findings suggest that sleep facilitates creative think-
Walker, 2004). In recent years, the facilitatory effect of sleep ing for harder problems. While evidence for the role of sleep in
on creativity has also received empirical support. Research from creative problem-solving has been looked at by prior research,
Barrett (1993) has shown that college students incubated answers underlying mechanisms such as different stages of sleep had
to real-life homework and other objective problems on which not been explored. Cai et al. (2009) used the RAT, and tested
they were working, finding that in one weeks time, half of the participants in the morning, and again in the afternoon, after
students had dreamed about their topic and 25% had a dream either a nap with REM sleep, one without REM or a quiet rest
that provided an answer. Barrett (2001) also interviewed modern period. Participants grouped by REM sleep, non-REM sleep and
artists and scientists (including Nobel Prizes winners) about quiet rest were indistinguishable on measures of memory. Most
their use of their dreams and concluded that while anything importantly, although the quiet rest and NREM sleep groups
math, musical composition, business dilemmasmay get solved received the same prior exposure to the task, they displayed no
during dreaming, the two areas dreams are especially likely to improvement on the RAT test, whereas the REM sleep group
help are anything where vivid visualization contributes to the improved by almost 40% over their earlier performances. The
solution and any problem where the solution lies in thinking authors hypothesized that the formation of associative networks
outside the boxi.e., where the person is stuck because the from previously unassociated information in the brain, leading to
conventional wisdom on how to approach the problem is wrong. creative problem-solving, is facilitated by changes to neurotrans-
Moreover, in an experimental study Wagner et al. (2004) have mitter systems during REM sleep. Thus, REM sleep is assumed to
shown that sleep inspires creative insight. Subjects completed a enhance the integration of unassociated information for creative
number reduction task, and each numerical sequence could be problem solving.
completed in a slow, stepwise way, but the trials could also be To recap, various attempts have been made to investigate
completed according to a hidden, more abstract rule that would incubation effects in creativity and creative problem solving. The
speed up participants responses. The initial training was followed conclusion of the literature is that overall, a positive incubation
by 8 h of nighttime sleep, nighttime wakefulness, or daytime effect can be observed. Especially the work by Dodds et al. (2003)
wakefulness. Of the people who slept before they resumed, almost and Sio and Ormerod (2009), who conducted reviews of empirical
60% discovered the rule, as opposed to 23% of the people studies of incubation, justify the conclusion that incubation can
in the two groups that did not sleep. Thus, participants who enhance creative performance. This is also supported by research
got several hours of sleep were more than two times as likely on mind-wandering and sleep, which can be seen as short and
during retesting to gain insight into a hidden rule built into relatively long periods of incubation. However, the process(es)
the task. underlying incubation effects remain unclear. In the next section,
In addition, sleep has been shown to enhance important we aim to shed light on the question whether during an incuba-
aspects of creativity, including cognitive flexibility and the ability tion period unconscious processes contribute to creative thinking,
to find remote associations. In a study on cognitive flexibility or whether it is merely the absence of conscious thought that
across the sleep-wake cycle, Walker et al. (2002) found that when drives incubation effects.
woken from REM sleep, participants had a 32% advantage in the
number of anagrams solved compared with NREM awakenings, MECHANISMS UNDERLYING INCUBATION EFFECTS
which were equal to that of wake time performance. These find- Whereas the effects of incubation are generally accepted (Sio
ings suggest that REM sleep may offer a different mode of problem and Ormerod, 2009), its causes are controversial. The main
solving compared with wake and NREM. The authors hypothe- debate between different theories is about whether during an
sized that REM sleep is highly conducive to fluid reasoning and incubation period unconscious processes contribute to creative
flexible thought due to the lack of aminergic dominance in REM thinking (unconscious work theory), or whether it is merely the
sleep. In a study on the ability to find remote associations, Sio absence of conscious thought that drives creativity (conscious
et al. (2012) participants were presented with a set of Remote work theory). Historically, incubation effects refer to the idea
Associates Test (RAT) items. Each RAT item contains a triplet of that setting a problem aside for a while helps creative thought
words presented horizontally along with a blank space. For each and problem solving as unconscious processes are working on the
item, the participant has to find a fourth word that functions problem while the individual is not consciously thinking about
as an associative link between these three words (e.g., cookies, the problem (see Wallas, 1926, as well as, e.g., Hadamard, 1945;

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org April 2014 | Volume 8 | Article 215 | 4

4
Ritter and Dijksterhuis Creativityunconscious foundations of incubation

Kris, 1952; Rugg, 1963; Kubie, 1985). That is, the unconscious Betsch et al. (2001) demonstrated that people can unconsciously
actively thinks and contributes to solving a problem (see also integrate large amounts of information. Participants watched
Koestler, 1964; Claxton, 1997). In contrast, conscious work the- TV ads shown on a computer screen and simultaneously the
ories have ascribed incubation effects on creative performance numerical increases and decreases of hypothetical shares were
to relaxation (being well-rested, one can do better the next shown at the bottom of the screen. Participants could not cor-
time one engages in the problem; Helmholtz, 1896; Woodworth rectly answer specific questions about the shares, but they had
and Schlosberg, 1954) and to the effects of facilitating cues developed a liking or disliking towards each of the shares. These
from the environment (environmental cues trigger retrieval of findings suggest that participants processed and integrated the
previously un-retrieved relevant information; e.g., Yaniv and information while they were attending to the TV ads. Recently,
Meyer, 1987; Langley and Jones, 1988). Moreover, sometimes Ric and Muller (2012) have shown that people can unconsciously
old and inappropriate ideas can cause mental fixation, imped- initiate and follow arithmetic rules, such as addition. In several
ing the generation of new and appropriate ideas (Smith, 2003). studies participants were instructed to detect whether a symbol
Therefore, in addition to relaxation and facilitating cues, it has was a digit, and this symbol was preceded by two digits and a
been suggested that incubation effects can lead to forgetting of subliminal instruction (i.e., the add instruction or a control
fixating elements (Smith and Blankenship, 1989; Segal, 2004) instruction). Participants were faster at identifying a symbol as
and to mental set-shifting (wrong cues become less accessible, a number when the symbol was equal to the sum of the two digits
leading to a fresh, new and unbiased start; Schooler and Melcher, and they received the instruction to add the digits. In line with
1995). these findings, Sklar et al. (2012) demonstrated that presenting
Recently, Gupta et al. (2012) investigated whether high- participants with additions or subtractions subliminally leads to
frequency candidate answers should be avoided in order to find higher accessibility of correct answers (i.e., answers could be ver-
creative solutions in for instance a RAT. They tested individual balized faster) than incorrect answers. A recent review on uncon-
differences in creativity as measured with a complex problem- scious higher-order cognition conducted by Van Gaal et al. (2012)
solving task, and developed a computational model of the RAT. revealed strong evidence for unconscious response-inhibition,
Findings showed that individuals performed poorly on the RAT conflict resolution, as well as for error detection. Importantly, they
when they were biased to consider high-frequency candidate also concluded that people can unconsciously integrate multiple
answers. Storm and Angelo (2010) investigated whether inhibi- pieces of information across space and time.To resume, evidence
tion may facilitate creative problem solving by providing a mech- from various research areas demonstrates that processes that we
anism by which to bypass fixation. They measured participants consider thought processes can ensue unconsciously. This makes
retrieval-induced forgetting and, thereafter, participants had to it reasonable to assume that thought processes in the service of
solve RAT problems. Half of the participants were exposed to creativity and problem solving can, in principle at least, also take
misleading associates prior to problem solving (fixation condi- place unconsciously.
tion) and half were not (baseline condition). Correlating the The idea that during an incubation period unconscious pro-
retrieval-induced forgetting measure with performance on the cesses are active was one of the building blocks of Unconscious
RAT revealed that the propensity to inhibit irrelevant information Thought Theory (Dijksterhuis and Nordgren, 2006). Uncon-
comes at a price, as potentially relevant information may be scious thought, that is, deliberation in the absence of conscious
inhibited. However, inhibition can also provide a means by which attention directed at the problem, (Dijksterhuis et al., 2006,
to overcome fixation and, thereby, facilitate creativity. There is p.1005) has mainly been studied in the context of decision-
no denying that a period of distraction allows for forgetting of making (Strick et al., 2011; see also Dijksterhuis and Nordgren,
fixation and/or mental set-shifting, relaxation, and exposure to 2006; Bargh, 2011; Nieuwenstein and van Rijn, 2012). In the
environmental cues, and that these effects can contribute to cre- literature on unconscious thought in decision-making, partici-
ative thoughts or problem solving. However, it can be questioned pants are typically first presented with information pertaining
whether these effects are the only benefit of an incubation period, to a decision. Thereafter, they are distracted for a while, before
or whether during an incubation period unconscious processes they make a decision. For example, Bos et al. (2008) compared
contribute to creative thinking.1 participants decision performance after three conditions, a con-
Research from Bowers et al. (1990) suggests that the uncon- scious thought condition and two incubation conditions, that
scious is able to close in on the correct answer some time before is, an unconscious thought condition and a mere distraction
the answer is accessible to consciousness. They asked participants condition. Whereas participants in the unconscious thought con-
to find a target word while they were given successive hints, such dition were told that they would engage in an unrelated task
as an associated word. Individuals felt clueless for some time before returning to the actual task, participants in the mere
and then suddenly came up with the correct answer. However, distraction condition were told that they had finished the task
analysing the prior guesses revealed that individuals were slowly and would move on to unrelated tasks. In the mere distraction
getting closer to the right solution before the solution reached condition participants were, thus, distracted just as in the uncon-
consciousness. Participants successive guesses, thus, converged scious thought condition, but did not have a problem-solving
towards the correct answer. Moreover, a study conducted by goal. A period of distraction only improved decision-making in
the unconscious thought condition, that is, when participants
1 Note that the section below greatly overlaps with Dijksterhuis (submitted for expected to make a decision following the distraction period.
publication). Comparing an unconscious thought condition with a mere

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org April 2014 | Volume 8 | Article 215 | 5

5
Ritter and Dijksterhuis Creativityunconscious foundations of incubation

distraction condition provides evidence for true, active thought condition were told that they had finished the task and would
taking place unconsciously. Given the evidence for unconscious move on to unrelated tasks. To prevent conscious thought about
thought processes that we have from multiple research areas (e.g., the RAT items in the unconscious thought and mere distraction
decision making, lie detection), the question raises whether there conditions participants completed a 2-back task for 5 min (see
is also evidence for unconscious thought effects in the domain of Dijksterhuis (2004)). After 5 min of conscious thought or dis-
creativity. traction, all participants engaged in a lexical decision task (Bargh
Dijksterhuis and Meurs (2006) investigated the relation et al., 1995). Strings of letters appeared on the center of the
between different thought processes and the generation of cre- screen, and participants indicated whether or not each string
ative ideas. In several experiments participants were asked to constituted an English word by pressing one of two buttons. The
generate a list of items (new names for products, names of letter strings included the RAT answers plus control words. After
places beginning with a certain letter, things one can do with a completing the lexical decision task, participants in all three con-
brick), and three conditions were compared. In the immediate ditions were again shown the RAT items and were asked to report
condition (i.e., the baseline condition) participants started right their answers. In the current research two separate outcomes of
after receiving the instruction. In the conscious thought condi- the RAT test were assessed: implicit accessibility of correct RAT
tion, participants were given three minutes to consciously think answers (i.e., mental accessibility of RAT answers, as measured by
about the items before they were given time to list them. In a lexical decision task) versus expression of those correct answers
the unconscious thought condition, people were first given the (Wegner and Smart, 1997). A period of incubation, compared
instruction, and were then distracted for three minutes before with the same duration of conscious thought, did not increase the
they were given the opportunity to list the items. Conscious reporting of correct answers. The results on accessibility, however,
thought led to more accessible items and to items in line with revealed a striking difference: Unconscious thought, compared
a cue, whereas unconscious thought led to more inaccessible with conscious thought and mere distraction, increased the men-
items and to items diverging from the cue. Moreover, unconscious tal accessibility of RAT answers. These results are consistent with
thought led to more creative and unusual items than conscious unconscious thought theory, which systematically differentiates
thought. In all experiments, unconscious thinkers also differed conscious and unconscious thought processes, and suggest that
significantly from participants who were not given time to think unconscious processing is more adept at associating and integrat-
at all. These findings suggest that whereas conscious thought may ing information than conscious processing is (Dijksterhuis and
be focused and convergent, unconscious thought may be more Nordgren, 2006). Importantly, in the unconscious-thought con-
associative and divergent. Ritter et al. (2012b) investigated the dition the level of activation of RAT answers was higher than in
role of unconscious thought for both idea generation and idea the mere-distraction condition, which suggests that the increased
selection. Participants generated creative ideas immediately, after accessibility after unconscious thought was not due to relaxation,
conscious thought, or after a period of unconscious thought. forgetting or the release of incorrect associations (i.e., mental
After having listed their ideas, participants selected their most set-shifting). These findings indicate that unconscious processes
creative idea. Performance in idea generation was similar between may actively facilitate the discovery of remote associations, an
conscious and unconscious thought; however, individuals who important mental skill underlying creative thinking, and may
had unconsciously thought about ideas were better in selecting contribute to divergent thinking.
their most creative idea. These findings are in support of the Yang et al. (2012) investigated under what conditions uncon-
idea that unconscious processes actively contribute to creativity, scious thought can outperform conscious thought on creativity
as it is unlikely that these findings are the consequence of set- tasks. Their results demonstrated that unconscious thought did
shifting or relaxation. During task instruction no examples were not provide creative advantage over conscious thought when
provided and no hints or cues were given, meaning that no deliberation duration was either short or long (1 or 5 min,
fixating elements or specific mental sets were induced that could respectively). However, when deliberation duration was of a mod-
have become less accessible, changed, or forgotten altogether erate length (3 min), the creative output of unconscious thought
during a period of distraction. Recovering from fatigue is also exceeded that of conscious thought. These findings suggest that
unlikely to account for the current findings, as incubation effects the duration of unconscious thought has an inverted-U shaped
also occurred in the study when a cognitively demanding task (n- relationship with creativity. However, as different tasks require
back task) was used as distracter task (Dijksterhuis and Meurs, different amounts of mental effort, the appropriate duration of
2006). a moderate length can be assumed to be task dependent. In
Zhong et al. (2008) investigated the effect of unconscious line with these findings, a meta-analysis on unconscious thought
thought on the ability to find remote associations, as measured by effects on decision-making (Strick et al., 2011) has shown that
the RAT. Participants were presented difficult RAT triads (selected unconscious thought effects are larger with moderate uncon-
from Bowden and Jung-Beeman, 2003). Afterwards, participants scious thought intervals. Moreover, unconscious thought effects
in the conscious thought condition were told that they had 5 min in decision-making have been shown to be larger when a task is
to think about these triads, and during this time, they were shown used that does not require much processing capacity, that is, a
the screen containing all triads, but were not allowed to write relatively undemanding task. Similarly, Sio and Ormerods (2009)
down notes or answers. Participants in the unconscious-thought statistical meta-analytic review of incubation effects revealed that
condition were told that they would engage in an unrelated task the benefits of an incubation period are greater when participants
before returning to the word task. Participants in the distraction are occupied by an undemanding task than when they engage in

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org April 2014 | Volume 8 | Article 215 | 6

6
Ritter and Dijksterhuis Creativityunconscious foundations of incubation

either a demanding task or no task at all (Sio and Ormerod, 2009). the conclusions can be transferred to consciousness. Recently,
Moreover, Ellwood et al. (2009) demonstrated that the type of the first neuroscientific evidence into unconscious thought was
break during the incubation period effects later solutions. As the provided. As in earlier studies on unconscious thought and
functional fixedness theory as well as the general fatigue theory decision-making, Creswell et al. (2013) showed that uncon-
predict that a break, independent of its content, should be equally scious thinkers made better decisions than conscious thinkers
effective in producing an incubation effect, these findings suggest and than immediate decision makers. Moreover, their fMRI
that systematic effects beyond relief from functional fixedness or data demonstrated that participants who thought unconsciously
general fatigue are at play. while doing a distraction task showed more activity in the right
Gallate et al. (2012) and Ritter et al. (2012b) investigated DLPFC and left intermediate visual cortex than participants who
whether one can manipulate unconscious thought processes. In merely performed the same distraction task. These areas were
the study from Gallate et al., participants were either aware already involved in the initial encoding of the information in
or unaware that they would soon be returning to a divergent the first place, and the authors proposed a neural reactivation
thinking task. During the break period, all participants were account for unconscious thought, indeed demonstrating uncon-
distracted from the task (they did an arithmetic task), ensur- scious processing to continue after encoding. Importantly, neural
ing that any ongoing problem solving was not conscious, but reactivation in the right DLPFC and left intermediate visual
unconscious. Immediately after finishing the arithmetic task, par- cortex was predictive of decision quality of unconscious thinkers.
ticipants returned to the divergent thinking task. Participants in Further neuroscientific research on creativity and incubation
the aware condition had significantly higher post-break creativity should investigate brain activity during incubation to shed fur-
scores than those in the unaware condition. Ritter et al. (2012a) ther light on the underlying cognitive mechanisms of incubation
investigate whether one can actively enhance the beneficial effect effects.
of sleep on creativity by covertly reactivating the creativity task To conclude, several studies suggest that it is not merely the
during sleep. Individuals creative performance was compared absence of conscious thought that drives creativity, but that dur-
after three different conditions: sleep-with-conditioned-odor; ing an incubation period unconscious processes can contribute to
sleep-with-control- odor; or sleep-with-no-odor. In the evening creative thinking. Often, it takes time to come up with creative
prior to sleep, all participants were presented with a problem that ideas and solutions. It is reasonable to assume that most thought
required a creative solution. In the two odor conditions, a hidden processes underlying creative thought are neither fully conscious
scent-diffuser spread an odor while the problem was presented. nor fully unconscious. Instead, prolonged creative thought pro-
In the sleep-with-conditioned-odor condition, task reactivation cesses may have both conscious and unconscious elements, and
during sleep was induced by means of the odor that was also conscious and unconscious thought may alternate. You think
presented while participants were informed about the problem. In about a problem consciously, you get stuck and perform another
the sleep-with-control-odor condition, participants were exposed task, you think some more, you sleep on it for a while, you
to a different odor during sleep than the one diffused during then think a bit more after youve encountered relevant new
problem presentation. In the no odor condition, no odor was information, et cetera.
presented. After a night of sleep with the conditioned odor, par-
ticipants were found to be more creative and better able to select PRACTICAL APPLICATIONS AND FUTURE RESEARCH
their most creative idea than participants who had been exposed Previous studies have shown that creativity training can enhance
to a control odor or no odor while sleeping. Task reactivation everyday creative performance (e.g., Scott et al., 2004), and many
during sleep seems to actively trigger creativity-related processes tactics have been identified to facilitate creative thinking skills,
during sleep. These findings give a first indication that one can such as set-shifting, questioning assumptions, and using analogies
manipulate unconscious thought processes and, thereby, facilitate (i.e., finding correspondence of inner relationship or function
creative performance. between different concepts). The application of unconscious
The idea that unconscious processes work on a problem in the processes, however, has not been systematically introduced to
absence of conscious guidance has been described by many great educational, innovation and business contexts. By demonstrat-
artists and thinkers, and the above mentioned findings provide ing that unconscious processes can be important for creativity,
first scientific evidence for the idea that a period of incuba- the current findings may encourage practitioners to use uncon-
tion benefits from unconscious processes. This may be related scious processes in order to enhance creative thinking. Applying
to the fact that unconscious thought organizes information. unconscious processes could, for example, entail that people
Representations become better organized and more polarized, set a goal to find creative solutions for a problem before they
and memory becomes more gist-based. Moreover, unconscious are distracted from the problem by doing something different.
thought theory postulates that unconscious thought leads to a What people do in the meantime should be chosen carefully.
process of weighting whereby the importance of information Sio and Ormerods (2009) meta-analytic review revealed that the
is assessed. However, this idea awaits further study, as it was benefits of an incubation period are greater when participants are
supported in some experiments (Bos et al., 2011; Usher et al., occupied by an undemanding task than when they engage in a
2011), but not in others (Ashby et al., 2011; Pachur and Forrer, demanding task or no task at all. Moreover, Gilhooly et al. (2013)
2013). These findings may suggest that unconscious thought is found that spatial incubation benefited verbal-rated creativity,
a process whereby disorganized information becomes more and and verbal incubation benefited spatial-rated creativity but not
more organized until some kind of equilibrium is reached, and vice versa. Therefore, when stuck on a creative task, during an

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org April 2014 | Volume 8 | Article 215 | 7

7
Ritter and Dijksterhuis Creativityunconscious foundations of incubation

incubation period one should do something undemanding that REFERENCES


is very different from the main task, before returning to it. Amabile, T. M. (1983). The Social Psychology of Creativity. New York, NY: Springer-
Verlag.
Although unconscious processes can be a powerful source to
Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in Context: Update to the Social Psychology of
facilitate creativity, only engage in daydreaming or sleeping to Creativity. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
produce groundbreaking discoveries or great artistic creations will Amabile, T. M. (1997). Entrepreneurial creativity through motivational synergy. J.
not do the trick. A plethora of raw materials has to be available to Creat. Behav. 31, 1826. doi: 10.1002/j.2162-6057.1997.tb00778.x
be connected and one has to be able to focus on some options Andreasen, N. C. (1995). The Creating Brain: The Neuroscience of Genius. New York
and Washington DC: Dana Press.
out of an array of options. In this sense, conscious processing is Ashby, N. J. S., Glckner, A., and Dickert, S. (2011). Conscious and unconscious
needed to establish a knowledge base, to know what problems to thought in risky choice. Testing the capacity principle and the appropriate
tackle, and to verify and implement new ideas. Future research weighting principle of unconscious thought theory. Front. Psychol. 2:261.
may investigate what combination of conscious and unconscious doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00261
processes is most fruitful for creativity. One could think about Baer, J. (1998). The case for domain specificity of creativity. Creat. Res. J. 11, 173
177. doi: 10.1207/s15326934crj1102_7
the order of the two processes (e.g., a period of task-related Baird, B., Smallwood, J., Mrazek, M. D., Kam, J. W., Franklin, M. S., and Schooler,
conscious thought that is followed by a period during which one J. W. (2012). Inspired by distraction mind wandering facilitates creative incuba-
refrains from task-related conscious thought, or repeatedly switch tion. Psychol. Sci. 23, 11171122. doi: 10.1177/0956797612446024
between the two modes of thought), and the optimal duration Bargh, J. A. (2011). Unconscious thought theory and its discontents: a critique of
the critiques. Soc. Cogn. 29, 629647. doi: 10.1521/soco.2011.29.6.629
of each of the two processes. People are likely to benefit more
Bargh, J. A., Raymond, P., Pryor, J., and Strack, F. (1995). Attractiveness of
from an incubation period when they get stuck and, therefore, the underling: an automatic power sex association and its consequences for
one can assume that a relatively long period of conscious thought sexual harassment and aggression. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 68, 768781. doi: 10.
should be preferred above a short period of conscious thought. 1037//0022-3514.68.5.768
Also for unconscious processes the duration of the incubation Barrett, D. (1993). The committee of sleep: a study of dream incubation for
problem solving. Dreaming 3, 115122. doi: 10.1037/h0094375
period seems to be of importance. In a recent study from Yang
Barrett, D. (2001). The Committee of Sleep: How Artists, Scientists and Athletes use
et al. (2012), 3 min (as compared to 1 min and 5 min) seemed their Dreams for Creative Problem Solvingand How you can too. NY: Crown
to be the optimal duration of unconscious thought. However, it Books/Random House.
is likely that 3 min of incubation is not the most appropriate Bass, B. (1990). From transactional to transformational leadership: learning to
duration for all creativity tasks. It can be assumed that the optimal share the vision. Organ. Dyn. 18, 1931. doi: 10.1016/0090-2616(90)90061-s
Bennington, J. H., and Frank, M. G. (2003). Cellular and molecular connections
duration is contingent on the task (Weisberg, 1999). Besides between sleep and synaptic plasticity. Prog. Neurobiol. 69, 71101. doi: 10.
exploring the optimal duration of unconscious processes as a 1016/s0301-0082(03)00018-2
function of task characteristics and the optimal combination of Betsch, T., Plessner, H., Schwieren, C., and Gtig, R. (2001). I like it but i dont
conscious and unconscious processes, future research could also know why: a value-account approach to implicit attitude formation. Pers. Soc.
focus on the similarities and disparities between the different Psychol. Bull. 27, 242253. doi: 10.1177/0146167201272009
Bos, M. W., Dijksterhuis, A., and van Baaren, R. B. (2008). On the goal-dependency
unconscious processes (i.e., incubation, unconscious thought, of unconscious thought. J. Exp. Psychol. 44, 11141120. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.
mind-wandering and sleep) and could investigate which process is 01.001
most beneficial for creativity and for the distinct mental processes Bos, M. W., Dijksterhuis, A., and van Baaren, R. B. (2011). The benefits of sleeping
underlying creative thought (Baer, 1998). Finally, future research on things: unconscious thought leads to automatic weighting. J. Consum.
Psychol. 21, 48. doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2010.09.002
may study potential moderators, for example, whether experts
Bowden, E. M., and Jung-Beeman, M. (2003). Aha! Insight experience correlates
and people with ample prior knowledge exhibit a different pattern with solution activation in the right hemisphere. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 10, 730
of creative performance as a result of unconscious and conscious 737. doi: 10.3758/bf03196539
processes. Bowers, K. S., Regehr, G., Balthazard, C., and Parker, K. (1990). Intuition
in the context of discovery. Cogn. Psychol. 22, 72110. doi: 10.1016/0010-
The present article aimed to provide an overview of the
0285(90)90004-n
domain of incubation and creativity, and to shed more light on Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brains default
the causes of incubation effects. Research on incubation, mind network: anatomy, function and relevance to disease. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1124,
wandering, and sleep was presented and discussed, and it was 138. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.011
investigated whether people can think unconsciously and whether Cai, D. J., Mednick, S. A., Harrison, E. M., Kanady, J. C., and Mednick, S. C.
(2009). REM, not incubation, improves creativity by priming associative net-
unconscious processes can contribute to creativity. The current works. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 106, 1013010134. doi: 10.1073/pnas.09002
findings provide first empirical support for the idea that during 71106
an incubation period unconscious processes contribute to creative Christoff, K., Gordon, A. M., Smallwood, J., Smith, R., and Schooler, J. W. (2009).
thinking, and that it is not merely the absence of conscious Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system
thought that drives incubation effects. We hope that the current contributions to mind wandering. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 106, 87198724.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900234106
article inspires researchers to further tackle the unconscious foun- Christoff, K., Gordon, A. M., and Smith, R. (2011). The role of spontaneous
dations of creativity. This will not only increase our theoretical thought in human cognition, in Neuroscience of Decision Making, eds O.
knowledge on the role of unconscious processes in creativity, but Vartanian and D. R. Mandel (New York, NY: Psychology Press), 259284.
will also offer valuable insights for practical implication. Under- Claxton, G. (1997). Hare Brain, Tortoise Mind: How Intelligence Increases When you
Think Less. New York, NY: Harper Collins.
standing and facilitating creativity is important, as the ability to
Cole, M. W., and Schneider, W. (2007). The cognitive control network: integrated
think creatively plays an important role in many areas of our life, cortical regions with dissociable functions. Neuroimage 37, 343360. doi: 10.
such as education, arts, sciences, and the economic sector. 1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.071

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org April 2014 | Volume 8 | Article 215 | 8

8
Ritter and Dijksterhuis Creativityunconscious foundations of incubation

Creswell, J. D., Bursley, J. K., and Satpute, A. B. (2013). Neural reactivation Khn, S., Ritter, S. M., Mller, B. C. N., van Baaren, R. B., Brass, M., and
links unconscious thought to decision-making performance. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Dijksterhuis, A. (2013). The importance of unconscious processes in creativity
Neurosci. 8, 863869. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst004 a structural MRI study. J. Creat. Behav. doi: 10.1002/jocb.45
Cropley, A. J. (1990). Creativity and mental health in everyday life. Creat. Res. J. 3, Langley, P., and Jones, R. (1988). A computational model of scientific insight,
167187. doi: 10.1080/10400419009534351 in The Nature of Creativity: Contemporary Psychological Perspectives, ed R. J.
Dietrich, A., and Kanso, R. (2010). A review of EEG, ERP and neuroimaging studies Sternberg (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 177201.
of creativity and insight. Psychol. Bull. 136, 822848. doi: 10.1037/a0019749 Manni, R. (2005). Rapid eye movement sleep, non-rapid eye movement
Dijksterhuis, A. (2004). Think different: the merits of unconscious thought in sleep, dreams and hallucinations. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 7, 196200. doi: 10.
preference development and decision making. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87, 586598. 1007/s11920-005-0053-0
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.5.586 Mason, M. F., Norton, M. I., Van Horn, J. D., Wegner, D. M., Grafton, S. T., and
Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M. W., Nordgren, L. F., and Van Baaren, R. B. (2006). On Macrae, C. N. (2007). Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-
making the right choice: the deliberation-without-attention effect. Science 311, independent thought. Science 315, 393395. doi: 10.1126/science.1131295
10051007. doi: 10.1126/science.1121629 Mazzarello, P. (2000). What dreams may come? Nature 408, 523. doi: 10.
Dijksterhuis, A., and Meurs, T. (2006). Where creativity resides: the generative 1038/35046170
power of unconscious thought. Conscious Cogn. 15, 135146. doi: 10.1016/j. Mumford, M. D. (2002). Social innovation: ten cases from Benjamin Franklin.
concog.2005.04.007 Creat. Res. J. 14, 253266. doi: 10.1207/s15326934crj1402_11
Dijksterhuis, A., and Nordgren, L. F. (2006). A theory of unconscious thought. Nieuwenstein, M., and van Rijn, H. (2012). The unconscious thought advantage:
Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 1, 95109. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00007.x further replication failures from a search for confirmatory evidence. Judgm.
Dodds, R. D., Ward, T. B., and Smith, S. M. (2003). Incubation in problem solving Decis. Mak. 7, 779798.
and creativity, in The Creativity Research Handbook (Vol. 3), ed M. A. Runco Olton, R. M., and Johnson, D. M. (1976). Mechanisms of incubation in
(Cresskill, NJ: Hampton). creative problem solving. Am. J. Psychol. 89, 617630. doi: 10.2307/14
Ellwood, S., Pallier, G., Snyder, A., and Gallate, J. (2009). The incubation effect: 21461
hatching a solution? Creat. Res. J. 21, 614. doi: 10.1080/10400410802633368 Orlet, S. (2008). An expanding view on incubation. Creat. Res. J. 20, 297308.
Ericsson, K. A. (2006). The influence of experience and deliberate practice on the doi: 10.1080/10400410802278743
development of superior expert performance. Camb. Handb. Expertise Expert Pachur, T., and Forrer, E. A. (2013). Selection of decision strategies after conscious
Perform. 683703. doi: 10.1017/cbo9780511816796.038 and unconscious thought. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 5, 477488. doi: 10.1002/bdm.
Feist, G. J., and Gorman, M. E. (1998). The psychology of science: review and 1780
integration of a nascent discipline. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2, 347. doi: 10.1037/1089- Patrick, A. S. (1986). The role of ability in creative incubation. Pers. Indiv. Diff. 7,
2680.2.1.3 169174. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(86)90052-8
Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., Van Essen, D. C., and Reverberi, C., Toraldo, A., DAgostini, S., and Skrap, M. (2005). Better without
Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, (lateral) frontal cortex? Insight problems solved by frontal patients. Brain 128,
anticorrelated functional networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 102, 96739678. 28822890. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh577
doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504136102 Ric, F., and Muller, D. (2012). Unconscious addition: when we unconsciously
Gallate, J., Wong, C., Ellwood, S., Roring, R. W., and Snyder, A. (2012). Creative initiate and follow arithmetic rules. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 141, 222226. doi: 10.
people use nonconscious processes to their advantage. Creat. Res. J. 24, 146151. 1037/a0024608
doi: 10.1080/10400419.2012.677282 Ritter, S. M., Strick, M., Bos, M. W., Van Baaren, R. B., and Dijksterhuis, A. (2012a).
Ghiselin, B. (1952). The Creative Process. New York, NY: New American Library. Good morning creativity: task reactivation during sleep enhances beneficial
Giambra, L. M. (1979). Sex differences in daydreaming and related mental activity effect of sleep on creative performance. J. Sleep Res. 21, 643647. doi: 10.1111/j.
from the late teens to the early nineties. Int. J. Aging Hum. Dev. 10, 134. doi: 10. 1365-2869.2012.01006.x
2190/01bd-rfne-w34g-9eca Ritter, S. M., van Baaren, R. B., and Dijksterhuis, A. (2012b). Creativity: the role of
Giambra, L. M., and Grodsky, A. (1989). Task-unrelated images and thoughts while unconscious processes in idea generation and idea selection. Think. Skills Creat.
reading. Imagery 2731. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4899-0876-6_3 7, 2127. doi: 10.1016/j.tsc.2011.12.002
Gilhooly, K. J., Georgiou, G., and Devery, U. (2013). Incubation and creativity: do Rugg, H. (1963). Imagination. New York, NY: Harper and Row.
something different. Think. Reason. 19, 137149. doi: 10.1080/13546783.2012. Runco, M. A. (2004). Creativity. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 55, 657687. doi: 10.
749812 1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141502
Gupta, N., Yang, Y., Mednick, S. C., and Huber, D. E. (2012). The road not taken: Runco, M. A., and Pritzker, S. (1999). The Encyclopedia of Creativity. San Diego:
creative solutions require avoidance of high-frequency responses. Psychol. Sci. Academic Press.
23, 288294. doi: 10.1177/0956797611429710 Sawyer, R. K. (2012). Explaining Creativity: The Science of Human Innovation. 2nd
Hadamard, J. (1945). Essay on The Psychology of Invention in the Mathematical Field. Edn. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Schooler, J. W., and Melcher, J. (1995). The ineffability of insight, in The Creative
Helmholtz, H. von. (1896). Vortraege Und Reden. Brunswick: Friedrich Viewig und Cognition Approach, eds S. M. Smith, T. B. Ward, and R. A. Fink (Cambridge,
Sohn. MA: MIT Press), 97134.
Jung, R. E., Segall, J. M., Bockholt, H. J., Chavez, R. S., Flores, R., and Haier, R. J. Scott, G., Leritz, L. E., and Mumford, M. D. (2004). The effectiveness of cre-
(2010). Neuroanatomy of creativity. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 398409. doi: 10. ativity training: a quantitative review. Creat. Res. J. 16, 361388. doi: 10.
1002/hbm.20874 1080/10400410409534549
Kaplan, C. A. (1989). Hatching a Theory of Incubation: Does Putting a Problem Aside Segal, E. (2004). Incubation in insight problem solving. Creat. Res. J. 16, 141148.
Really Help? If so, Why? (Pittsburgh, PA: Doctoral dissertation Carnegie-Mellon doi: 10.1207/s15326934crj1601_13
University). Simonton, D. K. (1999). Origins of Genius: Darwinian Perspectives on Creativity.
Kaufman, J. C. (2002). Dissecting the golden goose: components of studying New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
creative writers. Creat. Res. J. 14, 2740. doi: 10.1207/s15326934crj1401_3 Sio, U. N., and Ormerod, T. C. (2009). Does incubation enhance problem
Koestler, A. (1964). The Act of Creation. New York, NY: Penguin Books. solving? A meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 135, 94120. doi: 10.1037/a00
Kounios, J., and Jung-Beeman, M. (2009). Aha! The cognitive neuroscience 14212
of insight. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 18, 210216. doi: 10.1111/j.1467- Sio, U. N., Monaghan, P., and Ormerod, T. (2012). Sleep on it, but only if it is
8721.2009.01638.x difficult: effects of sleep on problem solving. Mem. Cognit. 41, 159166. doi: 10.
Kris, E. (1952). Psychoanalytic Explorations in Art. New York, NY: International 3758/s13421-012-0256-7
Universities Press. Sklar, A. Y., Levy, N., Goldstein, A., Mandel, R., Maril, A., and Hassin, R. R. (2012).
Kubie, L. S. (1985). Neurotic Distortion of the Creative Process. Lawrence, Kansas: Reading and doing arithmetic unconsciously. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 109,
University of Kansas Press. 1961419619. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1211645109

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org April 2014 | Volume 8 | Article 215 | 9

9
Ritter and Dijksterhuis Creativityunconscious foundations of incubation

Smallwood, J., Nind, L., and OConnor, R. C. (2009). When is your head at? An Wagner, U., Gais, S., Haider, H., Verleger, R., and Born, J. (2004). Sleep inspires
exploration of the factors associated with the temporal focus of the wandering insight. Nature 427, 352355. doi: 10.1038/nature02223
mind. Conscious. Cogn. 18, 118125. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.11.004 Walker, M. P., Liston, C., Hobson, J. A., and Stickgold, R. (2002). Cognitive
Smallwood, J., Obonsawin, M., and Heim, D. (2003). Task unrelated thought: the flexibility across the sleepwake cycle: REM-sleep enhancement of anagram
role of distributed processing. Conscious. Cogn. 12, 169189. doi: 10.1016/s1053- problem solving. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 14, 317324. doi: 10.1016/s0926-
8100(02)00003-x 6410(02)00134-9
Smallwood, J., and Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychol. Bull. 132, Wallas, G. (1926). The Art Of Thought. New York, NY: Harcourt Brace.
946958. doi: 10.4324/9781315037509 Wegner, D. M., and Smart, L. (1997). Deep cognitive activation: a new approach to
Smith, S. M., and Blankenship, S. E. (1989). Incubation effects. Bull. Psychon. Soc. the unconscious. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 65, 984995. doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.
27, 311314. doi: 10.3758/BF03334612 65.6.984
Smith, S. M., and Dodds, R. A. (1999). Incubation. Encyclopedia Creat. 2, 3943. Weisberg, R. W. (1999). Creativity and knowledge: a challenge to theories, in
doi: 10.1787/9789264173781-en Handbook of Creativity, ed R. J. Sternberg (Cambridge, England: Cambridge
Smith, S. M. (2003). The constraining effects of initial ideas, in Group Creativity: University Press), 226250.
Innovation through Collaboration, eds P. Paulus and B. Nijstad (New York: Woodworth, R. S., and Schlosberg, H. (1954). Experimental Psychology (Rev. ed.).
Oxford University Press). 3146. Oxford, UK: Holt.
Spreng, R. N., Stevens, W. D., Chamberlain, J., Gilmore, A. W., and Schacter, D. Yang, H., Chattopadhyay, A., Zhang, K., and Dahl, D. W. (2012). Unconscious
L. (2010). Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control creativity: when can unconscious thought outperform conscious thought? J.
network, supports goal-directed cognition. Neuroimage 53, 303317. doi: 10. Consum. Psychol. 22, 573581. doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2012.04.002
1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.016 Yang, J., Weng, X., Zang, Y., Xu, M., and Xu, X. (2010). Sustained activity within
Stickgold, R., Hobson, J. A., Fosse, R., and Fosse, M. (2001). Sleep, learning the default mode network during an implicit memory task. Cortex 46, 354366.
and dreams: off-linememory reprocessing. Science 294, 10521057. doi: 10. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.05.002
1126/science.1063530 Yaniv, I., and Meyer, D. E. (1987). Activation and metacognition of inaccessible
Stickgold, R., Scott, L., Rittenhouse, C., and Hobson, J. A. (1999). Sleep-induced stored information: potential bases for incubation effects in problem solving.
changes in associative memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 11, 182193. doi: 10. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 13, 187205. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.13.
1162/089892999563319 2.187
Stickgold, R., and Walker, M. P. (2004). To sleep, perchance to gain creative insight? Zhong, C. B., Dijksterhuis, A. J., and Galinsky, A. D. (2008). The merits of
Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 191192. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.03.003 unconscious thought in creativity. Psychol. Sci. 19, 912918. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-
Storm, B. C., and Angelo, G. (2010). Overcoming fixation: creative problem 9280.2008.02176.x
solving and retrieval-induced forgetting. Psychol. Sci. 21, 12631265. doi: 10.
1177/0956797610379864 Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors declare that the research was conducted
Strick, M., Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M. W., Sjoerdsma, A., van Baaren, R. B., and in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed
Nordgren, L. F. (2011). A meta-analysis on unconscious thought effects. Soc. as a potential conflict of interest.
Cogn. 29, 738762. doi: 10.1521/soco.2011.29.6.738
Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z., and Williams, J. M. G. (1995). How does cognitive therapy Received: 31 October 2013; accepted: 26 March 2014; published online: 11 April
prevent depressive relapse and why should attentional control (mindfulness) 2014.
training help? Behav. Res. Ther. 33, 2539. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(94)e0011-7 Citation: Ritter SM and Dijksterhuis A (2014) Creativitythe unconscious founda-
Usher, M., Russo, Z., Weyers, M., Brauner, R., and Zakay, D. (2011). The impact tions of the incubation period. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:215. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.
of mode of thought in complex decisions: intuitive decisions are better. Front. 00215
Psychol. 2:37. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00037 This article was submitted to the journal Frontiers in Human Neuroscience.
Van Gaal, S., de Lange, F. P., and Cohen, M. X. (2012). The role of consciousness Copyright 2014 Ritter and Dijksterhuis. This is an open-access article distributed
in cognitive control and decision making. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:121. doi: 10. under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, dis-
3389/fnhum.2012.00121 tribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s)
Vincent, J. L., Patel, G. H., Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Baker, J. T., Van Essen, or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in
D. C., et al. (2007). Intrinsic functional architecture in the anaesthetized monkey accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is
brain. Nature 447, 8386. doi: 10.1038/nature05758 permitted which does not comply with these terms.

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org April 2014 | Volume 8 | Article 215 | 10

10
Psychological Bulletin 2009 American Psychological Association
2009, Vol. 135, No. 1, 94 120 0033-2909/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0014212

Does Incubation Enhance Problem Solving? A Meta-Analytic Review

Ut Na Sio and Thomas C. Ormerod


Lancaster University

A meta-analytic review of empirical studies that have investigated incubation effects on problem solving
is reported. Although some researchers have reported increased solution rates after an incubation period
(i.e., a period of time in which a problem is set aside prior to further attempts to solve), others have failed
to find effects. The analysis examined the contributions of moderators such as problem type, presence of
solution-relevant or misleading cues, and lengths of preparation and incubation periods to incubation
effect sizes. The authors identified a positive incubation effect, with divergent thinking tasks benefiting
more than linguistic and visual insight tasks from incubation. Longer preparation periods gave a greater
incubation effect, whereas filling an incubation period with high cognitive demand tasks gave a smaller
incubation effect. Surprisingly, low cognitive demand tasks yielded a stronger incubation effect than did
rest during an incubation period when solving linguistic insight problems. The existence of multiple types
of incubation effect provides evidence for differential invocation of knowledge-based vs. strategic
solution processes across different classes of problem, and it suggests that the conditions under which
incubation can be used as a practical technique for enhancing problem solving must be designed with
care.

Keywords: incubation, problem solving, insight, meta-analysis

Anecdotal reports of the intellectual discovery processes of proposed incubation as the second of four phases in problem
individuals hailed as geniuses (see, e.g., Ghiselin, 1985; Wallas, solving (the others being preparation, illumination, and verifica-
1926; Woodworth & Schlosberg, 1954) share a common theme: A tion). Insight may be characterized as a sudden, unpredictable, and
flash of insight pops unexpectedly into the mind of the individual nonverbalizable solution discovery (e.g., Metcalfe & Weibe,
after he or she has put an unsolved problem aside for a period of 1987). Some researchers see the apparently unconscious nature of
time, having failed in initial attempts to solve it. This temporary solution discovery as evidence that the processes required to
shift away from an unsolved problem that allows a solution to achieve insight in problem solving are qualitatively different from
emerge seemingly as if from no additional effort is termed an those used to tackle problems that do not require insight (e.g.,
incubation period (Wallas, 1926). Its importance in current think- Jung-Beeman & Bowden, 2000; Wertheimer, 1985). Incubation
ing and practice is illustrated by a recent search in Google Scholar might serve a valuable role in arbitrating between theories of
for the term incubation along with either creativity, insight, or insight, in particular between special-process theories based on
problem that yielded 5,510 articles, with the search restricted to the unconscious mechanisms of spreading activation (e.g., Knoblich,
years 1997 to 2007 and the subject areas to social sciences, arts, Ohlsson, Haider, & Rhenius, 1999) and theories of insight as
and humanities. An additional 1,970 articles were yielded when normal problem-solving processes based on conscious mecha-
the subject areas business, administration, and economics were nisms of search (e.g., MacGregor, Ormerod, & Chronicle, 2001).
included. Yet there are many conflicting accounts of incubation, Understanding the role of incubation periods may also allow us
with some studies reporting strong effects (e.g., Smith & Blanken- to make use of them effectively to promote creativity in areas such
ship, 1989) and others failing to find any effect at all (Olton & as individual problem solving, classroom learning, and work en-
Johnson, 1976). This article aims to resolve the uncertainties vironments. Educational researchers have tried to introduce incu-
surrounding the phenomenon by providing a statistical meta- bation periods in classroom activity, and positive incubation ef-
analytic review of empirical studies of incubation. We suggest that fects in fostering students creativity have been reported (Lynch &
it is only armed with the results of an integrative and quantitatively Swink, 1967; Medd & Houtz, 2002; Rae, 1997; Webster, Camp-
based review that progress in understanding the mechanisms that bell, & Jane, 2006). However, in the absence of a comprehensive
might underlie the phenomenon can be made. theory or model that can explain how and why positive incubation
One theoretical reason for studying incubation is because it is effects might emerge and under what conditions they are best
closely associated with insightful thinking. Indeed, Wallas (1926) fostered, no general pedagogic recommendations can be made.
Several hypotheses have been proposed to account for the
alleged positive effects of incubation periods on problem solving,
and they can be divided into two main kinds: conscious work and
Ut Na Sio and Thomas C. Ormerod, Department of Psychology, Lan-
caster University, Lancaster, United Kingdom. unconscious work. The conscious-work hypothesis holds that in-
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Thomas cubation effects are due to issues such as reduction of mental
C. Ormerod, Department of Psychology, Lancaster University LA1 4YF, fatigue (Posner, 1973) or additional covert problem solving during
United Kingdom. E-mail: t.ormerod@lancaster.ac.uk the incubation period (Browne & Cruse, 1988; Posner, 1973). Both

94

11
INCUBATION AND PROBLEM SOLVING 95

sources implicate changes in consciously controlled problem- the opposite pattern: Participants who took a rest during an incu-
solving activities during an incubation period. In contrast, the bation period performed better than those who had to perform
unconscious-work hypothesis suggests that positive incubation tasks during an incubation period. There are also studies that report
effects are the result of gradual and unconscious problem-solving the same level of performance by participants with filled and
processes that occur during an incubation period (Bowers, Regehr, unfilled incubation periods (Olton & Johnson, 1976; Smith &
Balthazard, & Parker, 1990; Seifert, Meyer, Davidson, Patalano, & Blankenship, 1989). However, these studies vary in terms of the
Yaniv, 1995; Simon, 1966; Smith, 1995; Smith & Blankenship, length of incubation period, the target problems tackled, and the
1991; Yaniv & Meyer, 1987). nature of the interpolated tasks during the incubation period.
Three different unconscious processes have been proposed to Because of inconsistent findings concerning incubation, some
account for incubation effects. The first involves eliciting new researchers have doubted the existence of the effect, in particular
knowledge: Over time, activation will spread toward previously rejecting the unconscious-work hypotheses (Browne & Cruse,
ignored but relevant memory items. Even if relevant items do not 1988; Olton & Johnson, 1976; Perkins, 1995). However, one
receive above-threshold activation, this process can still sensitize explanation for conflicting findings is that there are procedural
individuals to related concepts, and thus they will be more likely moderators other than task type that influence the occurrence of
to make use of external cues to solve a problem. In addition, problem-solving processes during an incubation period, such as the
partially activated concepts may combine with others to yield length of the incubation period or the nature of the problem. The
fortuitous insightful ideas (Bowers et al., 1990; Smith, 1995; Smith field lacks a comprehensive review that summarizes and evaluates
& Blankenship, 1991; Yaniv & Meyer, 1987). The second hypoth- these studies. There have been two reviews to date of relations
esis is selective forgetting: An incubation period will weaken the between different procedural variables and the incubation effect,
activation of inappropriate solution concepts that distract individ- and both are qualitative in nature. Oltons (1979) review of past
uals during initial attempts, allowing a fresh view of the problem incubation studies led him to question the existence of an incuba-
(Smith, 1995; Smith & Blankenship, 1991). The third hypothesis is tion effect, given that no experimental paradigm appeared to
problem restructuring: An individuals mental representation of a demonstrate an incubation effect reliably. Yet, a limited number of
problem will be reorganized into a more appropriate and stable studies were available at that time: Only 10 incubation studies
form after initial unsuccessful attempts. The individual is then were included in his review. A recent review by Dodds, Ward, and
more able to capitalize upon relevant external information or to Smith (in press) with more studies included suggested that several
rearrange problem information in a manner that allows a solution variables may interact to influence the effectiveness of an incuba-
to be found more readily (Seifert et al., 1995). Problem restruc- tion period. However, the qualitative nature of their review led
turing might emerge either from switching the strategy used to them to conclude that findings of past studies are too divergent and
search for moves to attempt (e.g., MacGregor et al., 2001) or from that more studies are needed to assess the impact of each variable
relaxing self-imposed inappropriate constraints on the problem and to identify the optimum settings for an incubation effect.
representation (Knoblich et al., 1999). Studies of metacognition The wide variation in experimental parameters among studies
indicate that strategy switching can be unconscious (Newton & makes it difficult to draw cross-experiment conclusions from a
Roberts, 2005; Reder & Schunn, 1996; Siegler & Stern, 1998) and qualitative review. To overcome these problems, a systematic
that different strategies compete for activation during the strategy meta-analytic review is needed. Meta-analytic review allows a
selection process (Siegler & Stern, 1998). quantitative evaluation of research domains that describes the
The conscious- and unconscious-work accounts generate differ- typical strength of the effect or phenomenon and also the relation
ent predictions concerning the effects of activities that individuals of each moderator to the size of the effect by using statistical
engage in during an incubation period. According to the conscious- analysis methods (Rosenthal, 1995). The objectives of the current
work account, individuals benefit most from an unfilled incubation study were to carry out the first statistical meta-analysis of incu-
period, as this gives them an opportunity either to relax, reduce bation studies to assess the effect size of the experimental incu-
fatigue, or continue working on the problem. In contrast, bation effect, and more important, the impact of potential moder-
unconscious-work accounts suggest that unconscious problem- ators on the incubation effect size. However, in order to undertake
solving processes occur when individuals shift their attention away the meta-analysis, we first need to identify the likely key moder-
from the problem to other mental activities. Thus, a certain level of ators, which we achieve in the next section by reviewing the
involvement in other tasks during an incubation period should methods used in previous studies. A particular focus of this review
facilitate postincubation problem solving. is to identify moderators that might discriminate conscious-work
A number of experimental studies have examined the role of and unconscious-work hypotheses and also the mechanisms (re-
task type during an incubation period. The experimental paradigms duction of fatigue, additional work, activation of new information,
of these incubation studies are fairly uniform: The work of one forgetting, restructuring) that might underlie each hypothesis.
group of participants is interrupted with an incubation period
(having a break or performing other tasks) while solving a prob-
Potential Moderators
lem, whereas the other group works on the problem continuously.
Performance differences between these two groups are then com- The Interpolated Task Used During the Incubation Period
pared. The findings of the published studies do not give uncondi-
tional support to either the unconscious-work or the conscious- Various types of interpolated task have been used in past stud-
work account. Patrick (1986) found that participants who had a ies, and they can be divided into tasks of high or low cognitive
filled incubation period outperformed those who had an unfilled demand. Examples of high cognitive demand tasks include mental
incubation period. However, Browne and Cruse (1988) reported rotation, counting backwards, and visual memory tests, whereas

12
96 SIO AND ORMEROD

reading is commonly adopted as a low cognitive demand task. obtained in an empirical study carried out by Silveira (1972),
High demand tasks should fully occupy the individuals mind and showing that problem solvers performed better with longer prep-
prevent further conscious work on the unsolved problem. Some aration and incubation periods.
studies have reported that undertaking a high cognitive demand
task during an incubation period is beneficial to the problem- Nature of the Problem
solving process (Kaplan, 1990; Patrick, 1986; Segal, 2004). None-
theless, studies using low cognitive demand tasks that do not Various different types of problem have been used in incubation
require individuals to focus their conscious attention on task per- studies. Some problems, which we term creative problems here,
formance have reported similar benefits (Beck, 1979; Silveira, require individuals to produce multiple new ideas to meet a spe-
1972; Smith & Blankenship, 1989). cific brief. For example, a verbal divergent-production task is the
consequences task (e.g., What would be the result if everyone
Length of the Incubation Period suddenly lost the ability to read and write; Fulgosi & Guilford,
1968). Typically, there is no right or wrong answer to this kind of
Longer incubation periods may allow additional problem- problem, and performance is assessed in terms of the number of
solving activity or a greater degree of forgetting of misleading solution ideas that are generated.
items or spreading of activation memory. Thus, problem solvers Other problems require individuals to discover a specific target
may show a larger performance improvement when they return to solution that is known in advance by the experimenter. Problems
the problem after a long incubation period than after a short one. of this kind that have been studied in the literature on incubation
Some studies have reported evidence supporting this contention are generally of a type described as insight problems, in that they
(Beck, 1979; Fulgosi & Guilford, 1968; Silveira, 1972; Smith & require the solver to reject initial solution ideas by achieving
Blankenship, 1989). However, it is difficult to draw cross-experiment insight into an alternative strategy or knowledge domain. The
conclusions, because there is no standard operationalization of what insight problems used in the incubation studies can be divided into
constitutes long and short incubation periods. In Smith and visual problems, which typically require the solver to consider a
Blankenships (1989) study, for example, a 15-min incubation visuospatial array (e.g., the nine-dot problem; Scheerer, 1963), and
period was defined as a long incubation period, and they reported linguistic problems, which typically require the solver to consider
that participants receiving this length of incubation period per- linguistic information related to the problem. The remote associ-
formed better than those receiving a 5-min incubation period. ates task (RAT; S. A. Mednick, 1962) is one of the most com-
However, in Becks (1979) study, a 20-min incubation period was monly used linguistic problems in incubation studies. In each
considered short, and participants performance in this group did RAT, three stimulus words are presented to individuals, who then
not differ from that of the control group. Kaplan (1990) suggested have to think of a fourth word that can form an association with
that to judge whether the incubation period is short or long, the each of the three words. For example, if the three stimulus words
length of time that problem solvers spend on initial attempts to of a RAT are electric, wheel, and high, the fourth word can be
solve (named the preparation period by Wallas, 1926) should also chair. Bowden and Jung-Beeman (2003b) have developed a pool
be taken into account. Kaplan found that a larger incubation effect of remote associates problems and collected normative data re-
was observed after increasing the ratio of the length of the prep- garding the resolution rates and response times for the problems.
aration period to the incubation period. Thus, in addition to in- The classification of insight problems into visual- and linguistic-
cluding incubation and preparation periods as separate moderators based categories is supported by research findings from Gilhooly
in the meta-analysis reported below, we also undertook a second- and Murphy (2005) showing that solving visual and linguistic
ary analysis using the ratio of preparation to incubation time as an insight problems requires different types of cognitive skills.
alternative moderator. In the remainder of this article, we refer to problem types as
creative, visual, and linguistic. Descriptions of the types of prob-
Length of the Preparation Period lem used in incubation studies are illustrated in Appendix A.
Problem type is likely to be an important determinant of incuba-
During the preparation period, problem solvers gather informa- tion, because it seems likely that each type creates different task
tion to formulate a problem representation and make initial at- demands. For instance, the nine-dot problem appears to require the
tempts to solve, which may lead to an impasse. Although a solver to restructure an initially faulty or incomplete problem
problem may not be solved during the preparation period, this does representation in searching for a representation that allows solu-
not mean that the effort the problem solver spends on the problem tion, whereas the consequences task appears to require the activa-
is fruitless. Schank (1982, 1999) and VanLehn (1988) both sug- tion of as wide a range of different concepts as possible. One
gested that failure in problem solving is important in the human question the meta-analysis allows us to address is whether an
learning process. Studies by Patalano and Seifert (1994) and incubation period favors one type of problem more than another.
Seifert et al. (1995) have found evidence of a Zeigarnik effect in
insight problem solving (Zeigarnik, 1927, 1938), wherein individ- The Presence of Solution-Relevant Cues
uals remembered the problems on which they got stuck better
than those they solved immediately. Seifert et al. hypothesized that Some unconscious processes proposed to explain incubation
having a better memory for failed problems might help individuals effects are purely internal and thus independent of the external
return efficiently to the problem once relevant new information is environment, such as the inhibition of irrelevant memory (Smith,
encountered during an incubation period, thereby maximizing the 1995; Smith & Blankenship, 1991) and the recombination of
chance of solving. Evidence concerning this prediction has been partially activated concepts (Bowers et al., 1990). Others stress

13
INCUBATION AND PROBLEM SOLVING 97

interactions with the external environment, such as the proposal for all other moderators. This approach allows us to summarize the
that spreading activation can partially activate previously ignored past studies systematically even though they vary widely in num-
relevant memory and therefore sensitize the problem solver to ber and type of experimental parameters. In addition, interactions
chance encounters with related stimuli (Seifert et al., 1995). A few between different moderators, such as the nature of the interpola-
studies have examined the effects of the presence of cues during an tion task during the incubation period and the nature of the prob-
incubation period (Browne & Cruse, 1988; Dodds, Smith, & Ward, lem, were examined.
2002; Dorfman, 1990; Dreistadt, 1969; Olton & Johnson, 1976).
Most failed to find any positive effect of cues on the incubation
Literature Search
effect. However, the failure reported in these studies may be due to
other factors, such as the difficulty of the unsolved problems. In We collected publications that contained studies relevant to a
order to have a fair evaluation of the impact of this moderator, we meta-analysis of incubation through a search of the ERIC, Psy-
first have to isolate the effect of other moderators on the incubation cINFO, PsycARTICLES, and MEDLINE databases using the key-
effect. word incubat!, intersected with one of fixation, creativ!, diver-
gent!, insight!, or problem. Then, references given in all the
Misleading Cues obtained articles were systematically searched for additional rele-
vant publications. There is a concern that studies with statistically
Another factor that may influence the occurrence of incubation significant results are more likely to get published than those
effects is the presence of misleading cues. Smith and Blankenship without significant results, and this may lead to a biased retrieval
(1989) carried out a series of experiments to examine the effect of of studies. To ameliorate this to some extent, we carried out similar
an incubation period on solving RATs, in which participants had to literature searches in the ProQuest Digital Dissertations database
find a word that might accompany each of three presented words. and used Google Scholar for retrieving doctoral dissertations,
Smith and Blankenship presented cues (shown here in italics) unpublished articles, and conference articles concerning the incu-
comprising misleading associates and the target word next to each bation effect. In total, 37 relevant publications were identified and
of the three stimulus words. An example of a misleading RAT is: obtained. Studies meeting the following criteria were assimilated
SHIP ocean, OUTER space, CRAWL floor. The target solution is in the analysis:
space. Performance improvements after an incubation period were
observed only when participants solved tasks containing mislead- 1. The settings and difficulty of the problems were the same
ing cues. They concluded that a problem solver who is fixated on among all the experimental conditions.
misleading information benefits more from an incubation period.
The misleading cues data provide critical support for forgetting- 2. The total length of time that participants could spend on
based explanations of incubation. The presence of misleading cues solving the problem consciously was the same among all
is therefore one of the potential moderators examined in this the conditions.
meta-analysis.
3. The study included a control (no-incubation) group, and
participants in that group worked on the problem contin-
The Meta-Analysis uously.
The variables mentioned above were the potential moderators of 4. Participants problem-solving performance in pre- and
incubation chosen for this meta-analysis. Note that other potential postincubation periods was measured.
moderators might have been included (e.g., number of trials,
participant characteristics), but we focused on those we believe are 5. The study reported information that allowed the compu-
fundamental to discriminating differing theoretical accounts of tation of an effect size.
incubation.
The statistical meta-analysis that follows addressed two ques- The first and second selection criteria ensured that tasks were
tions: First, is there reliable evidence for incubation; and second, presented in an identical way among different conditions and that
what are the most influential moderators? To address the first any between-conditions performance differences could be attrib-
question, we computed the effect size of the incubation effect uted to differences in settings of the incubation period. The inclu-
reported in each available study. Given that the variability among sion of Criterion 3, a control condition (no break between the first
effect sizes is likely to be greater than that resulting from subject- and the second attempts at the problem), is essential to provide a
level sampling, a random-effects model was adopted in this meta- baseline against which performance in incubation conditions can
analysis. A heterogeneity test was carried out to verify this as- be compared. Only publications that assessed the problem-solving
sumption, and then the weighted mean under the assumption of performance in both first and second attempts were included in the
random-effects model was computed and assessed to determine if analysis (Criterion 4). Therefore, some studies (e.g., Sio &
it was significantly larger than zero. Rudowicz, 2007; Yaniv & Meyer, 1987) that did not assess postin-
To address the second question, we carried out weighted least- cubation problem-solving performance were excluded. The infor-
squares linear regressions using the aforementioned moderators as mation required for computing effect sizes is discussed in the
predictor variables and the incubation effect size in each study section Estimation of Effect Sizes. Eight publications were ex-
weighted by the inverse of its variance as the criterion variable. cluded because the experimental studies contained within them
The results of the regression show the independent contributions of failed to meet one or more of the mentioned criteria. The specific
each potential moderator to the incubation effect, while controlling reasons for excluding the publications are described in Table 1,

14
Table 1 98
Summary of Incubation Studies

Misleading cues Preparation Incubation Cues during Incubation effect Included in


Year Author Problem in problems period period Incubation task incubation period Other factors reported meta-analysis

1964 Mednick et al. RAT No 1 min Not specified Analogy vs. analogy Yes Yes, in analogy No, no control group
(Experiment 1) ! cues ! cues condition
1964 Mednick et al. RAT No 1 min Not specified Analogy vs. analogy Yes Cue relevance and Yes, in high-ability No, no effect size
(Experiment 2) ! cues correctness; problem- group in analogy information
solving ability ! cues condition
1967 Gall & RAT No 2 min 0, 25 min Nonverbal vs. free Yes No Yes, estimated from
Mendelsohn associates ! cues p " value
1968 Fulgosi & Consequences task No 2 min 0, 10, 20 min Number series (10 and 20 No Yes, in 20-min Yes, estimated from
Guilford min) condition p " value and
statement of
significance
1969 Dreistadt Farm & tree planting No 5 min 0, 8 min Guess card vs. guess card Yes In control condition, half Yes, with cues, farm Yes
problems ! cues the participants problem only
received cues
1969 Murray & Denny Saugstads ball problem No 5 min 0, 5 min Syllogisms, and tracing No Problem-solving ability Yes, in low-ability Yes
complex sequences of group
digits/numbers
1972 Fulgosi & Consequences Task No 2 min 0, 30, 60 min Number series (30 and 60 No Yes, in 30-min No, no effect size
Guilford min) condition information
1972 Dominowski & Hat rack problem No 5 min 0, 10 min Free association vs. Yes One group received cues No Yes, estimated from
Jenrick anagram throughout statement of
(Experiment 1) significance
1972 Dominowski & Hat rack problem No 3 min 0, 3 min Free association vs. No One group received No Yes, estimated from
Jenrick anagram cues; problem-solving statement of

15
(Experiment 2) ability significance
1972 Silveira Necklace problem No 3 min vs. 13 0, 30, Read ! free activity No Yes Yes
(Experiment 1) min 210 min
1972 Silveira Necklace problem No 13 min 0, 210 min Read ! free activity No Yes Yes
SIO AND ORMEROD

(Experiment 2)
1972 Silveira Consequences task No 2 min 210 min Read ! free activity No No No, no control group
(Experiment 3)
1974 Peterson Anagram No 0.33 min 1.8 min Other anagram No Task difficulty Yes Yes
1975 Bennett RAT No 1 min 10 min Hear music vs. mathematics No No No, no control group
problems
1976 Olton & Johnson Farm problem No 10 min 0, 15 min Rest vs. Stroop ! count Yes No Yes
backward vs. review
problem vs. lecture vs.
hear music
1979 Beck Verbal divergent- No 12 min 0, 20, 30 min Relax vs. write essay No Yes, with longer Yes
thinking task incubation period
1985 Brockett Brick task and RAT No 10 min (brick 0, 20 min Questionnaires No Yes Yes
task), 0.33
min (RAT)
1986 Patrick RAT No 2 min 0, 5 min Conversation vs. mental No Yes, in mental Yes, estimated from
rotation rotation condition p " value and
statement of
significance
1987 Yaniv & Meyer Rare-word definition No Not specified Not specified Questionnaire No Yes No, no postincubation
task performance
measure
1988 Browne & Cruse Farm problem No 20 min or 25 0, 5 min Hear music, draw graph, Yes No Yes
(Experiment 2) min memorize text
(table continues)
Table 1 (continued )

Misleading cues Preparation Incubation Cues during Incubation effect Included in


Year Author Problem in problems period period Incubation task incubation period Other factors reported meta-analysis

1989 Smith & Rebus Misleading or 0.5 min 0, 5, 15 min Rest vs. music perception No Useful cues presented Yes, 15-min ! 5- Yes
Blankenship useful cues for second attempt min incubation
(Experiment 1) and control group
1989 Smith & Rebus Misleading or 0.5 min 0, 5, 15 min Rest vs. music perception No Yes, 15- and 5-min Yes
Blankenship useful cues incubation !
(Experiment 2) control group
1989 Smith & Rebus Misleading or 0.5 min 0, 10, 15 min Rebus with music No Yes, no difference Yes, estimated from
Blankenship useful cues perception, mathematics, among incubation p " value
(Experiment 3) or rest conditions
1989 Smith & Rebus Yes, misleading 0.5 min 0, 5 min Read vs. mathematics No Yes, in read story Yes, estimated from
Blankenship or useful condition p " value and
(Experiment 4) cues statement of
significance
1990 Dorfman Word puzzle task No 0.49 min 0, 5, 15 min Word problems vs. word No Yes, in both Yes
(Experiment 3) problems incubation
# cues conditions
1990 Dorfman Word puzzle task No 0.49 min 0, 3, 8, 13 Number series # cues No Yes, estimated from
(Experiment 4) min statement of
significance
1990 Kaplan Consequences task No 2 min 0, 30 min Psychometric test battery No Yes Yes
(Experiment 1)
1990 Kaplan Consequences task No 2 min 0, 32 min Mathematics # lecture No Yes Yes
(Experiment 2)
1990 Kaplan Consequences task No 2 min 0, 30 min Lecture No No Yes, estimated from
(Experiment 3) statement of
significance

16
1990 Kaplan Consequences task No 4.57 min 28.08 min Mathematics and insight No Yes Yes
(Experiment 4) problems
1991 Smith & RAT Yes 0.5 min 0, 5 min Read science fiction No Problem-solving ability Yes, with misleading Yes
Blankenship cues, greater for
(Experiment 1) low-ability
participants
1991 Smith & RAT Yes 1 min 0, 5 min Read science fiction No Problem-solving ability Yes, with misleading Yes
Blankenship cues
(Experiment 2)
INCUBATION AND PROBLEM SOLVING

1991 Smith & RAT Yes 0.5 min 0, 0.5, 2 min Free association No Problem-solving ability Yes, with misleading Yes, estimated from
Blankenship cues, less for low- p " value and
(Experiment 5) ability participants statement of
significance
1992 Goldman et al. Anagram No 0.25 min 0, 20, 1,440 General knowledge No Yes, in 1,440-min Yes
min # free activity condition
1992 Houtz & Frankel Life-relevant problem No 10 min 10 min Anagram task No Yes Yes
1997 Torrance-Perks Word completion Yes 0.5 min 0, 10 min Rest # lexical decision vs. Yes No Yes
(Experiment 1) problems rest # lexical decision #
cues
1997 Torrance-Perks Candle & radiation Yes, misleading 1 min (RAT 0, 8 min Memory (candle); read Yes No Yes
(Experiment 2) problems, RAT cues and candle (radiation); analogy
problem), (RAT)
5 min
(radiation
problem)
1998 Hansberry Riddles Yes 1 min 0, 15 min RAT No No Yes
(Experiment 2)
(table continues)
99
Table 1 (continued )
100

Misleading cues Preparation Incubation Cues during Incubation effect Included in


Year Author Problem in problems period period Incubation task incubation period Other factors reported meta-analysis

1998 Hansberry RAT Yes 0.5 min 0, 10 min RAT No Problem-solving ability No Yes
(Experiment 3)
1999 Henley Anagram No 0.25 min 0, 1,440 min Free activity No No Yes, estimated from
(Experiment statement of
3.2) significance
1999 Henley Anagram No 0.93 min 0, 1,440 min Free activity No No Yes, estimated from
(Experiment 4) statement of
significance
1999 Jamieson RAT Yes 0.33 min 0, 5 min Mathematics No No Yes
(Experiment 1)
1999 Jamieson RAT Yes 0.33 min 0, 5 min Mathematics No No Yes
(Experiment 2)
2002 Dodds et al. RAT No 0.5 min 15 min Insight problem with: Yes Cues (answer, relevant Yes, when answer or No, no control group
(Experiment 1) Drawing vs. make a information, or related unrelated word
word test vs. make a word) presented during
word test ! cues incubation
2002 Dodds et al. RAT No 0.5 min 0, 15 min Insight problem with: Yes Cues (answer, relevant Yes, when answer Yes
(Experiment 2) Drawing vs. make a information, or related presented during
word test vs. make a word) incubation
word test ! cues
2002 Medd & Houtz Creative writing Yes 10 min 0, 10 min Unrelated writing task vs. No Prompt to think about Yes, if working on Yes, estimated from
related writing task problem during related task during p " value and
incubation incubation statement of
significance
2002 Moss RAT No 0.5 min 15 min Verbal reasoning task Yes Tasks described as Yes, interaction No, no control group
! cues solvable or between ability

17
unsolvable; problem- and problem type
solving ability
2003 Seabrook & Anagram No 0.25 min 7 min Word generation tasks Yes Cues (irrelevant or Yes, in relevant-cue Yes, estimated from
Dienes relevant) group p " value.
SIO AND ORMEROD

2004 Both et al. Anagram No 1.67 min 0, 6 min Letter search; questionnaire No Yes Yes
2004 Both et al. Anagram Yes 1.67 min 0, 6 min Letter and digit search; Yes Yes Yes
questionnaire
2004 Penney et al. Anagram No 5.75 min 15, 180 min Word completion No Yes, in 15-min No, no control group
(Experiment 1) ! cues vs. inactivity condition
2004 Penney et al. Anagram No 5.75 min 15, 1,440 Word completion No Yes, in 15-min No, no control group
(Experiment 2) min ! cues vs. inactivity condition
2004 Penney et al. Anagram No 5.75 min 0, 30, 120 Word completion No Yes Yes
(Experiment 3) min ! cues vs. inactivity
2004 Segal Insightful mathematic No 20 min 0, 4, 12 min Read vs. word puzzle No Yes, with word Yes
puzzle puzzle
2004 Snyder et al. Divergent-thinking task No 5 min 5 min Conversation No Unknown No
2005 Christensen & Insight puzzle No Not specified Not specified Analogy or distractor rating Yes Cues (analogous cue or Yes, in analogous No, no control group
Schunn task distractor cue) cue condition
2007 Sio & Rudowicz RAT Yes 1 min 0, 2 min Mental rotation task No Chess expertise; cues to Yes, experts more No, no postincubation
! mathematics task vs. chess expertise were sensitive to performance
listening to music related, neutral, or relevant concepts measure
misleading after incubation
2007 Vul & Pashler Anagram No 1 min 0, 5 min Video game No Task difficulty No Yes
(Experiment 1)
2007 Vul & Pashler RAT Yes 1 min 0, 5 min Video game No Task difficulty Yes, in the fixation Yes
(Experiment 2) group

Note. RAT # remote associates task.


INCUBATION AND PROBLEM SOLVING 101

which also describes the settings of studies included in the meta- Table 2
analysis. Of the remaining 29 publications, 20 were refereed Coding System
journal articles, 8 were doctoral dissertations, and 1 was a confer-
ence article. The ratio of the refereed to other studies is 2.2:1, Variable Coding description
which is within the suggested range of 128:1 to 1:1 for including Author Author(s) of the study
unpublished studies in an effort to avoid publication bias (Thorn- Year Year the study was published
ton & Lee, 2000). Most publications included multiple experi- Total Total number of participants
ments, thereby allowing a reasonable sample size of independent Problem type 0 ! creative problem (e.g., consequences task)
1 ! visual problem (e.g., farm problem, radiation
studies (N ! 117) to be achieved.
problem)
2 ! linguistic problem (e.g., remote associates
task, anagram, rebus)
Coding Procedure Misleading cues 0 ! no misleading cues
1 ! misleading cues embedded in the problem
Many of the experiments reported in the selected publications Preparation period Amount of time spent on each problem before the
had two or more experimental conditions, such as incubation incubation period (in minutes)
periods of different lengths or different types of task in the incu- Incubation period Length of the incubation period (in minutes)
bation period. For the sake of the meta-analysis, experiments with Incubation task 0 ! rest
1 ! low cognitive demand task (e.g., drawing a
more than one incubation condition were broken down into inde- picture, reading)
pendent studies with one incubation condition and one control 2 ! high cognitive demand task (e.g., mental
condition. The same control group may be included in more than rotation task, memory test)
one independent study and compared with more than one incuba- Cues Presence of relevant cues during the incubation
period
tion condition. For example, in Goldman, Wolters, and Wino-
0 ! no cue
grads (1992) experiment, there were control, short incubation 1 ! yes
period, and long incubation period conditions. The experiment was
decomposed into two studies, one consisting of the control and
short incubation period conditions and the other consisting of the
control and long incubation period conditions. To avoid inflating or p values. If a p-less-than value was given instead of an exact p
the degrees of freedom available, we split the number of partici- value, the p-less-than value was treated as an exact value, and an
pants in the control condition across studies entered into the
estimate of Cohens d was generated. For studies that did not
analysis, a method advocated by Bar-Haim, Lamy, Pergamin,
include any of the abovementioned information but only provided
Bakermans-Kranenburg, and van IJzendoorn (2007).
statements of nonsignificant differences between the control and
There were also studies that had more than one control condi-
incubation groups, the Cohens d was assumed to be zero. Among
tion. In such cases, the control condition that had the most similar
setting to the incubation condition was chosen. For example, in the studies that included multiple incubation conditions, some
Hansberrys (1998) third experiment, participants had to solve a provided a statement of nonsignificant performance differences
list of RATs under one of three conditions: two control and one among the incubation conditions and reported only the overall
incubation. In one control condition, the RATs were presented performance difference between the control and the incubation
individually for 60 s. In the other control condition, as well as in conditions. In such cases, all incubation conditions were assumed
the incubation condition, each RAT was presented in two separate to have generated the same magnitude of incubation effect sizes.
30-s blocks. Data from the latter control condition were therefore Of the 117 effect sizes, 88 were extracted directly from the means
used in computing the effect size, because this control condition and standard deviations, t value, F value, frequencies, or p value;
and the incubation condition had the closest settings in terms of 8 were computed from a p-less-than value; and 21 were estimated
RAT presentation. from statements of significance.
After separating the experiments into independent studies, a In some incubation studies, problem-solving performance was
standard system was used to code each study. Background infor- assessed along more than one dimension. For example, in the study
mation on each independent study (author, publication year, and carried out by Vul and Pashler (2007), participants performance
the number of participants in each condition), as well as potential on RATs was measured in terms of the time spent on solving
moderator variables, were extracted. Table 2 presents the coding RATs and the number of correct solutions. In such cases, we
system used in this meta-analysis. Appendix B presents the infor- computed a single effect size by averaging the effect size from
mation we extracted from each independent study by using the each measure (cf. Durlak & Lipsey, 1991).
coding system. Following Hedge and Olkins (1985) suggestion for removing
bias caused by small-sample studies, we computed an unbiased
Estimation of Effect Sizes effect size estimate by multiplying the effect size of each single
The effect size, Cohens d, was computed for each study entered study by a factor of 13/[4(total N " 2) " 1], where total N is the
into the meta-analysis. Cohens d in this meta-analysis comprised total number of participants of that study. Any unbiased effect size
the difference in mean problem-solving performance scores be- larger than 2 SDs from the group mean was considered an outlier
tween the control and incubation conditions divided by their and was recoded to the value of the effect size found at 2 SDs,
pooled standard deviation (Hedges & Olkin, 1985). In some cases, following a procedure for reducing the bias caused by extreme
effect sizes had to be calculated from t and F values, frequencies, effect sizes reported by Lipsey and Wilson (2001).

18
102 SIO AND ORMEROD

Heterogeneity Analysis follow-up analysis to model the effect sizes. The unbiased effect
size estimate weighted by the inverse of the variance was the
In this analysis, we predicted that the variance in magnitude of outcome variable of the regression analysis. The predictor vari-
the unbiased effect sizes among studies was not due simply to ables included problem type, misleading cues, (solution-relevant)
sampling error but instead to the difference in settings of each cues, incubation task, preparation period, and incubation period.
study (e.g., length of incubation period, nature of incubation task, The categorical variables were represented with an appropriate
presence of cues). Therefore, analyses of the effect sizes should be number of dummy-coded vectors. The categories (with the asso-
carried out under the assumption of a random-effects model. To ciated predictor variable following in parentheses) of creative
confirm the assumption of a heterogeneous distribution of effect problem (problem type), rest (incubation task), no misleading cues
sizes, we carried out a heterogeneity test before running any (misleading cues), and no cue (cues) were used as reference groups
analyses on the effect sizes. The standard measure of heterogeneity in the analysis, and their coefficients were restricted to zero.
is Cochrans Q test. The Q statistic is the weighted1 sum of the The second section of the analyses investigated the general
squared difference between the unbiased effect size estimate of impact of the moderators on the incubation effect sizes. In this
each independent study and the weighted average unbiased effect section, all the incubation studies were grouped together, and a
size estimate across studies. Q is distributed as a chi-square sta- weighted least-squares regression analysis was carried out to in-
tistic with k ! 1 degrees of freedom, where k is the number of vestigate the general impact of each moderator. Again, the
independent studies. If Cochrans Q test for heterogeneity is sta- weighted unbiased effect size was the outcome variable of the
tistically significant (Q is larger than the chi-square value with k ! regression analysis. The predictor variables were problem type,
1 degrees of freedom), the assumption of the random-effects model misleading cues, cues, incubation task, preparation period, and
is supported. incubation period. Another weighted least-squares regression was
carried out to examine the interaction between the categorical
Publication Bias variables problem type and incubation task. The predictor vari-
ables of the regression model were the appropriate number of
Prior to investigating the impact of potential moderators, we dummy-coded vectors and the multiplicative terms of these two
undertook a preliminary analysis to assess if a publication bias categorical variables, as well as the variables misleading cues,
existed in the selection of studies despite the inclusion of unpub- preparation period, incubation period, and cues. A more detailed
lished studies. A funnel plot of sample size against unbiased effect description of the selection of the dummy-coded vectors and the
size estimates was created. In the absence of any publication bias, multiplicative terms of problem type and incubation task is pre-
it is expected that the plot would be a funnel shape, such that the sented in Appendix C.
amount of scatter about the mean effect size deceases with increas-
ing sample size. In addition to checking the presence of publication
bias qualitatively, we carried out a weighted least-squares linear Results
regression using the unbiased effect size estimates as the depen-
dent variable and the sample sizes weighted by the inverse of the There were 117 studies included in this meta-analysis. The total
variance in a random-effects model, which is the sum of the number of participants was 3,606, and the median number of
between-studies variance2 (random variance component) and participants per study was 25. An unbiased effect size estimate was
within-study variance of the unbiased effect size. The regression computed for each independent study. Among these studies, 85
slope (unstandardized coefficient of the predictive variable) would reported positive effect sizes. The unbiased effect size estimates
be expected to approach zero if there is no publication bias ranged from !0.71 to 4.07, and the median effect size was 0.26.
(Macaskill, Walter, & Irwing, 2001). The outcome of this analysis The unweighted mean of the unbiased effect size estimate was
is reported below. 0.41, with a standard deviation of 0.71. The upper and lower
bounds of the 95% confidence interval were 0.54 and 0.28. Unbi-
ased effect sizes larger than 2 SDs from the mean were recoded to
Regression Model Testing
Due to the wide variation in experiment settings among incu- 1
The weighting was the inverse of the within-study variance of the
bation studies, observed incubation effect size differences may effect estimate, and the formula for the within-study variance was [(2 "
reflect the combined impact of different moderators. Hence, square root of total N) # (N of experimental " N of control " square root
weighted least-squares regression analyses were carried out to of the unbiased effect size)]/(2 " total N " N of experimental " N of
reveal the true impact of each moderator on incubation effects. The control), where N is the number of participants in that condition (Cooper &
regression analyses were organized into two main sections. In the Hedges, 1994). Three studies were excluded when computing the weighted
first section, the incubation studies were first classified into dif- average unbiased effect size estimate and the Cochrans Q value because
they had a within-subjects design, and thus all participants were involved
ferent groups, in terms of the type of problem used, the cognitive
in both control and incubation conditions. Thus, the weighting formula
load of the incubation tasks, the presence of misleading cues, and
could not apply to them.
the presence of relevant cues during an incubation period. Within 2
The between-studies variance was equal to [Q ! (k ! 1)]/c, where Q
each subgroup, the random variance component of the studies was is the Cochrans Q value and k is the number of studies. The formula for
computed. A larger than zero random variance component implies c was [(the sum of the inverse of the within-study variance) (the sum of
that the variability of effect sizes within these studies is not simply the square of the inverse of the within-study variance)]/(the sum of the
due to subject-level sampling error (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). A inverse of the within-study variance) and was suggested by Cooper and
weighted least-squares regression analysis was carried out as a Hedges (1994).

19
INCUBATION AND PROBLEM SOLVING 103

the value of the effect size found at 2 SDs. Table 3 gives the 300
stem-and-leaf display showing the distribution of the unbiased
effect sizes. The unweighted mean of the adjusted unbiased effect 250
size estimate was 0.36, with a standard deviation of 0.51. The
200

Sample Size
upper and the lower bounds of the 95% confidence interval were
0.26 and 0.45. The confidence interval does not include zero, 150
implying that the estimate of the mean unbiased effect size is
significantly larger than zero. 100
The heterogeneity statistic, Cochrans Q, was 173.99, and was
significantly larger than the chi-square critical value, df ! 113, 50
p " .001. This supports the use of the random-effects model. The
0
variance of each unbiased effect size in the random-effects model
was the sum of the between-studies variance and within-study -1 -0.5 0 0.5 1 1.5 2
variance of the unbiased effect size. The between-studies variance, Unbiased Effect Size
also called the random variance component, among these incuba-
tion studies was .0834. The mean of the weighted unbiased effect Figure 1. Funnel plot of the studies included in this meta-analysis. The
dotted line indicates the mean unbiased effect size.
size was 0.29, with a standard deviation3 of 0.04, and the 95%
confidence interval was 0.21, 0.39. The nonzero confidence inter-
val implies that the weighted mean is significantly larger than zero.
suggesting the absence of publication bias. Thus, no correction has
This answers our first question, showing the existence of a positive
been made for publication bias.
incubation effect.
Table 4 presents the weighted mean, standard deviation, 95%
Figure 1 presents the funnel plot of sample size against the
confidence interval, and random variance component in each sub-
estimated unbiased effect size of each study in the meta-analysis.
group of each categorical moderator. Six of the subgroups (lin-
We carried out a weighted least-squares regression using unbiased
guistic problems, creative problems, absence of misleading cues,
effect sizes weighted by the inverse of the variance as the depen-
absence of relevant cues, high cognitive load task, unoccupied
dent variable and sample size as the predictive variable. The
incubation period) had larger than zero random variance compo-
regression coefficient of the predictive variable was not signifi-
nents. New weightings, under the assumption of a random-effects
cantly different from zero (standardized # ! $.08, p ! .41), model, were computed for each of the subgroups. Weighted least-
squares regression analyses were carried out to find the moderators
that accounted for the effect size variability among these sub-
Table 3 groups. Small numbers of studies using creative tasks and studies
Stem-and-Leaf Display of 117 Unbiased Effect Sizes having unoccupied incubation periods preclude the possibility of
regression analyses with these moderators. An effect of applying a
Stem Leaf weighting to this regression analysis is to underestimate the orig-
inal standard error of each unstandardized coefficient. Thus, we
$0.7 1
$0.5 9, 8, 8
computed an adjusted standard error by dividing the original
$0.4 1 standard error by the square root of the mean square residual, a
$0.3 8, 5, 3 procedure suggested by Lipsey and Wilson (2001). The corrected
$0.2 standard error was used in the significance test (z test) of each
$0.1 8, 7, 4, 4, 0 unstandardized coefficient.
$0.0 3
0.0 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, Tables 5,6,7, and 8 present a summary of the regression analysis
0, 0, 0, 3, 4, 5, 5, 6, 6, 7, 9, 9 results of each subgroup. With the subgroup of studies using
0.1 0, 0, 0, 0, 0, 1, 1, 3, 4, 4, 4, 5, 7, 7 linguistic problems, low cognitive load tasks generated larger
0.2 1, 4, 5, 8, 8 incubation effects than did rest alone (# ! .54, p " .05). Also,
0.3 1, 4, 6, 6, 7
0.4 0, 0, 1, 2, 2, 4, 5, 7, 7, 8, 8, 9
there was an interaction between problem type and incubation task
0.5 0, 2, 2, 6, 6, 6, 6, 6, 6, 7 with this subgroup, such that that low cognitive load tasks facili-
0.6 2, 2, 4, 5, 6, 6, 6 tated the incubation effect only when solving linguistic problems.
0.7 0, 1, 2, 4, 4 With the absence of misleading cue and absence of relevant cue
0.8 6 subgroups, regression analyses revealed that, in the absence of
0.9 9, 9
1.0 5, 5, 7, 7, 8, 9 these cues, individuals solving visual problems had smaller incu-
1.1 7 bation effects than did those solving creative and linguistic prob-
1.2 lems. There was also a positive impact of longer preparation periods
1.3 on the incubation effect sizes. The cognitive load of the incubation
1.4
1.5
1.6 8 3
1.7 0 The standard deviation of the weighted mean, also known as the
1.8 2, 2, 2, 2 standard error, was calculated as the square root of 1/%wi, and the 95%
confidence interval was calculated as the weighted mean &1.96 times the
Note. Outliers were recoded. standard deviation of the weighted mean (Hedges & Olkin, 1985).

20
104 SIO AND ORMEROD

Table 4
The Random Variance Component, Weighted Mean, Standard Deviation, Standard Error, and 95% Confidence Interval of the Effect
Size Estimate by Each Categorical Moderator

Problem type Misleading cues Incubation task Relevant cues

Moderator Linguistic Verbal Creativea Yes Noa High load Low load Resta Yes Noa

Number of studies 65 35 14 29 85 76 22 16 32 82
Random variance
component .00281 0 .37418 0 .12004 .06478 0 .30000 0 .10409
Mean 0.22 0.26 0.29 0.35 0.32 0.24 0.52 0.46 0.24 0.34
Standard deviation 0.05 0.08 0.14 0.09 0.06 0.05 0.10 0.18 0.09 0.06
95% confidence interval 0.32, 0.13 0.41, 0.10 0.55, 0.02 0.53, 0.17 0.43, 0.20 0.35, 0.13 0.72, 0.32 0.82, 0.11 0.41, 0.07 0.45, 0.22
Mean comparison with
the reference group t(77) ! "0.43 t(47) ! "0.19 t(112) ! 0.30 t(90) ! 1.17 t(36) ! 0.28 t(112) ! 0.90

Note. The lower confidence intervals of all the weighted unbiased effects are larger than zero, suggesting that the mean is significantly larger than zero, p % .05.
a
The reference groups in mean comparisons.

tasks did not have any impact on the magnitude of the incubation The length of the preparation period was found to have a
effects in these two subgroups. Note, however, that the presence or significant impact on the magnitude of the incubation effect ($ !
absence of these effects with these specific subgroups does not .03, p % .05). Three bivariate correlations were carried out to
necessarily imply converse effects in other subgroups, hence a check for positive relationships between the length of the prepa-
shift to analysis of general impacts of each moderator. ration period and the magnitude of the weighted incubation effect
In the second stage of the analysis, a weighted least-squares when solving the three types of problem. There was a statistically
regression analysis was carried out to look at the general impact of significant positive correlation between the weighted incuba-
each moderator on the incubation effect sizes. A summary of the tion effect size and the length of the preparation period with
regression analysis results is presented in Table 9. The negative visual problems, r(35) ! .40, p ! .02, and creative problems,
coefficients associated with visual problems and linguistic prob- r(14) ! .60, p ! .03, but not with linguistic problems, r(65) !
lems indicate that individuals solving these two types of insight ".04, p ! .75.
problem showed a smaller incubation effect than did individuals We carried out another weighted least-squares regression anal-
solving creative problems. A z test was carried out to compare the ysis to examine the interaction between problem type and incuba-
coefficients of the visual problem and linguistic problem groups. tion task, using misleading cues, preparation period, incubation
The result was not statistically significant (z ! "1.25, p # .05), period, cues, and an appropriate number of dummy vectors and the
suggesting the magnitude of the incubation effect for visual and multiplicative terms of the variables problem type and incubation
linguistic insight problems was comparable. task as predictor variables. Table 10 presents the results of this

Table 5
Regression Model for Linguistic Problem Studies (N ! 65)

Variable Sum of squares df Mean square F p value

ANOVA significance test


Model 13.42 5 2.68 3.15! .01
Residual 5.24 59 0.85
Total 63.66 64

Summary of the regression model

Unstandardized $ Standardized $ Corrected SE$ z score


Incubation task
High cognitive load task .06 .06 .21 0.27
Low cognitive load task .54! .43 .25 2.15
Misleading cues .15 .17 .12 1.24
Ratio of the preparation period to the
incubation period .15 .12 .19 0.80
Relevant cue ".04 ".04 .13 "0.30

Note. Random variance component !.00281, R2 ! .21. Another regression with the same predicting variables,
except replacing the variable ratio of length of the preparation to the incubation period by the variables
incubation period and preparation period, was carried out. The pattern of the results was comparable, but it had
lower explanatory power, and a lower significance level, R2 ! .20, F(6, 58) ! 2.47, p ! .034. Neither the
variable preparation period nor incubation period was significant. ANOVA ! analysis of variance.
!
p % .05.

21
INCUBATION AND PROBLEM SOLVING 105

Table 6
Regression Model for Misleading Cue Studies (N " 85)

Variable Sum of squares df Mean square F p value

ANOVA significance test


Model 2.25 7 2.89 3.56! .002
Residual 62.62 77 0.81
Total 82.87 84

Summary of the regression model


Unstandardized $ Standardized $ Corrected SE$ z score
Problem type
Visual problem !.59! !.51 .19 !3.02
Linguistic problem !.31 !.29 .18 !1.74
Incubation task
High cognitive load task !.18 !.14 .22 !0.80
Low cognitive load task .06 .04 .26 0.23
Length of the incubation period #.001 .055 #.001 0.45
Length of the preparation period .03! .36 .01 2.16
Relevant cue !.03 !.02 .16 !0.20

Note. Random variance component ".12004, R2 " .24. ANOVA " analysis of variance.
!
p # .05.

regression analysis. To examine the interaction effects, we exam- for incubation periods filled with low or high cognitive load tasks
ined the coefficient differences between their multiplicative terms or rest. However, this regression model has a problem in exploring
to see if they were significantly larger than zero by using z tests. the interaction between problem type and incubation task. Among
The details of equations for computing the coefficient difference studies examining the role of an incubation period with creative
can be found in Appendix C. Table 11 presents the coefficient problems, 12 out of 14 employed high cognitive load tasks, and the
differences between multiplicative terms. remaining 2 studies employed rest during the incubation period.
With creative problems, undertaking high cognitive load tasks This unbalanced distribution may cause bias when examining the
was associated with smaller incubation effects than with low interaction between incubation task and problem type. Hence,
cognitive load tasks or rest during the incubation period (CjHj ! another regression analysis was carried out that excluded studies
CjRj " !.91, p # .05; CjHj ! CjLj " !.79, p " .08, where C using creative problems. This third regression model included the
refers to creative problems, H to high cognitive demand tasks, R to variables misleading cues, preparation period, cues, the dummy
rest, and L to low cognitive demand tasks, all in study j). When variables of problem type (excluding creative problem) and incu-
solving linguistic and visual problems, no differences were found bation task, and their multiplicative variables. Appendix D pre-

Table 7
Regression Model for No Relevant Cue Studies (N " 82)

Variable Sum of squares df Mean square F p value

ANOVA significance test


Model 29.34 6 4.89 7.35! #.001
Residual 49.94 75 0.67
Total 79.28 81

Summary of the regression model


Unstandardized $ Standardized $ Corrected SE$ z score
Problem type
Visual problem !.79!! !.66 .19 !4.10
Linguistic problem !.22 !.21 .18 1.24
Incubation task
High cognitive load task !.25 !.22 .21 !1.20
Low cognitive load task .16 .12 .25 0.64
Length of the incubation period #.001 .02 0 0.13
Length of the preparation period .05!! .51 .02 3.01
Misleading cue .09 .07 .18 0.53

Note. Random variance component ".10409, R2 " .37. ANOVA " analysis of variance.
!
p # .05. !! p # .001.

22
106 SIO AND ORMEROD

Table 8
Regression Model for High Cognitive Load Incubation Task Studies (N " 75)

Variable Sum of squares df Mean square F p value

ANOVA significance test


Model 7.36 6 1.47 1.53 .19
Residual 66.19 69 0.96
Total 73.55 74

Summary of the regression model


Unstandardized # Standardized # Corrected SE# z score
Problem type
Visual problem !.41! !.34 .20 !2.01
Linguistic problem !.28 !.30 .17 !1.58
Length of the incubation period !.01 !.12 .01 !0.87
Length of the preparation period .02 .21 .02 1.31
Misleading cue .08 .06 .16 0.51
Relevant cue .09 .08 .14 0.62

Note. Random variance component ".06478, R2 " .10. ANOVA " analysis of variance.
!
p $ .05.

sents a detailed description of the selection of the dummy-coded among the three incubation conditions were comparable. This
vectors and the multiplicative terms. The results of this analysis are pattern of findings is consistent with the previous regression
presented in Table 12, and the coefficient differences between the analysis results. In addition, the exclusion of creative problem
multiplicative terms are presented in Table 13. studies makes the positive impact of a low cognitive load
The regression results indicated an interaction between prob- incubation period on linguistic problems more significant.
lem type and incubation task. When solving linguistic prob- The model using the variables preparation period and incubation
lems, a low cognitive load task generated significantly larger period was found to be not significant, and the variable preparation
incubation effects than did rest (LijLj ! LijRj " .45, p " .05, period was not significant in the analysis. This may be due to the
where Li refers to linguistic problems, L to low cognitive decrease in the number of studies included in the current regres-
demand tasks, and R to rest, all in study j). The difference sion model. Moreover, as mentioned above, a positive association
between low and high cognitive loads was in the same direction between the length of preparation period and incubation effect
but did not reach significance, and there was no significant size was found when solving visual and creative problems. Thus,
difference between the rest condition and the high cognitive the exclusion of creative problem studies appears to decrease the
load condition. When solving visual problems, the effect sizes significance of the variable preparation period and the significance

Table 9
Regression Model for All Studies (N " 114)

Variable Sum of squares df Mean square F p value

ANOVA significance test


Model 25.56 8 3.20 4.08 $.001
Residual 82.22 105 0.78
Total 107.79 113

Summary of the regression model


Unstandardized # Standardized # Corrected SE# z score
Problem type
Visual problem !.60!! !.52 .15 !3.61
Linguistic problem !.31! !.31 .14 !1.96
Misleading cues .16 .13 .12 1.16
Incubation task
Low cognitive load .14 .10 .18 0.72
High cognitive load !.16 !.15 .14 !1.02
Preparation period .03! .35 .01 2.29
Incubation period $.001 .04 $.001 $0.001
Cue presented !.01 !.01 .11 !0.09

Note. Random variance component " .0834, R2 " .24. ANOVA " analysis of variance.
!
p $ .05. !! p $ .001.

23
INCUBATION AND PROBLEM SOLVING 107

Table 10
Regression Model for All StudiesInteractions (N ! 114)

Variable Sum of squares df Mean square F p value

ANOVA significance test


Model 35.60 12 2.97 4.15 ".001
Residual 72.18 101 0.72
Total 107.78 113

Summary of the regression model


Unstandardized # Standardized # Corrected SE# z score
Problem Type $ Incubation Task
Visual Problem $ Low
Cognitive Load Task 0.17 0.08 0.35 0.50
Visual Problem $ High
Cognitive Load Task 0.90 !
0.61 0.35 2.60
Linguistic Problem $ Low
Cognitive Load Task 0.72 0.43 0.38 1.93
Linguistic Problem $ High
Cognitive Load Task 1.00 !
0.97 0.38 2.59
Problem type
Visual problem %1.29!! %1.10 0.32 %4.00
Linguistic problem %1.24!! %1.21 0.40 %3.14
Incubation task
High cognitive load task %0.91!! %0.81 0.30 %3.00
Low cognitive load task %0.12 %0.09 0.33 %0.38
Misleading cues 0.24 0.19 0.15 1.54
Length of the incubation period "0.001 0.018 "0.001 "0.001
Length of the preparation period 0.03! 0.01 0.01 1.97
Answer was presented 0.03 0.03 0.13 0.26

Note. Random variance component ! .08340, R2 ! .33. ANOVA ! analysis of variance.


!
p " .05. !! p " .001.

level of the regression model that used preparation period as one of inappropriately. Under this account, incubation supports knowl-
the predictor variables. edge activation, but it does not support restructuring.
Another finding of the meta-analysis was the beneficial effect of
Discussion an incubation period filled with low demand tasks on solving
linguistic problems. A positive effect of a filled incubation period
The meta-analysis supports the existence of incubation effects
on problem solving compared with rest during the incubation
and also identifies some potential moderators, including the prob-
period undermines the conscious-work hypothesis that incubation
lem type, length of the preparation period, and the incubation task.
effects are due to the mental fatigue reduction (Posner, 1973).
Individuals solving creative problems were more likely to benefit
There remains a possibility, of course, that a sufficiently light load
from an incubation period than individuals solving linguistic and
might allow additional covert problem solving compared with a
visual problems. Longer preparation periods gave rise to larger
heavier task load (Browne & Cruse, 1988; Posner, 1973), but this
incubation effects. When solving linguistic problems, a low cog-
does not explain why a light load should be more facilitative than
nitive load task gave the strongest incubation effects.
We suggest that the positive incubation effects found with rest alone.
creative problems are a direct reflection of their multiple-solution The positive effect of a light cognitive load may indicate com-
nature. When solving a creative problem, individuals benefit from petition between controlled and automatic processes in solving
performing a wide search of their knowledge to identify as many linguistic problems. It has been suggested with remote associates
relevant connections as possible with the presented stimuli. Each task (RAT) performance that only strong (and in this context,
time individuals reapproach the problem, they improve their per- incorrect) associates are accessed when individuals focus their
formance by extending the search to previously unexplored areas attention on seeking solutions, whereas remote associates are more
of their knowledge network. Incubation appears to facilitate the likely to be accessed when an individuals cognitive resources are
widening of search of a knowledge network in this fashion. allocated in a diffuse manner (Ansburg & Hill, 2003; Finke, Ward,
Linguistic and visual problems typically have only one possible & Smith, 1992; Martindale, 1995). During an incubation period,
solution. In order to solve them, individuals have to explore their low demand tasks may occupy part of the problem solvers atten-
memory or environment to look for specific relevant knowledge or tion, preventing the focused concentration that yields strong asso-
to adapt a specific strategy. Widening search to new items of ciates. Resting during an incubation period may allow individuals
knowledge may not be facilitative if the solution to a problem lies to continue consciously working on the problem, whereas perform-
within already activated knowledge that is currently represented ing high demand tasks may shift attention entirely to that interpo-

24
108 SIO AND ORMEROD

Table 11 and Brooks (1993), in which the act of verbalizing can impair
Summary of Coefficient Differences of the Original Regression performance by focusing individuals attention inappropriately on
Model verbalizable components of a task. The suggested role of light-load
incubation tasks receives indirect support from recent findings that
Multiplicative terms Coefficient difference Corrected SE z score show that visual search can be more efficient when performed
LijHj LijRj 0.08 .49 0.17 concurrently with an unrelated task than when performed alone
LijLj LijRj 0.60 .50 1.21 (Smilek, Enns, Eastwood, & Merikle, 2006). They suggest that the
LijLj LijHj 0.52 .70 0.74 dual-task condition prevents a narrow attentional focus in search-
VjHj VjRj !0.01 .46 !0.03 ing for stimuli.
VjLj VjRj 0.05 .48 0.10
VjLj VjHj 0.06 .66 0.09 With visual problems, the magnitude of the incubation effect
CjHj CjRj !0.91! .30 !3.00 was independent of the setting of an incubation period (filled or
CjLj CjRj !0.12 .33 !0.38 unfilled). Differences between visual and linguistic insight prob-
CjLj CjjHj 0.79 .45 1.77 lems may arise through a greater reliance on strategic search rather
VjHj CjHj !0.39 .47 !0.82
LijHj CjHj !0.25 .55 !0.45
than knowledge activation in the former than the latter. MacGregor
LijHj VjHj 0.14 .72 0.19 et al. (2001) proposed that in solving the nine-dot problem, indi-
VjLj CjLj !1.11! .47 !2.36 viduals select and execute moves that maximally reduce the dis-
LijLj CjLj !0.52 .55 !0.95 tance between current and goal states, essentially drawing lines
LijLj VjLj 0.60 .72 0.82
that connect as many dots as possible. While there remain moves
VjRj CjRj !1.29!! .32 !4.00
LijRj CjRj !1.24!! .40 !3.14 available that satisfy a criterion of satisfactory progress (in this
LijRj VjjRj 0.04 .51 0.08 case, the ratio of dots cancelled to lines available), individuals will
persevere with an initial representation of the problem that, in the
Note. Li " linguistic problem; H " high cognitive demand task; R " case of the nine-dot problem, does not include consideration of
rest; L " low cognitive demand task; V " visual problem; C " creative
problem; j " study j. space outside the dot array. According to MacGregor et al.s
!
p # .05. !! p # .001. p " .08. account, individuals must experience a failure to find moves that
meet a criterion of satisfactory progress before they change their
initial representation of the problem, thereby including space out-
lated task, leading to a narrow rather than diffused attentional side the dot array in their attempts. An incubation period would be
focus. The impact of performing high demand tasks is analogous helpful only if the problem solvers became aware of the necessity
to the verbal overshadowing effect reported by Schooler, Ohlsson, of a strategy shift, but according to MacGregor et al. they are

Table 12
Regression Model for Studies Excluding Creative Problem (N " 100)

Variable Sum of squares df Mean square F p value

ANOVA significance test


Model 15.04 8 1.88 2.04! .05
Residual 83.92 91 0.92
Total 98.96 99

Summary of the regression model


Unstandardized $ Standardized $ Corrected SE$ z score
Problem Type % Incubation Task
Visual Problem % Low
Cognitive Load Task !.21 !.12 .28 !0.77
Visual Problem % High
Cognitive Load Task .09 .08 .24 0.38
Problem type
Visual problem !.15 !.15 .21 !0.68
Incubation task
High cognitive load task !.01 !.01 .18 !0.05
Low cognitive load task .45! .41 .20 2.27
Misleading cues .13 .13 .11 1.10
Length of the preparation period/
incubation .08 .20 .05 1.56
Answer was presented .01 .01 .11 0.13

Note. Random variance component " .00031, R2 " .15. Another regression was carried out with the same
variables, replacing the variable ratio of length of preparation to incubation period with incubation period and
preparation period. The regression model was not significant, F(9, 90) " 1.27, p " .10. Neither the variable
preparation period nor incubation period was significant. ANOVA " analysis of variance.
!
p # .05.

25
INCUBATION AND PROBLEM SOLVING 109

Table 13 larger incubation effect, but an opposite pattern of results was


Summary of Coefficient Differences of the Original Regression found in studies employing visual problems, suggesting that the
Model impact of misleading cues may be modality specific. In order to
test whether the presence of misleading cues affects incubation
Coefficient Corrected with linguistic problems alone, we ran a weighted one-way anal-
Multiplicative terms difference SE z score
ysis of variance to compare the incubation effect sizes of studies
LijHj LijRj ".01 .17 "0.05 using linguistic problems that included misleading cues (25 stud-
LijLj LijRj .45! .20 2.27 ies) against studies using linguistic problems that did not include
LijLj LijHj .46 .26 1.76
misleading cues (40 studies), F(1, 65) ! 3.04, MSe ! 3.00, p !
VjHj VjRj .08 .30 0.28
VjLj VjRj .24 .34 0.69 .08. Thus, where problem materials involve linguistic stimuli,
VjLjj VjHj .15 .45 0.34 getting rid of misleading concepts may be the key to task solution,
VjHj LijHj ".06 .32 "0.17 in contrast with visual problems in which the key may be to
VjLj LijLj ".36 .35 "1.03 restructure the knowledge that is currently active. The effect of
VjRj LijRj ".15 .21 "0.68
misleading cues also offers some support for the selective-
Note. Li ! linguistic problem; H ! high cognitive demand task; R ! forgetting hypothesis, but this effect may be task specific.
rest; L ! low cognitive demand task; V ! visual problem; j ! study j. In contrast with previous reports (e.g., Dominowski & Jenrick,
!
p # .05. 1972; Dreistadt, 1969; Mednick, Mednick, & Mednick, 1964), the
moderator presence of solution-relevant cues was not found to be
a significant predictor of incubation effects. It has been hypothe-
unlikely to do so unless they encounter criterion failure as a result sized that, during an incubation period, unconscious processes
of reaching impasse. Seifert et al. (1995) offered an alternative such as spreading activation sensitize individuals to solution con-
account that also points to the criticality of experiencing failure cepts and make them more likely to utilize externally presented
and impasse for eventual success in insight problem solving. cues. To examine this hypothesis, researchers have presented the
If the hypothesis that visual problems require impasse for a answers of unsolved problems during an incubation period and
strategy switch to occur is correct, a long preparation period (i.e.,
compared their postincubation performance with that of partici-
preincubation problem solving) should be more likely to yield
pants not receiving any cues during the incubation period (e.g.,
benefits from subsequent incubation with visual problems because
Dominowski & Jenrick, 1972; Dodds, Smith, & Ward, 2002).
it allows individuals to reach impasse prior to incubation. The
Findings of these studies are equivocal, but it does appear that
results of the regression analysis and the follow-up bivariate cor-
problem solvers do not always make use of solution-relevant cues,
relations are consistent with this prediction, showing a statistically
even when the cue includes the solution itself. For example,
significant positive correlation between the incubation effect size
Chronicle, Ormerod, and MacGregor (2001) found that presenting
and the length of the preparation period with visual problems.
the nine-dot problem with a shaded background in the shape of the
A positive correlation between the length of the preparation
solution did not lead to significant levels of facilitation, even when
period and incubation effect size was also found with creative
the relevance of the shading was drawn to participants attention.
problems. A long preparation period may allow individuals to
exhaust search in one domain, making it more likely for them to However, because of the small number of studies that presented
explore a new domain in the second phase of solving. A positive solution-relevant cues (three with linguistic, seven with visual
correlation was not found when solving linguistic problems, problems, none with creative problems) and the wide variation in
though this may simply reflect the small variability in length of experimental parameters among these studies, it is impossible to
preparation period among studies using linguistic problems (the carry out further statistical analysis.
preparation period of 82% of these studies ranged from 0.5 In summary, the meta-analysis results support the existence of
to 1 min). incubation effects, though there appears to be a range of effects
The meta-analysis reveals that embedding misleading cues in specific to particular tasks and performance conditions. When
the problems was not a significant predictor overall. This result is attempting to solve creative problems that require a wide search of
in contrast with previous reports (e.g., Smith & Blankenship, 1989, knowledge, individuals benefit from an incubation period. Prob-
1991), which suggested that incubation effects arise through for- lems that involve reaching some kind of insight into a unique
getting of inappropriate information. The true effect of misleading solution do not always benefit from incubation under all condi-
cues is underestimated in our regression analyses, as we examined tions. In the case of linguistic problems, such as the RAT, there is
only the overall effect of misleading cues on problem solving in a modest incubation effect but only where the incubation period is
general. There were 29 independent studies included in this meta- filled with a low cognitive demand task. One possible explanation
analysis that examined the impact of misleading cues; 25 of them is that performing low cognitive demand incubation tasks allows
examined the impact on linguistic problems, the rest on visual the occurrence of some unconscious problem-solving processes,
problems. The weighted means of the effect size estimates of these such as spreading activation and selective forgetting. In the case of
studies for each problem type by the presence of misleading cues visual problems, incubation effects arise only where there has been
were as follows: linguistic with misleading cue: M ! 0.36, SD ! a sufficiently long preparation period prior to incubation for the
0.09; linguistic without misleading cue: M ! 0.17, SD ! 0.06; problem solver to have entered a state of impasse. Only under
visual with misleading cue: M ! 0.18, SD ! 0.39; visual without these conditions can an incubation period contribute to the strate-
misleading cue: M ! 0.26, SD ! 0.08. For studies employing gic shift needed to restructure a problem representation. Thus, the
linguistic problems, the presence of misleading cues induced a theoretical positions of spreading activation, selective forgetting,

26
110 SIO AND ORMEROD

and restructuring each receive support. However, evidence for is not amenable to drawing strong cross-study conclusions. It is
each appears to be specific to particular problem types. perhaps disappointing that relatively few published studies met the
Spreading activation and strategic search are basic mechanisms necessary criteria for inclusion in the meta-analysis, since a failure
underlying different types of general cognitive process. For in- either to measure postincubation performance or to provide effect
stance, the adaptive control of thoughtrational (ACT-R) com- size information limits the conclusions that can be drawn from
putational framework (Anderson, 1994) utilizes both these mech- them. Nonetheless, sufficient studies remain for the meta-analysis
anisms. Different researchers have successfully adapted this model to be undertaken and to reveal some intriguing results.
to simulate a wide range of noninsight problem-solving processes. One remaining problem is the relatively narrow range of prob-
Examining the occurrence of incubation effects in terms of fun- lem types that have been explored. For instance, the majority of
damental cognitive processes would offer important findings for studies that explored incubation effects with linguistic problems,
developing a computational model of insight problem solving. which is the majority of studies overall, used the RAT. It is unclear
Although the conscious-work hypothesis receives little support whether the RAT can be considered an insight problem or a
here, the meta-analysis leaves open the possibility that uncon- linguistic completion task, suggesting it may not be representative
scious processes may reflect forgetting, activation of new knowl- of all linguistic problem-solving tasks. Bowden and Jung-Beeman
edge, or restructuring. Further experimental studies might focus on (2003a) have found that participants claimed that they solved
comparing the occurrence of these different unconscious processes RATs sometimes with insight and sometimes without insight.
during an incubation period in different experimental settings. One Further studies should aim to explore task-specific experimental
methodology that might allow such comparisons was employed by settings for maximizing the incubation effect with a wider range of
Sio and Rudowicz (2007), who examined the occurrence of tasks. A further research issue of value might also be to explore
spreading activation by measuring individuals sensitivity to an- individual differences in incubation effects. For example, if the
swers of the unsolved RATs before and after filled and unfilled role of incubation is to encourage diffused attention, then individ-
incubation periods. They found that the spreading-activation pro- uals who show a propensity toward allocating attention broadly
cess occurred only in the filled incubation period condition and in (e.g., as measured via field dependence) may benefit differentially
a fixated mind, though this study did not measure postincubation from an incubation period. Also, studies have revealed that strat-
period performance. egy switching is related to working memory capacity (Geary,
Given the outcome that both spreading activation and restruc- Hoard, Byrd-Craven, & DeSoto, 2004). Thus, memory capacity
turing might arise from an incubation period, depending on the may also interact with incubation effects in solving visual prob-
task, then proponents of both the current views of insight problem lems.
solving may take some comfort from the results. The evidence One the whole, the results of this meta-analysis support the
from the linguistic problems is consistent with release from inap- existence of multiple types of problem-specific incubation effect.
propriate constraints (Knoblich et al., 1999), whereas evidence We suggest that the concept of incubation can be understood only
from the visual problems is consistent with restructuring that through a close examination of the problems to which it is applied
results from a strategic shift following impasse (MacGregor et al., and the conditions under which it is elicited.
2001). Given that these theoretical alternatives have to date been
explored only with different task sets, it seems quite possible that
a complete account of insight might need both theoretical compo- References
nents. Such a view may well be consistent with the account of
insight offered by Kershaw and Ohlsson (2004), who proposed a References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the
meta-analysis.
multiple-source account of the difficulties individuals encounter in
insight problem solving. Anderson, R. J. (1994). Representations and requirements: The value of
The finding of a positive impact of an incubation period on ethnography in system design. HumanComputer Interaction, 9, 151
solving creative thinking problems supports the contention that 182.
Ansburg, P. I., & Hill, K. (2003). Creative and analytic thinkers differ in
incubation periods help the elicitation of new ideas. Incubation is
their use of attentional resources. Personality and Individual Differ-
a concept central to many methodologies for encouraging creative ences, 34, 11411152.
decision making, especially among management science and busi- Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., &
ness communities (e.g., Rickards, 1991), and this result may be van IJzendoorn, M. J. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious
taken as supporting the inclusion of an incubation phase in such and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bul-
methodologies. letin, 133, 124.
It should be noted that, despite efforts to include studies from !Beck, J. (1979). The effect of variations in incubation activity and
sources other than peer-reviewed journals, the meta-analysis may incubation time on creative response production. Unpublished doctoral
be influenced by a bias in favor of reporting significant effects at dissertation, New York University.
the expense of null effects. Thus, incubation effects may be to !Bennett, S. (1975). Effect of incubation on the associative process of
creativity. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Notre Dame.
some extent overstated in this meta-analysis, a problem common to
!Both, L., Needham, D., & Wood, E. (2004). Examining tasks that facil-
all meta-analyses. Nonetheless, the reasonably large effect sizes
itate the experience of incubation while problem solving. Alberta Jour-
found with creative problems indicates that, with this class of nal of Educational Research, 50(1), 57 67.
problem at any rate, incubation is a potentially valuable mecha- Bowden, E. M., & Jung-Beeman, M. (2003a). Aha! Insight experience
nism for fostering creative thought. correlates with solution activation in the right hemisphere. Psychonomic
Clearly, the empirical data on incubation are not straightfor- Bulletin & Review, 10, 730 737.
ward. As a consequence, the traditional narrative review approach Bowden, E. M., & Jung-Beeman, M. (2003b). Normative data for 144

27
INCUBATION AND PROBLEM SOLVING 111

compound remote associate problems. Behavior Research Methods, underlying insight. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of
Instruments, & Computers, 35(5), 634 639. Georgia.
Bowers, K. S., Regehr, G., Balthazard, C. G., & Parker, K. (1990). Jung-Beeman, M., & Bowden, E. (2000). The right hemisphere maintains
Intuition in the context of discovery. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 72110. solution-related activation for yet-to-be solved insight problems. Mem-
!Brockett, C. (1985). Neuropsychological and cognitive components of ory and Cognition, 28, 12311241.
creativity and incubation. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Virginia !Kaplan, C. (1990). Hatching a theory of incubation: Does putting a
Commonwealth University. problem aside really help? If so, why? Unpublished doctoral disserta-
!Browne, B. A., & Cruse, D. F. (1988). The incubation effect: Illusion or tion, Carnegie Mellon University.
illumination. Human Performance, 1(3), 177185. Kershaw, T. C., & Ohlsson, S. (2004). Multiple causes of difficulty in
!Christensen, B. T., & Schunn, C. D. (2005). Spontaneous access and insight: The case of the nine-dot problem. Journal of Experimental
analogical incubation effects. Creativity Research Journal, 17(23), Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 313.
207220. Knoblich, G., Ohlsson, S., Haider, H., & Rhenius, D. (1999). Constraint
Chronicle, E. P., Ormerod, T. C., & MacGregor, J. N. (2001). When insight relaxation and chunk decomposition in insight problem solving. Journal
just wont come: The failure of visual cues in the nine-dot problem. of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25,
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54(A), 903919. 1534 1555.
Cooper, H., & Hedges, L. V. (1994). The handbook of research synthesis. Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. Thousand
New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Oaks, CA: Sage.
!Dodds, R. A., Smith, S. M., & Ward, T. B. (2002). The use of environ- Lynch, M. D., & Swink, E. (1967). Some effects of priming, incubation
mental clues during incubation. Creativity Research Journal, 14(3 4), and creative aptitude on journalism performance. Journal of Communi-
287305. cation, 17(4), 372382.
Dodds, R. A., Ward, T. B., & Smith, S. M. (in press). Incubation in Macaskill, P., Walter, S. D., & Irwing, L. (2001). A comparison of methods
problem solving and creativity. In M. A. Runco (Ed.), Creativity re- to detect publication bias in meta-analysis. Statistics in Medicine, 20,
search handbook (Vol. 3). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press. 641 654.
!Dominowski, R. L., & Jenrick, R. (1972). Effect of hints and interpolated MacGregor, J. N., Ormerod, T. C., & Chronicle, E. P. (2001). Information-
activity on solution of an insight problem. Psychonomic Science, 26(6), processing and insight: A process model of performance on the nine-dot
335338.
and related problems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
!Dorfman, J. (1990). Metacognitions and incubation effects in insight
Memory, and Cognition, 27, 176 201.
problem solving. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Cali-
Martindale, C. (1995). Creativity and connectionism. In S. M. Smith, T. B.
fornia, San Diego.
Ward, & R. A. Finke (Eds.), The creative cognition approach (pp.
!Dreistadt, R. (1969). The use of analogies and incubation in obtaining
249 268). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
insights in creative problem solving. The Journal of Psychology, 71,
!Medd, E., & Houtz, J. C. (2002). The effects of facilitated incubation on
159 175.
fourth graders creative writing. Educational Research Quarterly, 26(2),
Durlak, J. A., & Lipsey, M. W. (1991). A practitioners guide to meta-
1316.
analysis. American Journal of Community Psychology, 19, 291332.
!Mednick, M., Mednick, S., & Mednick, E. (1964). Incubation of creative
Finke, R. A., Ward, T. B., & Smith, S. M. (1992). Creative cognition:
performance and specific associative priming. Journal of Abnormal and
Theory, research, and applications. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Social Psychology, 69, 84 88.
!Fulgosi, A., & Guilford, J. P. (1968). Short-term incubation in divergent
Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process.
production. American Journal of Psychology, 81, 241246.
Psychological Review, 69, 220 232.
!Fulgosi, A., & Guilford, J. (1972). A further investigation of short-term
incubation. Acta Instituti Psychologici, 64 73, 6770. Metcalfe, J., & Weibe, D. (1987). Intuition in insight and non-insight
!Gall, M., & Mendelsohn, G. A. (1967). Effects of facilitating techniques problem-solving. Memory and Cognition, 15, 238 246.
and subject experimenter interaction on creative problem solving. Jour- !Moss, S. A. (2002). The impact of environmental clues in problem
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 5, 211216. solving and incubation: The moderating effect of ability. Creativity
Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., Byrd-Craven, J., & DeSoto, M. C. (2004). Research Journal, 14(2), 207211.
Strategy choices in simple and complex addition: Contributions of !Murray, H. G., & Denny, J. P. (1969). Interaction of ability level and
working memory and counting knowledge for children with mathemat- interpolated activity (opportunity for incubation) in human problem
ical disability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 88, 121151. solving. Psychological Reports, 24(1), 271276.
Ghiselin, B. (1985). The creative process: A symposium. Berkeley: Uni- Newton, E. J., & Roberts, M. J. (2005). The window of opportunity: A
versity of California Press. model for strategy discovery. In M. J. Roberts & E. J. Newton (Eds.),
Gilhooly, K. J., & Murphy, P. (2005). Differentiating insight from non- Methods of thought: Individual differences in reasoning strategies (pp.
insight problems. Thinking and Reasoning, 11, 279 302. 129 158). Hove, United Kingdom: Psychology Press.
!Goldman, W., Wolters, N., & Winograd, E. (1992). A demonstration of Olton, R. M. (1979). Experimental studies of incubation: Searching for the
incubation in anagram problem solving. Bulletin of the Psychonomic elusive. Journal of Creative Behavior, 13, 9 22.
Society, 30, 36 38. !Olton, R. M., & Johnson, D. M. (1976). Mechanism of incubation in
!Hansberry, M. T. (1998). Fixation and incubation effects in problem creative problem solving. American Journal of Psychology, 89(4), 617
solving. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Hampshire, 630.
United Kingdom. Patalano, A. L., & Seifert, C. M. (1994). Memory for impasses during
Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. problem solving. Memory and Cognition, 22, 234 242.
Orlando, FL: Academic Press. !Patrick, A. S. (1986). The role of ability in creative incubation. Per-
!Henley, R. J. (1999). Priming incubated problems. Unpublished doctoral sonality and Individual Differences, 7(2), 169 174.
dissertation, University of Sussex, United Kingdom. !Penney, C. G., Godsell, A., Scott, A., & Balsom, R. (2004). Problem
!Houtz, J. C., & Frankel, A. D. (1992). Effects of incubation and imagery variables that promote incubation effects. Journal of Creative Behavior,
training on creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 5(2), 183189. 38, 3555.
!Jamieson, B. A. (1999). Incubation and aging: The nature of processing Perkins, D. N. (1995). Insight in minds and genes. In R. J. Sternberg & J. E.

28
112 SIO AND ORMEROD

Davidson (Eds.), The nature of insight (pp. 495533). Cambridge, MA: !Sio, U. N., & Rudowicz, E. (2007). The role of an incubation period in
MIT Press. creative problem solving. Creativity Research Journal, 19(23), 307
!Peterson, C. (1974). Incubation effects in anagram solution. Bulletin of 318.
the Psychonomic Society, 3, 29 30. Smilek, D., Enns, J. T., Eastwood, J. D., & Merikle, P. A. (2006). Relax!
Posner, M. I. (1973). Cognition: An introduction. Glenview, IL: Scott, Cognitive style influences visual search. Visual Cognition, 14, 543564.
Foresman. Smith, S. M. (1995). Fixation, incubation, and insight in memory and
Rae, C. M. (1997). The creative power of doing nothing. Writer, 110(7), creative thinking. In S. M. Smith, T. B. Ward, & R. A. Finke (Eds.), The
1315. creative cognition approach (pp. 135146). Cambridge, MA: MIT
Reder, L. M., & Schunn, C. D. (1996). Metacognition does not imply Press.
awareness: Strategy choice is governed by implicit learning and mem- !Smith, S. M., & Blankenship, S. E. (1989). Incubation effects. Bulletin of
ory. In L. M. Reder (Ed.), Implicit memory and metacognition (pp. the Psychonomic Society, 27(4), 311314.
4578). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. !Smith, S. M., & Blankenship, S. E. (1991). Incubation and the persistence
Rickards, T. (1991). Innovation and creativity: Woods, trees and pathways. of fixation in problem solving. American Journal of Psychology, 104,
R&D Management, 21(2), 97108. 61 87.
Rosenthal, R. (1995). Writing meta-analytic reviews. Psychological Bul- !Snyder, A., Mitchell, D. J., Ellwood, S., Yates, A., & Pallier, G. (2004).
letin, 118, 183192. Nonconscious idea generation. Psychological Reports, 94, 13251330.
Schank, R. C. (1982). Dynamic memory: A theory of reminding and Thornton, A., & Lee, P. (2000). Publication bias in meta-analysisIts
learning in computers and people. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cam- causes and consequences. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 53(2),
bridge University Press. 207216.
Schank, R. C. (1999). Dynamic memory revisited. Cambridge, United !Torrance-Perks, J. (1997). The incubation effect: Implication for under-
Kingdom: Cambridge University Press. lying mechanisms. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Wa-
Scheerer, M. (1963). Problem solving. Scientific American, 208(4), 118 terloo, Ontario, Canada.
128. VanLehn, K. (1988). Towards a theory of impasse-driven learning. In H.
Schooler, J. W., Ohlsson, S., & Brooks, K. (1993). Thoughts beyond Mandl & A. Lesgold (Eds.), Learning: Issues for intelligent tutoring
words: When language overshadows insight. Journal of Experimental systems (pp. 19 41). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Psychology: General, 122, 166 183. !Vul, E., & Pashler, H. (2007). Incubation is helpful only when people are
!Seabrook, R., & Dienes, Z. (2003, July). Incubation in problem solving as misled. Memory and Cognition, 35(4), 701710.
a context effect. In R. Alterman & D. Kirsh (Eds.), Proceedings of the Wallas, G. (1926). The art of thought. London: Cape.
25th annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 10651069). Webster, A., Campbell, C., & Jane, B. (2006). Enhancing the creative
Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society. Available from http:// process for learning in primary technology education. International
www.cogsci.rpi.edu/CSJarchive/Proceedings/2003/pdfs/203.pdf Journal of Technology & Design Education, 16(3), 221235.
!Segal, E. (2004). Incubation in insight problem solving. Creativity Re- Wertheimer, M. (1985). A Gestalt perspective on computer simulations of
search Journal, 16(1), 141149. cognitive processes. Computers in Human Behavior, 1, 19 33.
Seifert, M. C., Meyer, D. E., Davidson, N., Patalano, A. L., & Yaniv, I. Woodworth, R. S., & Schlosberg, H. (1954). Experimental psychology.
(1995). Demystification of cognitive insight: Opportunistic assimilation New Delhi, India: Oxford & IBH.
and the prepared-mind perspective. In R. J. Sternberg & J. E. Davidson !Yaniv, I., & Meyer, D. E. (1987). Activation and meta-cognition of
(Eds.), The nature of insight (pp. 65124). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. inaccessible stored information: Potential bases for incubation effects in
Siegler, R. S., & Stern, E. (1998). Conscious and unconscious strategy problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Mem-
discoveries: A microgenetic analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychol- ory, and Cognition, 13(2), 187205.
ogy: General, 127, 377397. Zeigarnik, B. (1927). Untersuchungen zur Handlungs- und Affektpsy-
!Silveira, J. (1972). Incubation: The effect of interruption timing and chologie: III. Das Behalten erledigter und unerledigter andlungen [In-
length on problem solution and quality of problem processing. Unpub- vestigations on the psychology of action and affection: III. The memory
lished doctoral dissertation, University of Oregon. of completed and uncompleted actions]. Psychologische Forschung, 9,
Simon, H. A. (1966). Scientific discovery and the psychology of problem 1 85.
solving. In R. G. Colodny (Ed.), Mind and cosmos: Essays in contem- Zeigarnik, B. (1938). On finished and unfinished tasks. In W. D. Ellis
porary science and philosophy (pp. 22 40). Pittsburgh, PA: University (Ed.), A sourcebook of Gestalt psychology (pp. 300 314). New York:
of Pittsburgh Press. Humanities Press.

29
INCUBATION AND PROBLEM SOLVING 113

Appendix A

Task Descriptions
Task type Description Solution or sample task

Creative problems
Brick task Participant has to list all the uses he
or she can think of for a brick
Consequences task Participant has to list as many Sample task: What would be the results
consequences of an event as he if everyone suddenly lost the ability
or she can foresee to read and write?
Creative writing Sample task: Write about three concrete
objects: a Koosch ball, a wooden
type of propeller, and a triangular
frisbee
Visual insight problems
Candle problem Participant has to support a candle Solution: Use a tack to attach box to
on a wall by using some matches the wall, then drip some wax onto
and a box of tacks the box and mount the candle on the
box
Farm problem Participant has to divide an L- Solution:
shaped farm into four parts that
have the same size and shape

Hat rack problem Participant has to construct a stable Solution: Wedge the two boards
hat rack by using two boards and between the ceiling and the floor,
a clamp holding them in place with the
clamp, with the clamp also serving
as a hook
Insightful mathematic problem Participant has to compute Solution: Restructure the given shape
separately the area of the square into partially overlapping triangles
and that of the parallelogram ABG and ECD. The sum of their
shown below: area is 2 ! ab/2 " ab

Necklace problem Participant is given four pieces of Solution: Open all three links of one
chain, each made up of three chain, and join the other three chains
links; he or she has to join all the together
pieces by opening and closing
only three links
(table continues)

30
114 SIO AND ORMEROD

Table (continued)
Task type Description Solution or sample task

Radiation problem A patient has an inoperable tumor Solution: Direct multiple low-intensity
in the middle of the body. There rays simultaneously toward the tumor
is a ray at a strong intensity that from different directions
can destroy the tumor, but the ray
also harms the healthy tissue that
it travels through. At low
intensities, the ray will spare the
healthy tissue but will not destroy
the tumor. Participant has to
think out a way to use the ray to
destroy the tumor without
damaging healthy tissue.
Saugstads ball problem Participant has to transfer steel balls Solution: Bend the nail into a hook
from a drinking glass to a then attach it to the string. Use it to
cylinder from a distance of 8 ft drag back the frame and remove the
by using the following objects: a balls. Transfer balls into the cylinder
nail, a pair of pliers, a length of using a tube constructed of
string, a pulley, elastic bands, and newspaper and elastic band.
a newspaper. The glass sits on a
moveable frame.
Tree problem Participant has to plant 10 trees in Solution:
five rows with four trees in each
row

(The dots represent trees)


Linguistic insight problems
Anagram Participant has to rearrange the Sample task: The scambled letters are
scrambled letters to form a new t s l t i n e, and one possible
word solution is silent
Remote associates task (RAT) Three words are presented to the Sample task: The three stimulus words
participant, and he or she has to are blue, cake, and cottage, and one
think of a word that can form possible solution is the word cheese
associations with each one
Rebus Participant has to figure out the Sample task: The pictogram
phrase portrayed by the
pictogram

The answer is first aid


Riddle Sample task: A wine bottle is half-
filled and corked. How can you
drink all of the wine without
removing the cork from the bottle?
Answer: The cork can be opened by
pushing it in.
Word associates task Six words are presented to the Sample task: The six stimulus words
participant; he or she has to think are school, chair, jump, noon, heels,
out a word that can form an and wire, and one possible solution
association with each of them is the word high
Word fragment completion Participant has to complete a word Sample task: The stimulus is OC_
task that has various letters missing _N and one possible answer is
ocean

31
INCUBATION AND PROBLEM SOLVING 115

Appendix B

Information Extracted From Each Independent Study


Presence of Preparation Incubation Presence of
ID Total misleading period in period in relevant Unbiased
No. Year Author N Problem typea cuesb minutes minutes Incubation task typec cuesd effect size

1 1967 Gall & Mendelsohn 60 2 (RAT) 0 2 25 2 (nonverbal task) 0 !0.58


2 1967 Gall & Mendelsohn 60 2 (RAT) 0 2 25 2 (associations 1 !0.58
training)
3 1968 Fulgosi & Guilford 50 1 (consequences task) 0 2 10 2 (number series task) 0 0
4 1968 Fulgosi & Guilford 49 1 (consequences task) 0 2 20 2 (number series task) 0 0.52
5 1969 Dreistadt 20 1 (farm problem) 0 5 8 1 (guess playing card) 0 0.34
6 1969 Dreistadt 20 1 (farm problem) 0 5 8 1 (guess playing card) 1 1.68
7 1969 Dreistadt 20 1 (tree problem) 0 5 8 1 (guess playing card) 0 0.21
8 1969 Dreistadt 20 1 (tree problem) 0 5 8 1 (guess playing card) 1 2.80
9 1969 Murray & Denny 36 1 (Saugstads ball 0 5 5 2 (multiple choice 0 0.62
(Experiment 1) problem, low syllogisms and
ability group) traced sequences of
numbers and
letters)
10 1969 Murray & Denny 36 1 (Saugstads ball 0 5 5 2 (multiple choice 0 !0.59
(Experiment 2) problem, high syllogisms and
ability group) traced sequences of
numbers and
letters)
11 1972 Dominowski & Jenrick 27 1 (hat rack problem) 5 10 2 (free association) 0 0
12 1972 Dominowski & Jenrick 30 1 (hat rack problem) 3 3 2 (free association) 0 0
13 1972 Silveira (Experiment 1) 18 1 (necklace problem) 0 3 30 1 (read book) 0 0.11
14 1972 Silveira (Experiment 1) 18 1 (necklace problem) 0 3 210 1 (read book for 30 0 0.06
min and free
activity for 3 hr)
15 1972 Silveira (Experiment 1) 18 1 (necklace problem) 0 13 30 1 (read book) 0 0.17
16 1972 Silveira (Experiment 1) 18 1 (necklace problem) 0 13 210 1 (read book for 30 0 0.42
min and free
activity for 3 hr)
17 1972 Silveira (Experiment 2) 32 1 (necklace problem) 0 13 210 1 (read book for 30 0 0.44
min and free
activity for 3 hr)
18 1974 Peterson 24 1 (anagram) 0.33 1.8 Anagram 1 0.65
19 1976 Olton & Johnson 21 1 (farm problem) 0 10 15 0 (rest) 0 0.10
20 1976 Olton & Johnson 21 1 (farm problem) 0 10 15 2 (Stroop test " 0 0.11
counting backward)
21 1976 Olton & Johnson 21 1 (farm problem) 0 10 15 2 (review the 1 0.10
problem)
22 1976 Olton & Johnson 21 1 (farm problem) 0 10 15 2 (have lecture) 1 0
23 1976 Olton & Johnson 20 1 (farm problem) 0 10 15 0 (listen to music) 0 !0.10
24 1976 Olton & Johnson 20 1 (farm problem) 0 10 15 0 (rest) 1 0.10
25 1976 Olton & Johnson 20 1 (farm problem) 0 10 15 2 (have lecture) 1 !0.03
26 1979 Beck 60 0 (verbal divergent- 0 12 20 0 (relax) 0 2.19
thinking task)
27 1979 Beck 60 0 (verbal divergent- 0 12 30 2 (write essay) 0 1.07
thinking task)
28 1979 Beck 60 0 (verbal divergent- 0 12 20 0 (relax) 0 4.07
thinking task)
29 1979 Beck 60 0 (verbal divergent- 0 12 30 2 (write essay) 0 4.04
thinking task)
30 1985 Brockett 30 0 (brick task) 0 10 20 2 (questionnaire) 0 0.42
31 1985 Brockett 30 2 (RAT) 0 0.33 20 2 (questionnaire) 0 0.37
32 1986 Patrick 30 2 (RAT) 0 2 5 2 (conversation) 0 0
33 1986 Patrick 30 2 (RAT) 0 2 5 2 (mental rotation 0 0.66
task)
34 1988 Browne & Cruse 60 1 (farm problem) 0 20 5 0 (listen to music) 0 0.47
(Experiment 2)
35 1988 Browne & Cruse 53 1 (farm problem) 0 20 5 1 (graph drawing) 1 0.24
(Experiment 2)
(table continues)

32
116 SIO AND ORMEROD

Table (continued)
Presence of Preparation Incubation Presence of
ID Total misleading period in period in relevant Unbiased
No. Year Author N Problem typea cuesb minutes minutes Incubation task typec cuesd effect size

36 1988 Browne & Cruse 55 1 (farm problem) 0 20 5 2 (memory test) 0 !0.18


(Experiment 2)
37 1989 Smith & Blankenship 26 2 (rebus) 1 0.5 5 0 (rest) 0 0.45
(Experiment 1)
38 1989 Smith & Blankenship 26 2 (rebus) 1 0.5 5 2 (music perception 0 0.48
(Experiment 1) task)
39 1989 Smith & Blankenship 26 2 (rebus) 1 0.5 15 0 (rest) 0 0.49
(Experiment 1)
40 1989 Smith & Blankenship 26 2 (rebus) 1 0.5 15 2 (music perception 0 0.50
(Experiment 1) task)
41 1989 Smith & Blankenship 25 2 (rebus) 1 0.5 5 0 (rest) 1 0.05
(Experiment 2)
42 1989 Smith & Blankenship 25 2 (rebus) 1 0.5 5 2 (music perception 1 0.05
(Experiment 2) task)
43 1989 Smith & Blankenship 25 2 (rebus) 1 0.5 15 0 (rest) 1 0.40
(Experiment 2)
44 1989 Smith & Blankenship 25 2 (rebus) 1 0.5 15 2 (music perception 1 0.40
(Experiment 2) task)
45 1989 Smith & Blankenship 29 2 (rebus) 1 0.5 10 2 (rebus) 1 0.56
(Experiment 3)
46 1989 Smith & Blankenship 29 2 (rebus) 1 0.5 15 0 (rebus " rest) 1 0.56
(Experiment 3)
47 1989 Smith & Blankenship 29 2 (rebus) 1 0.5 15 2 (rebus " math) 1 0.56
(Experiment 3)
48 1989 Smith & Blankenship 29 2 (rebus) 1 0.5 15 0 (rebus " music) 1 0.56
(Experiment 3)
49 1989 Smith & Blankenship 29 2 (rebus) 1 0.5 15 2 (rebus) 1 0.56
(Experiment 3)
50 1989 Smith & Blankenship 49 2 (rebus) 1 0.5 5 1 (read story) 0 0
(Experiment 4)
51 1989 Smith & Blankenship 49 2 (rebus) 1 0.5 5 2 (math task) 0 0.49
(Experiment 4)
52 1990 Dorfman (Experiment 15 2 (word associate 0 0.49 15 2 (number series task) 0 0
3) task)
53 1990 Dorfman (Experiment 15 2 (word associate 0 0.49 5 2 (number series task) 0 1.07
3) task)
54 1990 Dorfman (Experiment 27 2 (word associate 0 0.49 3 2 (number series task) 0 0
4) task)
55 1990 Dorfman (Experiment 27 2 (word associate 0 0.49 8 2 (number series task) 0 0
4) task)
56 1990 Dorfman (Experiment 26 2 (word associate 0 0.49 13 2 (number series task) 0 0
4) task)
57 1990 Kaplan (Experiment 1) 278 0 (consequences task) 0 2 30 2 (psychometric test 0 0.06
battery)
58 1990 Kaplan (Experiment 2) 64 0 (consequences task) 0 2 32 2 (division problem) 0 0.71
59 1990 Kaplan (Experiment 3) 36 0 (consequences task) 0 2 30 2 (lecture) 0 0
60 1990 Kaplan (Experiment 4) 20 0 (consequences task) 0 4.57 28.08 2 (division and insight 0 1.08
problem)
61 1991 Smith & Blankenship 18 2 (RAT) 1 0.5 5 1 (read science 0 1.70
(Experiment 1) fiction)
62 1991 Smith & Blankenship 21 2 (RAT) 0 0.5 5 1 (read science 0 0.64
(Experiment 1) fiction)
63 1991 Smith & Blankenship 30 2 (RAT) 0 1 5 1 (read science 0 0.36
(Experiment 2) fiction)
64 1991 Smith & Blankenship 30 2 (RAT) 1 1 5 1 (read science 0 0.72
(Experiment 2) fiction)
65 1991 Smith & Blankenship 16 2 (RAT) 0 0.5 2 2 (free associations 0 0
(Experiment 5) task)
66 1991 Smith & Blankenship 18 2 (RAT) 0 0.5 0.5 2 (free associations 0 0
(Experiment 5) task)
67 1991 Smith & Blankenship 17 2 (RAT) 1 0.5 0.5 2 (free associations 0 0.99
(Experiment 5) task)
(table continues)

33
INCUBATION AND PROBLEM SOLVING 117

Table (continued)
Presence of Preparation Incubation Presence of
ID Total misleading period in period in relevant Unbiased
No. Year Author N Problem typea cuesb minutes minutes Incubation task typec cuesd effect size

68 1991 Smith & Blankenship 18 2 (RAT) 1 0.5 2 2 (free associations 0 0.99


(Experiment 5) task)
69 1992 Goldman et al. 36 2 (anagram) 0 0.25 20 2 (general knowledge 0 0.13
questionnaire)
70 1992 Goldman et al. 36 2 (anagram) 0 0.25 1,440 2 (general knowledge 0 0.66
questionnaire !
free activity)
71 1992 Houtz & Frankel 105 1 (creative writing) 0 10 10 1 (anagram) 0 0.03
72 1997 Torrance-Perks 15 2 (word fragment 0 0.5 10 0 (rest ! lexical 0 "0.38
(Experiment 1) completion) decision test)
73 1997 Torrance-Perks 15 2 (word fragment 0 0.5 10 0 (rest ! lexical 1 "0.41
(Experiment 1) completion task) decision test)
74 1997 Torrance-Perks 15 2 (word fragment 1 0.5 10 0 (rest ! lexical 0 0.14
(Experiment 1) completion) decision test)
75 1997 Torrance-Perks 15 2 (word fragment 1 0.5 10 0 (rest ! lexical 1 0.07
(Experiment 1) completion) decision test)
76 1997 Torrance-Perks 8 1 (candle problem) 0 1 8 2 (memory test: cues 1 0.17
(Experiment 2) presented as one of
the stimuli)
77 1997 Torrance-Perks 8 1 (candle problem) 0 1 8 2 (memory test) 0 "0.71
(Experiment 2)
78 1997 Torrance-Perks 8 1 (radiation problem) 0 5 8 1 (read story: analogy 1 0.41
(Experiment 2) to the problem)
79 1997 Torrance-Perks 8 1 (radiation problem) 0 5 8 1 (read unrelated 0 0
(Experiment 2) story)
80 1997 Torrance-Perks 8 2 (RAT) 0 1 8 2 (analogy: have the 1 0.15
(Experiment 2) same solution as
the unsolved RAT)
81 1997 Torrance-Perks 7 2 (RAT) 0 1 8 2 (neutral analogy) 0 0.14
(Experiment 2)
82 1997 Torrance-Perks 7 1 (candle problem) 1 1 8 2 (memory test: cue 1 0.56
(Experiment 2) presented a one of
the stimuli)
83 1997 Torrance-Perks 7 1 (candle problem) 1 1 8 2 (memory test) 0 "0.33
(Experiment 2)
84 1997 Torrance-Perks 7 1 (radiation problem) 1 5 8 1 (read story: analogy 1 0.52
(Experiment 2) to the problem)
85 1997 Torrance-Perks 7 1 (radiation problem) 1 5 8 1 (read unrelated 0 0
(Experiment 2) story)
86 1997 Torrance-Perks 7 2 (RAT) 1 1 8 2 (analogy: have the 1 0.14
(Experiment 2) same solution as
the unsolved RAT)
87 1997 Torrance-Perks 7 2 (RAT) 1 1 8 2 (neutral analogy) 0 0.28
(Experiment 2)
88 1998 Hansberry (Experiment 32 2 (riddle) 0 1 15 2 (RAT) 0 0.66
2)
89 1998 Hansberry (Experiment 20 2 (RAT) 0 0.5 10 2 (RAT) 0 0.04
3)
90 1998 Hansberry (Experiment 20 2 (RAT) 1 0.5 10 2 (RAT) 0 0.28
3)
91 1999 Henley (Experiment 48 2 (anagram) 0 0.25 1,440 1 (free activity) 0 0
3.2)e
92 1999 Henley (Experiment 4)e 26 2 (anagram) 0 0.93 1,440 1 (free activity) 0 0
93 1999 Jamieson (Experiment 52 2 (RAT) 0 0.33 5 2 (math problem) 0 0
1)
94 1999 Jamieson (Experiment 52 2 (RAT) 0 0.33 5 2 (math problem) 0 0.10
2)
95 2002 Dodds et al. 45 2 (RAT) 0 0.5 0.5 2 (insight problem ! 2 0.31
(Experiment 2) make a word task)
96 2002 Dodds et al. 45 2 (RAT) 0 0.5 0.5 2 (insight problem ! 1 "0.14
(Experiment 2) make a word task)
97 2002 Dodds et al. 42 2 (RAT) 0 0.5 0.5 2 (insight problem ! 0 0.10
(Experiment 2) make a word task)
(table continues)

34
118 SIO AND ORMEROD

Table (continued)
Presence of Preparation Incubation Presence of
ID Total misleading period in period in relevant Unbiased
No. Year Author N Problem typea cuesb minutes minutes Incubation task typec cuesd effect size

98 2002 Dodds et al. 70 2 (RAT) 0 0.5 0.5 2 (insight problem ! 0 0


(Experiment 2) drawing test)
99 2002 Medd & Houtz 15 1 (creative writing) 0 10 10 2 (writing) 1 1.05
100 2002 Medd & Houtz 15 1 (creative writing) 0 10 10 2 (writing) 0 0
101 2003 Seabrook & Dienese 60 2 (anagram) 0 0.25 7 2 (word generation) 1 0.74
102 2004 Both et al. 98 2 (anagram) 0 1.67 6 2 (search letter and 0 0.48
answer
questionnaire)
103 2004 Both et al. 82 2 (anagram) 0 1.67 6 2 (search letter and 0 0.09
answer
questionnaire)
104 2004 Penney et al. 9 2 (anagram) 0 5.75 30 1 (free activity) 1 0.74
(Experiment 3)
105 2004 Penney et al. 9 2 (anagram) 0 5.75 120 1 (free activity) 1 1.05
(Experiment 3)
106 2004 Penney et al. 9 2 (anagram) 0 5.75 30 1 (free activity) 0 0.62
(Experiment 3)
107 2004 Penney et al. 9 2 (anagram) 0 5.75 120 1 (free activity) 0 0.70
(Experiment 3)
108 2004 Segal 20 1 (insightful 0 20 4 1 (read paper) 0 1.17
mathematic
problem)
109 2004 Segal 21 1 (insightful 0 20 12 1 (read paper) 0 1.09
mathematic
problem)
110 2004 Segal 20 1 (insightful 0 20 4 2 (word puzzle) 0 0.90
mathematic
problem)
111 2004 Segal 23 1 (insightful 0 20 12 2 (word puzzle) 0 0.57
mathematic
problem)
112 2007 Vul & Pashler 14 2 (anagram) 0 1 5 2 (video game) 0 "0.17
(Experiment 1)
113 2007 Vul & Pashler 14 2 (anagram) 0 1 5 2 (video game) 0 "0.35
(Experiment 1)
114 2007 Vul & Pashler 14 2 (anagram) 0 1 5 2 (video game) 0 0.36
(Experiment 1)
115 2007 Vul & Pashler 14 2 (anagram) 0 1 5 2 (video game) 0 0.25
(Experiment 1)
116 2007 Vul & Pashler 25 2 (RAT) 0 1 5 2 (video game) 0 0.86
(Experiment 2)
117 2007 Vul & Pashler 25 2 (RAT) 1 1 5 2 (video game) 0 "0.14
(Experiment 2)

Note. RAT # remote associates task.


a
0 # creative problem; 1 # visual problem; 2 # linguistic problem. b0 # no; 1 # yes, embedded in problem. c0 # rest; 1 # low cognitive demand task;
2 # high cognitive demand task. d0 # no; 1 # yes. eStudy had a within-subject design: An individual was in both incubation and control conditions; the
weighting formula would not apply to this study, hence, it would be excluded from the regression analyses.

Appendix C

Description of the Regression Model for All StudiesInteractions

The regression model describing the interaction effect of nature Yj # ajXj ! b1LijHj ! b2LijLj ! b3LijRj ! b4VjHj ! b5VjLj
of problem and incubation task on the weighted unbiased effect
size estimate included the multiplicative terms between nature of ! b 6V jR j ! b 7C jH j ! b 8C jL j ! b 9C jR j ! k j,
problem (visual [V], linguistic [Li], creative [C]) and incubation where Yj is the weighted unbiased effect size estimation of the
task (high cognitive demand [H], low cognitive demand [L], rest study j and Xj is a vector of other categorical and explanatory
[R]) as predictors. The regression model is specified as follows: variables (misleading cues, cue, preparation period, and incu-

35
INCUBATION AND PROBLEM SOLVING 119

bation period) of that study. aj is the corresponding vector of action effect of nature of the problem and incubation task, we
coefficients. b1 to b9 are the coefficients of the multiplicative compared the difference between coefficients multiplicative terms
terms, and kj is the error term. The dummy variables Rj and Cj in the original model. For example, the coefficients of the com-
are eliminated through substituting equations Lij ! Vj ! Cj " pound dummies VjRj and CjRj were compared to check if an
1 and Rj ! Lj ! Hj " 1 into the model. The transformed incubation period filled with low cognitive demand tasks would
regression model is as follows: improve performance on visual problems more than on divergent
thinking tasks. A comparison between the original and the trans-
Yj " ajXj ! (b3 # b9)Lij ! (b6 # b9)Vj ! (b7 # b9)Hj
formed model indicated that the coefficient of each variable in the
! (b8 # b9)Lj ! (b1 # b7 # b3 ! b9)LijHj transformed model was actually the combination of the coeffi-
cients in the original model. The difference in the coefficients
! (b2 # b8 ! b3 ! b9)LijLj ! (b4 # b7 # b6 ! b9)VjHj between the compound dummies in the original model could be
found by reinterpreting the coefficient in the transformed model.
! (b5 # b8 # b6 ! b9)VjLj.
Table C1 displays a list of coefficient differences between com-
A regression analysis was carried out to find out the coefficient pound dummies in the original model and the equivalent combi-
of each variable in the transformed model. To examine the inter- nation of the coefficients in the transformed model.

Table C1

The Regression Coefficients in the Original and Transformed Regression Models


Coefficient difference between
compound dummies in the Combination of coefficients
original regression model in the regression analysis

LijHj#LijRjj LijHj ! Hj
LijLj#LijRjj LijLj ! Lj
VjHj#VjRjj VjHj ! Hj
VjLj#VjRjj VjLj ! Lj
CjHj#CjRjj Hj
CjLj#CjRjj Lj
VjHj#CjHjj VjHj ! Vj
LijHj#CjHjj LijHj ! Lij
VjLj#CjLjj VjLj ! Vj
LijLj#CjLjj LijLj ! Lij
VjRj#CjRjj Vj
LijRj#CjRjj Lij

Note. Li " linguistic problem; H " high cognitive demand task; R " rest; L " low cognitive demand task; V " visual
problem; C " creative problem; j " study j.

Appendix D
Description of the Regression Model for Studies Excluding Creative Problem

The third regression model describing the interaction effect of variables (misleading cues, cue, preparation period, and incubation
nature of problem and incubation task on the weighted unbiased period) of that study. aj is the corresponding vector of coefficients.
effect size estimate included the multiplicative terms between b1 to b9 are the coefficients of the multiplicative terms, and kj is the
nature of problem (visual [V], linguistic [Li]) and incubation task error term. The dummy variables Li and Rj were eliminated
(high cognitive demand [H], low cognitive demand [L], rest [R]) as through substituting equations Lij ! Vj " 1 and Rj ! Lj ! Hj "
predictors. The regression model is specified as follows: 1 into the model. The transformed regression model is as follows:

Yj " ajXj ! b1LijHj ! b2LijLj ! b3LijRj ! b4VjHj ! b5VjLj Yj " ajXj ! (b6 # b3)Vj ! (b1 # b3)Hj ! (b2 # b3)Lj

! b 6V jR j ! k j, ! (b3 # b1 # b6 ! b4)VjHj ! (b5 # b2 # b6 ! b3)VjLj.

where Yj is the weighted unbiased effect size estimation of the A regression analysis was carried out to find out the coeffi-
study j, and Xj is a vector of other categorical and explanatory cient of each variable in the transformed model. To examine the
(Appendixes continue)

36
120 SIO AND ORMEROD

interaction effect of nature of the problem and incubation task, cient of each variable in the transformed model was actually the
we compared the difference between coefficients multiplica- combination of the coefficients in the original model. The
tive terms in the original model. For example, the coefficients difference in the coefficients between the compound dummies
of the compound dummy variables VjRj and LijRj were com- in the original model could be found by reinterpreting the
pared to check if an incubation period filled with low cognitive coefficient in the transformed model. Table D1 displays a list of
demand tasks would improve performance on visual problems coefficient differences between compound dummies in the orig-
more than on linguistic insight tasks. A comparison between the inal model and the equivalent combination of the coefficients in
original and the transformed models indicated that the coeffi- the transformed model.

Table D1

The Regression Coefficients in the Original and Transformed Regression Models


Coefficient difference between
compound dummies in the Combination of coefficients
original regression model in the regression analysis

LijHjj!LijRjj Hj
LijLjj!LijRjj Lj
LijLjj!LijHjj Lj ! Hj
VjHjj!VjRjj VjHjj " Hj
VjLjj!VjRjj VjLjj " Lj
VjLjj!VjHjj VjLjj " Lj ! VjHjj ! Hj
LijHjj!VjHjj VjHjj " Vj
LijLjj!VjLjj VjLjj " Vj
LijRjj!VjRjj Vj

Note. Li # linguistic problem; H # high cognitive demand task; R # rest; L # low cognitive demand task; V # visual
problem; C # creative problem; j # study j.

Received December 11, 2007


Revision received August 13, 2008
Accepted August 14, 2008 !

37
Mem Cogn (2012) 40:966975
DOI 10.3758/s13421-012-0199-z

Dont wait to incubate: Immediate versus delayed incubation


in divergent thinking
Kenneth J. Gilhooly & George J. Georgiou &
Jane Garrison & Jon D. Reston & Miroslav Sirota

Published online: 2 March 2012


# Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2012

Abstract Previous evidence for the effectiveness of imme- thinking. In analyses of creative problem solving, it has often
diate incubation in divergent creative tasks has been weak, been claimed that setting creative problems aside for a while
because earlier studies exhibited a range of methodological can lead to novel ideas about the solution, either spontaneous-
problems. This issue is theoretically important, as a demon- ly while attending to other matters or very rapidly when the
stration of the effects of immediate incubation would previously intractable problem is revisited. Personal accounts
strengthen the case for the involvement of unconscious by eminent creative thinkers in a range of domains have
work in incubation effects. For the present experiment, we attested to this phenomenon (e.g., Csikszentmihalyi, 1996;
used a creative divergent-thinking task (alternative uses) in Ghiselin, 1952; Poincar, 1913). In his well-known four-
which separate experimental groups had incubation periods stage analysis of creative problem solving, Wallas (1926,
that were either delayed or immediate and that consisted of p. 80) labeled a stage at which the problem is set aside and
either spatial or verbal tasks. Control groups were tested not consciously addressed as incubation, and this stage is
without incubation periods, and we carried out checks for the focus of the present study.
intermittent conscious work on the target task during the Following Wallas (1926), a substantial body of experi-
incubation periods. The results showed significant incuba- mental research on incubation effects has accumulated using
tion effects that were stronger for immediate than for both insight problemsto which there is a single solution,
delayed incubation. Performance was not different between but the solver has to develop a new way of representing or
the verbal and spatial incubation conditions, and we found structuring the task in order to reach that solutionand
no evidence for intermittent conscious working during the divergent problemsto which there is no single correct
incubation periods. These results support a role for uncon- solution, but the solution process encourages seeking as
scious work in creative divergent thinking, particularly in many novel and useful ideas as possible. The prototypical
the case of immediate incubation. divergent task, which was the one used in the present study,
is the alternative-uses task, in which participants are asked
Keywords Creativity . Problem solving to generate as many uses as possible that are different from
the normal uses of one or more familiar objects, such as a
brick (Guilford, 1971; Guilford, Christensen, Merrifield, &
Creative problems are generally defined as problems that Wilson, 1978; Gilhooly, Fioratou, Anthony, & Wynn,
require the production of new approaches and solutions, 2007). In the classic laboratory paradigm for studying incu-
where by new we mean novel to the solver (Boden, bation effects, which we will label the delayed-incubation
2004). Explaining how such personally novel solutions are paradigm, participants in the incubation condition work on
reached is still a major challenge for the psychology of the target problem for an experimenter-determined amount
of time (preparation time), are then given an interpolated
K. J. Gilhooly (*) : G. J. Georgiou : J. Garrison : J. D. Reston : activity away from the target task for a fixed time (incuba-
M. Sirota
tion period), and finally return to the target problem for a
School of Psychology, University of Hertfordshire,
Hatfield, Hertfordshire AL10 9AB, UK postincubation work period. The performance of the incu-
e-mail: k.j.gilhooly@herts.ac.uk bation group is contrasted with that of a control group, who

38
Mem Cogn (2012) 40:966975 967

have worked continuously on the target task for a time equal participants had a goal of being creative. They may
to the sum of the preparation time and the postincubation have been reporting infrequent uses that they happened
conscious working time among the incubation group. A to know rather than generating uses novel to them.
recently developed variant (the immediate-incubation para- Raters tend to score infrequent responses as creative,
digm) employs an interpolated task for a fixed period although such uses may have been preknown, and there-
immediately after instructions on the target problem and fore could reflect memory retrieval rather than the
before any conscious work has been undertaken, followed generation of subjectively novel responses (Quellmalz,
by uninterrupted work on the target problem (Dijksterhuis & 1985). Gilhooly et al. (2007) developed a self-report
Meurs, 2006). method for assessing subjective novelty that addresses
the issue of individually creative responses as against
rare responses, and this method was used in the present
Previous studies of delayed- and immediate-incubation experiment. In this method, participants indicate which
effects of their responses were first thought of while doing the
task, and so were subjectively novel. Gilhooly et al.
Considerable evidence has now emerged from laboratory found converging evidence for the validity of this method of
studies for the efficacy of delayed incubationthat is, that assessing responses as personally old or new. Self-judged new
setting a problem aside after a period of work is beneficial responses were rated as significantly more creative by inde-
(see Dodds, Ward, & Smith, in press, for a qualitative review). pendent judges and were more frequently produced by partic-
A recent meta-analysis of 117 studies by Sio and Ormerod ipants with higher executive-functioning test scores. Self-
(2009) identified a positive effect of delayed incubation, in judged novel responses occurred later in the sequences of
which the overall average effect size was in the low responses, which is consistent with a reliance on memory for
medium band (mean d 0 0.32) over a range of insight the retrieval of early responses, followed by executively
and divergent tasks. For divergent tasks considered sep- demanding processes for generation of novel ideas when the
arately, the mean d was larger, 0.65, which may be pool of already known uses is exhausted.
considered to be in the highmedium band of effect sizes. Zhong, Dijksterhuis, and Galinsky (2008), using the
Overall, the existence of delayed-incubation effects can immediate-incubation paradigm with the remote associates
now be regarded as well established, particularly in the task (RAT)in which participants have to retrieve an asso-
case of divergent problem solving. ciate common to three given words (e.g., cottage, blue,
Regarding the efficacy of immediate incubation, Dijksterhuis mouse? Answer: cheese)found that, although immediate
and Nordgren (2006) reported studies in which better decisions incubation did not facilitate actual solution, it appeared
and more creative solutions were found when immediate to activate solution words on unsolved trials, as indicated
incubation breaks were given after the decision problems by lexical decision measures, as compared to unsolved trials
or divergent tasks had been presented. In the realm of without immediate incubation. However, it may be not-
decision problems, Nordgren, Bos, and Dijksterhuis (2011) ed that some theorists (e.g., Weisberg, 2006, p. 468)
found that delayed incubation produced better decisions than have disputed whether the RAT is a creative task, as the
did immediate incubation, and both were better than no solutions are already-known associations rather than
incubation. novel responses. A normal criterion for a creative task
However, the beneficial effects of immediate incubation is that it requires the participant to generate a response
on decision making have proven difficult to reproduce, and that is novel for the participant rather than one already
a number of unsuccessful replication attempts have now known.
been reported (e.g., Acker, 2008; Newell, Wong, Cheung, Overall, the evidence in favor of a beneficial effect of
& Rakow, 2009; Rey, Goldstein, & Perruchet, 2009; Payne, immediate incubation in creative tasks is rather weak, as
Samper, Bettman, & Luce, 2008). it is based on one study of a divergent task that did not
The present study concerns creative thinking using a require novel responses (Dijksterhuis & Meurs, 2006)
divergent task, and Dijksterhuis and Meurs (2006) did report and another study (Zhong et al., 2008) using a conver-
that, in their Experiment 3, participants produced responses gent task (the RAT) in which the responses are not
of higher rated average creativity when the instructions to themselves creative. The question of whether immediate
list things one can do with a brick were followed immedi- incubation is effective in creative tasks is important for
ately by a 3-min distractor task (immediate incubation) its bearing on theories of incubation, and the present
before generating uses, relative to participants who began study aimed to provide more solid evidence regarding
generating uses right away. It may be noted that the instruc- the efficacy, or otherwise, of immediate incubation than
tions did not ask for unusual uses, which is the norm in has been available hitherto. We now outline the main
divergent-thinking tasks, and so it is not clear whether theories regarding incubation effects.

39
968 Mem Cogn (2012) 40:966975

Theories of incubation effects arises of what form unconscious work might take. Is it
possible that unconscious work could be just like conscious
Intermittent conscious work This theory suggests that work, but carried out without conscious awareness? Or is it
although incubation is intended to be a period without better thought of as automatic spreading activation along
conscious work on the target task, nevertheless participants associative links, as against a rule- or strategy-governed
may carry out intermittent conscious work (Seifert, Meyer, activity? We will consider the question of what form uncon-
Davidson, Patalano, & Yaniv, 1995, p. 82; Weisberg, 2006, scious work might take more fully in the Discussion section.
pp. 443445). Any conscious work during the supposed The possible mechanisms outlined above are not mu-
incubation period would reduce the time required when the tually exclusive. A delayed-incubation condition could
target problem was readdressedbut would be expected to conceivably evoke all three mechanisms, with the per-
impair performance on the interpolated task. As a check son engaging in some intermittent conscious work when
against the possibility of intermittent conscious work, per- attention wanders from the interpolated incubation task,
formance on the interpolated task during the incubation and with some beneficial forgetting and unconscious
period should be compared with the performance of a con- work taking place when the person is attending to the
trol group working on the same interpolated task without interpolated incubation task. However, an immediate-
being in an incubation condition. A deficit in the interpolat- incubation effect would not be consistent with a fresh-
ed task on the part of the incubation group would be con- look explanation but could involve some intermittent
sistent with the hypothesis of some conscious work on the conscious work and/or some unconscious work. The
target task occurring during incubation. Although this seems present study aimed to clarify the contributions of the
a rather basic methodological check, surprisingly, it does not three types of processes in explaining immediate and
appear to have been carried out in previous research (Dodds delayed incubation, without assuming that one and only
et al., in press; Sio & Ormerod, 2009). The study reported one process can explain all of the findings.
here, on the other hand, did incorporate suitable checks for
intermittent conscious work on the target task during the
incubation period. Theories of incubation: Previous studies

Fresh look This view (e.g., Segal, 2004; Simon, 1966; What has previous research suggested regarding the possible
see also Dijksterhuis & Meurs, 2006) proposes an important mechanisms of incubation?
role for automatic passive reduction in idea strength or Dijksterhuis and Meurs (2006) argued, as outlined above,
activation during the incubation period. The proposal is that that in the immediate-incubation paradigm the fresh-look
misleading strategies, mistaken assumptions, and related approach may be ruled out, as there is no period of initial
mental sets weaken through forgetting during the incuba- work in which misleading fixations and sets could be
tion period, and thus a fresh start or set shifting is facili- developed. Thus, if immediate incubation is shown to be
tated when the problem is resumed. On this view, incubation effective, the unconscious-work hypothesis must remain
works by allowing the weakening of misleading approaches in contention for immediate-incubation effects, and
to the task during a break after a period of work (delayed would also be a candidate explanation for delayed incu-
incubation), thus allowing a fresh start. This approach bation. Dijksterhuis and Meurs took the beneficial effects
would not predict a beneficial effect of immediate incuba- of the immediate-incubation paradigm on a divergent
tion, because with immediate incubation there is no time for task in their Experiment 3 as support for the role of
sets or fixations to develop, so that forgetting of misleading unconscious work in incubation. However, as already
approaches cannot occur. mentioned, the task in this study did not clearly meet
the usual criteria for a creative task, and the scoring did not
Unconscious work This approach proposes that incubation distinguish infrequent from genuinely novel responses.
effects occur through active but unconscious processing of Hence, this study does not unequivocally address creative
the problem materials (as against the passive forgetting thinking as against free recall of possibly rare, but previously
processes envisaged in the fresh-look approach.) The term experienced, events from episodic and semantic memory.
unconscious work seems to have first been used in the Snyder et al. (2004) also found evidence consistent with
context of problem solving by Poincar (1913, p. 393). unconscious work from a study using the delayed-incubation
Other phrases referring to the same notion include noncon- paradigm, but with a surprise return to the target task.
scious idea generation (Snyder, Mitchell, Ellwood, Yates, Although the return to the main task was unexpected, benefi-
& Pallier, 2004) and unconscious thought (Dijksterhuis & cial effects were found, suggesting that automatic continua-
Nordgren, 2006), but we will generally use the term tion of unconscious work could have occurred when the task
unconscious work in this article. The question naturally was set aside. It should be noted, however, that Snyder et al.

40
Mem Cogn (2012) 40:966975 969

used a task that simply required production of uses for a the effects of incubationtarget task similarity in an immediate-
piece of paper, as against the generation of novel uses, so incubation paradigm, where fatigue can be ruled out, as well as
their task did not necessarily involve creative thinking in a delayed-incubation paradigm in which fatigue relief could
rather than recall. be a factor.
It is of interest that both Segal (2004) and Dijksterhuis
and Meurs (2006) used interpolated tasks during their incu-
bation periods that were different in character from the Present study: Outline
target tasks. Segals target task was spatial, while the inter-
polated tasks were verbal; Dijksterhuis and Meurss target The present study of the effects of varying incubation activi-
task was verbal, but the interpolated task was spatial. From ties (verbal vs. spatial), detailed below, used a clearly creative
Dodds et al.s (in press) extensive review, the issue of simi- verbal divergent task (alternative uses), scored for novelty as
larity between the target and interpolated tasks does not appear well as fluency, unlike the tasks of Ellwood et al. (2009) or
to have been addressed hitherto. The similarity relationship Hlie et al. (2008). Thus, the present study is clearly focused
between target and interpolated tasks could be important, in on incubation effects in creative thinking. The study used both
that the main competing hypotheses suggest different effects immediate and delayed incubation with spatial and verbal
of similarity. If unconscious work is the main process, then intervening tasks, so that the resource overlap predictions of
interpolated tasks similar to the target task should interfere the selective-forgetting and unconscious-work hypotheses, as
with any unconscious work using the same mental resources, well as the issue of the possible effects of differential fatigue
and so lead to weaker (or even reversed) incubation effects relief, could be addressed. The main aims of the study were to
when compared with the effects of dissimilar interpolated determine the extent to which immediate incubation is indeed
tasks. On the other hand, the selective-forgetting mechanism helpful in divergent creative tasks (which previous research
suggests that interpolated tasks similar to the target task would had not clearly addressed) and to assess the relative contribu-
cause greater interference, which would lead to more forget- tions of intermittent-work, unconscious-work, and fresh-look
ting and enhanced incubation benefits. mechanisms of incubation in such tasks.
Hlie, Sun, and Xiong (2008) found that more execu-
tively demanding interpolated tasks reduced reminiscence
scores for the free recall of pictures when a surprise free Method
recall was required after the interpolated task. In their study,
participants studied booklets of pictures for a set period, In this experiment, the target task was the divergent produc-
freely recalled the items, and then did various interpolated tion of alternative uses for a brick, which we classed as a
activities before being retested with free recall of the pic- verbal task. The incubation period (which was 4 min long)
tures. The reminiscence score was the number of new items was positioned either after 5 min of conscious work or
recalled on the second test. The results were consistent with immediately after the initial divergent-task instructions. The
Hlie and Suns (2010) explicitimplicit interaction model, activities during the incubation period were either verbal
which can be applied to creative problem solving and which (anagrams) or spatial (mental rotation tasks). All participants
allows for unconscious, implicit processes to occur in par- were instructed after 5 min of divergent production to draw a
allel with conscious, explicit processes. However, Hlie et line after their last response up to that point.
al.s (2008) target task was free recall rather than creative
thinking, so it does not speak directly to divergent thinking, Participants
which is the focus of the present study.
Ellwood, Pallier, Snyder, and Gallate (2009) found a ben- A group of 184 (123 female, 61 male) students at the University
eficial effect on the number of postincubation responses of a of Hertfordshire took part in the experiment.
dissimilar interpolated task in a delayed-incubation experi-
ment. However, their study used a fluency-of-uses task rather Design
than a novel-uses task. Also, as Ellwood et al. pointed out,
although their findings are consistent with an explanation in A 2 (incubation position: immediate vs. delayed.) 3 (interpo-
terms of unconscious work, an explanation in terms of selec- lated task: none vs. verbal vs. spatial) independent-groups
tive relief of fatigue could also be invoked to account for the design was used. The ns per experimental group were as
effects of similarity between the incubation and target tasks. follows: n 0 25 with 4 min delayed incubation and a spatial
On this view, for example, a spatial delayed-incubation task interpolated task, n 0 22 with 4 min delayed incubation and a
very different from a main verbal task could allow for more verbal interpolated task, n 0 30 with 4 min immediate incuba-
recovery from specific fatigue of verbal processes than would tion and a spatial interpolated task, and n 0 30 with 4 min
an intervening verbal task. The present study includes tests of immediate incubation and a verbal interpolated task. We also

41
970 Mem Cogn (2012) 40:966975

ran separate control groups for the delayed- and immediate-


incubation conditions (ns 0 47 and 30, respectively) that pro-
vided baseline performance data for target and interpolated
tasks in the absence of incubation periods.

Procedure

In the delayed-incubation conditions, the participants were


told that they would be asked to write down possible uses
for a brick different from the usual use; after 5 min of
working, participants were told that they would be returning
to the brick-uses task later in the study. During the 4-min
incubation periods, participants either undertook verbal
tasks (anagrams) or spatial tasks (mental rotation items)
presented in booklets. Sets of 73 five-letter single-solution
Fig. 1 Mean numbers of alternative uses produced during delayed and
anagrams (from Gilhooly & Hay, 1977) and 48 mental immediate incubation using verbal or spatial interpolated tasks. Error
rotation items (from Peters et al., 1995) were used, and bars represent 1 SEM
performance was scored in terms of correct solutions during
the period allowed. After the delayed incubation periods,
there were a further 2 min of work on the brick-uses task. incubation with spatial or verbal interpolated tasks (mental
In the immediate-incubation conditions, participants were rotations or anagrams). From these figures, it seems that
given the standard instructions about the brick-uses task and both immediate and delayed incubation periods were bene-
immediately told that the experimenter wanted them to do ficial as compared to the control conditions and that imme-
another task first, after which they would return to the uses diate incubation produced better performance than did
task, and they were assigned randomly to anagrams or mental delayed incubation.
rotation for 4 min. After the immediate incubation period, they An analysis of variance (ANOVA) indicated a significant
worked on the brick-uses task for 7 min without a break. effect of the type of interpolated activity (none/verbal/spatial)
Control participants worked on the uses task for 7 min on the number of uses reported [F(2, 178) 0 7.89, p < .001,
without any incubation periods and carried out mental rota- p2 0 .08] and on the number of self-judged novel uses
tions and anagrams for 4 min each. The order of the three [F(2, 178) 0 11.49, p < .001, p2 0 .11]. Post hoc tests indicated
tasks in the control groups was randomized. The control significant differences between no incubation and incubation
rotation and anagram measures were compared with perfor- tasks of both mental rotations and anagrams for number of uses
mance on the same tasks when they were used as interven- (p < .05) and for self-judged novelty (p < .05). No significant
ing activities during the incubation periods. The control differences were found between the verbal and spatial incuba-
uses-task measures were compared with performance on tion conditions.
the uses task in the incubation conditions. An ANOVA indicated a significant effect of the position of
At the end of the brick-uses task, participants reviewed their incubation (delayed/immediate) on the number of uses
response sheets and were asked to indicate (by circling) which reported [F(1, 178) 0 6.39, p < .05, p2 0 .04] and on the
of the uses that they had reported were subjectively novelthat number of self-judged novel uses [F(1, 178) 0 10.03, p < .01,
is, had first occurred to them during the task rather than being p2 0 .05], with immediate incubation being more beneficial
previously known from past direct experience or through films, for both measures.
books, television, and so on. Gilhooly et al. (2007) found that There were no significant interactions between the type
this was a valid measure of personal originality. of incubation activity and the position of incubation activity
on the number of uses or on self-judged novelty.

Results Pre- and postincubation performance in delayed-incubation


conditions
Incubation effects
In the delayed-incubation conditions, data were available for
Figure 1 shows the average numbers of brick uses, and the uses-task performance measures separately for the 5-min
Fig. 2 the average numbers of self-judged novel uses pro- preincubation period and the 2-min postincubation period and
duced over the total of 7 min on the uses task with 0 min of for the first 5 min and last 2 min of use production in the
incubation (control data) or 4 min of delayed or immediate control (no-incubation) condition. These data were examined

42
Mem Cogn (2012) 40:966975 971

the first 5 min (which corresponded to the preincubation


time in the delayed condition) and the last 2 min (which
corresponded to the postincubation time in the delayed
condition).
One way ANOVAs were carried out on the effects of
incubation activity (none/verbal/spatial) on both uses totals
and uses novelty for the first-5-min and the last-2-min work
periods. The mean scores are shown in Table 2.
Both the first-5-min and the last-2-min scores for uses
totals and novelty were significantly different between the
incubation activity conditions (none/verbal/spatial): for the
first-5-min uses totals, F(2, 87) 0 3.29, p < .05, p2 0 .07,
and for the last-2-min uses totals, F(2, 87) 0 7.01, p < .01,
p 2 0 .14; similarly, for the first-5-min novel scores,
Fig. 2 Mean numbers of self-rated novel uses produced by the F(2, 87) 0 4.54, p < .05, p2 0 .09, and for the last-2-min
delayed- and immediate-incubation groups using verbal or spatial
interpolated tasks. Error bars represent 1 SEM
novelty scores, F(2, 87) 0 5.78, p < .01, p2 0 .12. Thus, as
would be expected, the effects of the immediate incubation
manipulation were apparent immediately, in the first 5 min,
to check that any benefits in performance relative to controls and persisted into the final 2 min.
were concentrated in the postincubation (last 2-min) period.
One-way ANOVAs were carried out on the effects of incuba- Effects of interpolation on the interpolated incubation period
tion activity (none/verbal/spatial) on uses totals and uses tasks
novelty in the first and last 2 min of work (pre- and post-
incubation periods in the incubation conditions). The mean As a check on the intermittent-conscious-work hypothesis,
scores are shown in Table 1. we compared performance on the rotation and anagram
The first-5-min scores for both uses totals and novelty tasks when they were carried out in control conditions for
were not significantly different between the incubation ac- 4 min and as interpolated tasks for 4 min in the incubation
tivity conditions (none/verbal/spatial). However, the meas- conditions. The intermittent-work hypothesis makes a one-
ures in the last 2 min (postincubation in the incubation tailed prediction that performance should be impaired on a
conditions) were significantly different between the condi- task when it is used as an interpolated incubation activity,
tions. For uses totals, F(2, 91) 0 3.45, p < .05, p2 0 .07, and relative to controls, as participants would be distracted from
for uses novelty, F(2, 91) 0 6.54, p < .01, p2 0 .11. Thus, the interpolated task by the main target task if they were
the effects of the delayed-incubation manipulation were intermittently working on the main task during incubation.
concentrated in the postincubation period, during which However, from Fig. 3 it appears that carrying out mental
incubation produced more responses and more novel rotation as an interpolated task during incubation periods did
responses than did no incubation, as would be expected. not impair correct mental-rotation performance, and t tests
found no significant differences between interpolated and
Performance over the first 5 min and the last 2 min control performance. Also, there were no significant impair-
in immediate-incubation conditions ments between anagram solution rates when anagrams were
done as an incubation activity or as a stand-alone activity.
All uses-task performance in the immediate-incubation con- The possibility of fatigue effects for the control groups,
ditions was postincubation, but it was possible to compare who did the alternative-uses task, mental rotations, and

Table 1 Delayed incubation: Pre- and postincubation scores for total brick uses and brick use novelty over incubation task conditions (control/
verbal/spatial)

Control (N 0 47) Verbal (N 0 22) Spatial (N 0 25) Control (N 0 47) Verbal (N 0 22) Spatial (N 0 25)

M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD

First 5 min/Preincubation Brick Uses Last 2 min/Postincubation Brick Uses


7.90 3.24 8.59 3.67 9.60 4.09 1.85 1.25 2.21 1.06 2.76 1.85
First 5 min/Preincubation Brick Novelty Last 2 min/Postincubation Brick Novelty
2.31 1.77 2.86 1.91 3.52 2.51 0.77 0.78 1.36 0.95 1.56 1.22

43
972 Mem Cogn (2012) 40:966975

Table 2 Immediate incubation: First-5-min and last-2-min scores for total brick uses and brick use novelty over incubation task conditions
(control/verbal/spatial)

Control (N 0 30) Verbal (N 0 30) Spatial (N 0 30) Control (N 0 30) Verbal (N 0 30) Spatial (N 0 30)

M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD

First 5 min Brick Uses Last 2 min Brick Uses


8.67 3.97 11.23 3.92 11.54 6.02 1.50 1.41 3.03 1.84 2.83 2.14
First 5 min Brick Novelty Last 2 min Brick Novelty
3.00 2.90 4.37 2.91 5.53 3.88 0.73 0.86 1.83 1.53 1.63 1.50

anagrams, should be considered, as possibly depressing conditions, again, slightly more items were attempted for rota-
their performance and thus masking any effects of intermit- tions in the incubation condition (M 0 9.17, SD 0 2.81) than in
tent work for the experimental groups. The control partic- the controls (M 0 8.87, SD 0 3.95), and the numbers of
ipants did the tasks in counterbalanced orders. An ANOVA anagrams attempted were very similar in the incubation
revealed no significant order effects for any of the tasks. (M 0 12.58, SD 0 7.57) and control (M 0 12.70, SD 0 6.89)
That is, the control scores were not depressed due to possi- conditions; these differences were not significant. We may
ble fatigue effects, and the lack of significant differences note that, as with the correct scores, these results for anagrams
between the control and incubation groups on the interpo- and rotations attempted in the control and incubation condi-
lated tasks did not reflect fatigue. The control anagram and tions are generally counter to the one-tailed prediction of the
rotation scores tended to be lower than the incubation intermittent-work hypothesis, that performance should be im-
groups scores, but not significantly. paired on interpolated tasks relative to controls.
It may be suggested that the participants did not give their Although the type of interpolated activity in the incubation
full attention to the rotation task, given the correct rate of about periods did not seem to affect the level of the groups use
four rotations in 4 min. The numbers of rotation items attemp- performance, it may be that, over participants, those who gave
ted were, of course, higher than the correct rates, with means of more attention to the interpolated tasks might have done worse
6.68 (SD 0 2.62) in the control and 7.12 (SD 0 3.14) in the on the target tasks, as they would have had less scope for
relevant incubation condition (delayed), and these figures were intermittent work on the target task during incubation than
not significantly different. In the case of anagrams, the delayed- those who attended less to the interpolated tasks. Thus,
incubation group attempted more anagrams than did the con- according to the intermittent-work hypothesis, negative corre-
trols, with means of 18.91 (SD 0 9.02) and 14.06 (SD 0 5.86), lations might be expected between performance on the inter-
respectively, F(1, 67) 0 7.02, p < .01, although the groups did polated and target tasks. In the immediate-incubation
not differ in numbers correct. In the immediate-incubation conditions, the correlations for anagrams correct in incubation
were r(28) 0 .19, n.s., with total uses, and .11, n.s., with
novel uses; for rotations correct in incubation, the correlations
were r(28) 0 .31, n.s., with total uses, and .36, p < .05, with
novel uses. In the delayed-incubation conditions, the correla-
tions for uses totals after the incubation period were
r(23) 0 .11, n.s., with anagrams correct in incubation, and
r(20) 0 .03, n.s., with rotations correct in incubation; and
finally, for novel uses after the incubation period, the correla-
tions were r(23) 0 .07, n.s., with anagrams correct, and .18,
n.s., with rotations correct in incubation. The only significant
correlation (in two-tailed tests) out of the eight was in the
direction opposite from the one predicted by the intermittent-
work hypothesisthat is, positive rather than negative.

Discussion

First, it seems that the intermittent-work hypothesis can be


Fig. 3 Mental rotation and anagram performance when carried out
ruled out, since under that hypothesis we would have expected
as an interpolated (incubation) task or as a control task. Error bars an impairment of performance on the anagram and rotation
represent 1 SEM tasks when they were performed as interpolated activities

44
Mem Cogn (2012) 40:966975 973

during the incubation periods, relative to when they are per- but carried out without conscious awareness? Perhaps un-
formed as control activities. No such negative effects were conscious work would be better thought of as involving
found. If anything, the effects were in the direction opposite spreading activation along associative links, rather than as
the one predicted by the intermittent-work hypothesis. Fur- a rule- or strategy-governed activity?
thermore, the intermittent-work hypothesis predicts negative To explore the idea that unconscious work might be a
correlations between performance on the interpolated tasks subliminal version of conscious work, it would seem useful
and on the target-uses tasks, but no significant correlations to consider the nature of conscious processing in the
were found in the predicted direction in any of the incubation alternative-uses task. This issue was addressed in a think-
conditions. Indeed, only two of the eight correlations were aloud study by Gilhooly et al. (2007), in which they found
even in the predicted, negative direction. Thus, we conclude that participants used strategies such as scanning an objects
that the effects of incubation found here cannot be explained properties (e.g., a brick is heavy) and using the retrieved
by the intermittent-work hypothesis. properties to cue uses (e.g., heavy objects can hold down
Immediate incubation produced better performance than things like sheets, rugs, tarpaulin, etc., so a heavy brick
the control condition, which constructively replicates could do those things, too). However, it seems unlikely
Dijksterhuis and Meurss (2006) finding of immediate- that unconscious work could simply duplicate the form of
incubation effects with a clearly creative divergent- conscious work without awareness. The standard views in
thinking task requiring novel uses, and our result held over cognitive science are (a) that mental contents vary in acti-
two types of incubation activity (spatial and verbal). Fur- vation levels, (b) that, above some high activation level,
thermore, immediate incubation was more efficacious than mental contents become available to consciousness, (c) that
delayed incubation for the creative task used here. The we are conscious of only a limited number of highly acti-
different effects of immediate and delayed incubation sug- vated mental elements at any one time (that is, the contents
gest that different process mixtures are involved in the two of working memory), and (d) that strategy- or rule-based
forms of incubation. A possible interpretation is that, with processing, as found in Gilhooly et al.s think-aloud study,
delayed incubation, in which conscious work is carried out requires such highly activated (conscious) material as inputs
for a period before incubation, relatively strong sets could and generates highly activated (conscious) outputs. That is,
build up, so the delayed incubation period could involve the kind of processing involved in conscious work requires
both beneficial forgetting and unconscious work. Thus, the highly activated contents of working memory, of which
delayed incubation is handicapped relative to immediate we are necessarily aware, given that material is in con-
incubation, in which sets would be expected to be nonex- sciousness if and only if it is above a high activation thresh-
istent, or at least weaker, because sets would have less time old. Thus, it seems logically impossible that unconscious
to be established and strengthened. The immediate incuba- processes could duplicate conscious processes in every re-
tion period thus could involve only unconscious work, spect and yet remain unconscious. For example, using rules
without the need to overcome sets. We found immediate and working memory to multiply two three-digit numbers
incubation followed by conscious work to be better for (e.g., 364 279 0 ?) seems impossible without having in
creative performance in the uses task than was delayed incu- working memory highly activated representations of the
bation after conscious work, which is opposite Nordgren et numbers, the goal, and the intermediate results, and such
al.s (2011) finding with a decision task, and presumably representations are necessarily conscious. Unconscious
reflects differences between divergent creative thinking as multiplication of even moderately large numbers, not previ-
compared to convergent decision making. The decision task ously practiced, seems impossible. (With practice, of course,
required participants to absorb a number of facts about the it would be possible to store many three-digit multiplication
options, a stage that may benefit from initial conscious study; results in long-term memory that could then be directly
in contrast, the uses task draws on already stored semantic retrieveda type of unconscious processbut this is not
memory of object characteristics and of the requirements for the same as mental multiplication.) Overall, then, we dis-
various functions. count the idea that unconscious work or thought could be
Our conclusion in favor of the unconscious-work hypoth- just the same as conscious work minus awareness of any
esis as a viable mechanism is based on the benefits of imme- mental content. What, then, might unconscious work consist
diate incubation, in which sets are unlikely to have been of? Many theorists, such as Poincar (1913), Campbell
developed and in which we found no evidence for intermittent (1960), and Simonton (1995), have argued that unconscious
work. This leaves unconscious work as the likeliest explana- work in incubation involves the quasi-random generation of
tion for the benefits of immediate incubation. associations between mental elements to produce novel
As mentioned in the introduction, the question arises of combinations of ideas, some of which may be useful. Pro-
what form this unconscious work might take. Is it possible cesses such as parallel spreading activation through a
that unconscious work could be just like conscious work, semantic network could serve to form remote and unusual

45
974 Mem Cogn (2012) 40:966975

associations (Jung-Beeman et al., 2004) without requiring important factor affecting the incubation process. Future
activation levels to rise above the threshold of conscious- studies will address this issue. A second major difference
ness. In Hlie and Suns (2010) recent explicitimplicit is that Ellwood et al. used a fluency task that simply re-
interaction model, incubation is regarded as involving un- quired the reporting of uses for a piece of paper, rather than
conscious implicit associative processes that demand little original or novel uses. That Ellwood et al.s task did not tap
attentional capacity, in contrast with conscious, explicit, creativity is indicated by the reported lack of correlation
rule-governed attentionally demanding processes. Accord- between performance on their target task and the personality
ing to Dijksterhuis and Nordgrens (2006) unconscious- characteristic of openness on a Big-5 personality test,
thought theory (UTT), unconscious thought, or work, has because openness typically correlates well with creative
the following characteristics: It is parallel, bottom-up, inex- divergent-test performance (Batey & Furnham, 2006). Also,
act, andimportantly for the present studydivergent, it may be noted that our results included novelty scores,
whereas conscious thought is serial, exact, and generally which Ellwood et al.s did not.
convergent. There is broad agreement among a number of Another explanation for the lack of any effect of the type of
theorists that unconscious thinking, or workin the form of incubation activity in the present study is that we may have
implicit associative processes based on spreading activation misclassified the uses task as a purely verbal task. Indeed,
is a possible explanation of incubation effects. According Gilhooly et al. (2007) did find protocol evidence of imagery
to the unconscious-work view then, a beneficial effect of processes in the uses task, and it may be that the uses task is
immediate incubation would be expected, as a useful foun- better conceived as invoking both verbal and spatial processes.
dation of novel associations could be formed by spreading If so, both types of incubation activity could have similar
activation and could be highly accessible when the use- effects, according to both the unconscious-work and
reporting stage begins. selective-forgetting hypotheses for delayed incubation. Future
A possible difficulty of our results for the unconscious- research will aim to address this point by using creative tasks
work hypothesis is that it predicts that an incubation period that are more purely spatial (e.g., mental synthesis with shapes;
on a presumed verbal task, such as the brick-uses task, Pearson, Logie, & Gilhooly, 1999) or verbal (e.g., mental
should be more beneficial if the interpolated task is nonver- synthesis with words; Haught & Johnson-Laird, 2003).
bal rather than verbal. The rationale for this prediction is that Finally, we note that our results have a clear practical
verbal processing resources would be invoked in work on a application. When faced with a task requiring that familiar
verbal interpolated task, thus depleting the resources avail- objects be used in new ways, it seems that it would be helpful
able for simultaneous unconscious work on the target task. for respondents to put aside the task immediately and return to
A spatial interpolated incubation task would not compete it after a period, allowing unconscious incubation processes to
with simultaneous unconscious verbal activity, and so operate, before undertaking conscious work.
should produce stronger incubation effects for a verbal main
task. Hlie and Suns (2010) explicitimplicit interaction Author note This work was supported by a grant from the U.K.
model of creative thinking explicitly makes this prediction Economic and Social Research Council (RES-000-22-2191) to K.J.G.
and draws on supporting results; however, these results
came from reminiscence memory rather than creative-
thinking tasks. The selective-forgetting mechanism of the References
fresh-look approach, on the other hand, leads to opposite
predictions regarding the effects of interpolated tasks. How-
Acker, F. (2008). New findings on unconscious versus conscious
ever, neither hypothesis, both of which could apply to the thought in decision making: Additional empirical data and meta-
delayed-incubation condition, was supported, as the type of analysis. Judgment and Decision Making, 3, 292303.
interpolated activity did not affect target task performance. Batey, M., & Furnham, A. (2006). Creativity, intelligence, personality:
A critical review of the scattered literature. Genetic, Social, and
Thus, the present results did not support the predictions of
General Psychology Monographs, 132, 355429.
the unconscious-work or of the fresh-look (selective-forget- Boden, M. A. (2004). The creative mind: Myths and mechanisms (2nd
ting) hypotheses regarding the effects of the type of incuba- ed.). London: Routledge.
tion activity. In this regard, our results on the effects of type Campbell, D. T. (1960). Blind variation and selective retention in
creative thought as in other knowledge processes. Psychological
of interpolated activity are contrary to those of Ellwood et
Review, 67, 380400.
al. (2009), which were in line with the unconscious-work Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). Creativity: Flow and the psychology of
hypothesis. However, some differences between the present discovery and invention. New York, NY: HarperCollins.
study and that of Ellwood et al. may be relevant. For one Dijksterhuis, A., & Meurs, T. (2006). Where creativity resides: The
generative power of unconscious thought. Consciousness and
thing, Ellwood et al. did not inform their participants that the
Cognition, 15, 135146.
target task would be returned to after incubation, whereas in Dijksterhuis, A., & Nordgren, L. F. (2006). A theory of unconscious
our study the goal of returning was stated; this might be an thought. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 95109.

46
Mem Cogn (2012) 40:966975 975

Dodds, R. A., Ward, T. B., & Smith, S. M. (in press). A review of the Payne, J., Samper, A., Bettman, J. R., & Luce, M. F. (2008). Boundary
experimental literature on incubation in problem solving and conditions on unconscious thought in complex decision making.
creativity. In M. A. Runco (Ed.), Creativity research handbook Psychological Science, 11181123.
(Vol. 3). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press. Pearson, D. G., Logie, R. H., & Gilhooly, K. J. (1999). Verbal repre-
Ellwood, S., Pallier, P., Snyder, A., & Gallate, J. (2009). The incuba- sentations and spatial manipulations during mental synthesis.
tion effect: Hatching a solution? Creativity Research Journal, 21, Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 11, 295314.
614. Peters, M., Laeng, B., Latham, K., Jackson, M., Zaiyouna, R., &
Ghiselin, B. (1952). The creative process: A symposium. New York, Richardson, C. (1995). A redrawn Vandenberg and Kuse Mental
NY: Mentor. Rotations Test: Different versions and factors that affect perfor-
Gilhooly, K. J., Fioratou, E., Anthony, S. H., & Wynn, V. (2007). Divergent mance. Brain and Cognition, 28, 3958.
thinking: Strategies and executive involvement in generating novel Poincar, H. (1913). The foundations of science. New York, NY:
uses for familiar objects. British Journal of Psychology, 98, 611625. Science House.
Gilhooly, K. J., & Hay, D. (1977). Rated characteristics of 205 five Quellmalz, E. (1985). Test review of Alternate Uses. From J. V. Mitchell,
letter words having single solution anagrams. Behavior Research Jr. (Ed.), The ninth mental measurements yearbook [Electronic
Methods & Instrumentation, 9, 1217. version]. Retrieved from Buros Institutes Test Reviews Online web-
Guilford, J. P. (1971). The nature of human intelligence. New York, site: www.unl.edu/buros
NY: McGraw-Hill. Rey, A., Goldstein, R. M., & Perruchet, P. (2009). Does unconscious
Guilford, J. P., Christensen, P. R., Merrifield, P. R., & Wilson, R. C. thought improve complex decision making? Psychological Research,
(1978). Alternate uses: Manual of instructions and interpreta- 73, 372379.
tions. Orange, CA: Sheridan Psychological Services. Segal, E. (2004). Incubation in insight problem solving. Creativity
Haught, C., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2003). Creativity and constraints: Research Journal, 16, 141148.
The production of novel sentences. In R. Alterman & D. Kirsh Seifert, C. M., Meyer, D. E., Davidson, N., Patalano, A. L., & Yaniv, I.
(Eds.), Proceedings of the 25th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive (1995). Demystification of cognitive insight: Opportunistic assimi-
Science Society (pp. 528532). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. lation and the prepared-mind perspective. In R. J. Sternberg & J. E.
Hlie, S., & Sun, R. (2010). Incubation, insight, and creative problem Davidson (Eds.), The nature of insight (pp. 65124). Cambridge,
solving: A unified theory and a connectionist model. Psychological MA: MIT Press.
Review, 117, 9941024. doi:10.1037/a0019532 Simon, H. A. (1966). Scientific discovery and the psychology of problem
Hlie, S., Sun, R., & Xiong, L. (2008). Mixed effects of distractor tasks solving. In R. Colodny (Ed.), Mind and cosmos. Pittsburgh, PA:
on incubation. In B. C. Love, K. McRae, & V. M. Sloutsky (Eds.), University of Pittsburgh Press.
Proceedings of the 30th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Simonton, D. K. (1995). Foresight in insight? A Darwinian answer. In
Society (pp. 12511256). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society. R. J. Sternberg & J. E. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of insight.
Jung-Beeman, M., Bowden, E. M., Haberman, J., Frymiare, J. L., Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Arambel-Liu, S., Greenblatt, R., . . . Kounios, J. (2004). Neural Sio, U. N., & Ormerod, T. C. (2009). Does incubation enhance problem
activity when people solve verbal problems with insight. PLoS solving? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 94120.
Biology, 2, e97. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0020097 Snyder, A., Mitchell, J., Ellwood, S., Yates, A., & Pallier, G. (2004).
Newell, B. R., Wong, K. Y., Cheung, J. C. H., & Rakow, T. (2009). Nonconscious idea generation. Psychological Reports, 94, 13251330.
Think, blink or sleep on it? The impact of modes of thought on Wallas, G. (1926). The art of thought. New York, NY: Harcourt Brace.
complex decision making. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Weisberg, R. W. (2006). Creativity: Understanding innovation in
Psychology, 62, 707732. problem solving, science, invention, and the arts. New York,
Nordgren, L. F., Bos, M. W., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2011). The best of both NY: Wiley.
worlds: Integrating conscious and unconscious thought best solves Zhong, C.-B., Dijksterhuis, A., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). The merits
complex decisions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, of unconscious thought in creativity. Psychological Science, 19,
509511. 912918.

47
Social Cognition, Vol. 29, No. 6, 2011, pp. 683698
McMahon et al.
Driven to Distraction

DRIVEN TO DISTRACTION:
THE IMPACT OF DISTRACTER TYPE ON
UNCONSCIOUS DECISION MAKING
Kibby McMahon, Betsy Sparrow, Ljubica Chatman, and Travis Riddle
Columbia University

While there is a variety of wonderful ways to take a break from work, is


there a type of distraction that is actually more productive? Studies on the
deliberation-without-attention effect (Dijksterhuis, 2004; Dijksterhuis, Bos,
Nordgren, & Van Baaren, 2006) show that a period of distraction while
making complex decisions can actually lead to better decisions than a pe-
riod of conscious deliberation. Although there are a number of activities
that will distract participants from decision-making tasks, we investigated
if certain types of distracter tasks are better for demonstrating the deliber-
ation-without-attention effect. Since most people realistically take a break
with undemanding forms of activity, we hypothesized that undemanding
distracter tasks will yield the best results. In Experiment 1, participants
were given a choice between four different cars but before they made their
decision they instructed (1) to think consciously about the choices, (2) lis-
ten to their own music with a portable digital music player, (3) solve an
anagram puzzle, (4) solve a word search puzzle, or (5) just make a decision
immediately. In Experiment 2, participants were given a choice among four
different applicants for a hypothetical graduate school program and were
instructed (1) to think consciously about the applicants, (2) to engage in a
listening task with music, or (3) to just listen to the music. As predicted, the
participants who were distracted with the easier tasks (listening to music
and word search puzzles) made the best decision significantly more of-
ten than conscious thinkers and even outperformed participants distracted
with more difficult tasks.

Most of us have experienced mulling over a difficult decision or problem that ap-
peared impossible to solve when the answer just came to us only after putting
the matter aside for a while. Taking a break from tasks like complex decisions or
insight problems has shown to be actually productive for solving them (Beeftink,

Address correspondence to Kibby McMahon, 406 Schermerhorn Hall, 1190 Amsterdam Avenue
MC 5501, New York, NY 10027. Email: kibbymcmahon@gmail.com

2011 Guilford Publications, Inc.

683

48
684 MCMAHON ET AL.

Van Eerde, & Rutte, 2008; Dijksterhuis & Nordgren, 2006; Paulus, Nakui, Putman,
& Brown, 2006; Smith, 1995; Wallas, 1926), but are all forms of distraction created
equal? When you take a break for a few minutes, it feels very different to relax
with some music than to start busying yourself with something else on your to-do
list. In this article we examine types of distracters and their impact on decision
making.
Dijksterhuis and colleagues pioneered the work on what they call the deliber-
ation-without-attention effect (Dijksterhuis, 2004; Dijksterhuis, Bos, Nordgren, &
Van Baaren, 2006), which leads to improved decision making for complex deci-
sions. Specifically, they have shown that when participants are distracted from a
decision-making task involving a lot of pertinent information, they are more likely
to make the best decision than if they thought about it consciously. The common
paradigm Dijksterhuis et al. use consists of a pre-acquisition phase, followed by an
acquisition phase, an incubation period, and then the actual decision. In the pre-
acquisition phase, subjects are informed that they will read over the descriptions
of four different items and will have to choose the best one later in the study. After
reading over the descriptions of four different items, participants are assigned to a
conscious thought or an unconscious thought condition. Participants assigned
to the conscious thought condition are told to carefully consider the options before
making the choice. Those in the unconscious thought condition engage in an un-
related task before making their choice. Once the incubation period is finished, all
subjects indicate which item they think is best out of all four. Results consistently
show that participants in the unconscious thought condition make decisions or
judgments that favor the best choice more often than participants in the conscious
condition.
While there is evidence for the benefits of distraction, the quality of distraction
must be taken into account. Requiring participants to engage in another difficult
task in addition to the decision-making task adds cognitive demands (Hart, 1986;
Norman & Bobrow, 1975), which can lead to information overload that actually
hinders processing (Chewning & Harrell, 1990; Huber, 1985; Speier, Valacich, &
Vessey, 1999). Within a review of attention allocation and task demands, Kahne-
man (1973) mentions that when attending to different tasks, the type of one task
can influence the performance on the other. Working on two tasks can create at-
tentional conflict, which can be either structural interference if the content of the two
tasks are similar, or capacity interference if the task difficulties compete for cognitive
resources. The Distraction-Conflict Theory (Baron, 1986) argues that if a distract-
ing task is difficult enough to create such an attentional conflict, the arousal and
information overload will inhibit performance on complex tasks. Thus the type
and difficulty of a distracting task would have an influence on the performance
of a concurrent decision-making task. These theories do not directly address the
issue of a separate unconscious decision-making process. However, it has been
argued that conscious and unconscious processes are not completely independent
systems but instead have an interactive relationship (Evans, 2006; Kahneman &
Fredrick, 2002; Reber, 1993). Devoting conscious resources to an unrelated task al-
lows unconscious thought to process a different task, but it might not be so simple
to claim that all forms of distraction yield the same results; what we devote our
attention to might influence how the unconscious processes perform.
To induce unconscious thought, Dijksterhuis and colleagues (Dijksterhuis, 2004;
Dijksterhuis et al., 2006) have used a myriad of distracter tasks such as word search,

49
DRIVEN TO DISTRACTION 685

anagrams, and n-back tasks. All of these tasks seem to accomplish the goal of dis-
tracting participants from the decision-making task enough to let unconscious
thought processing arrive at the best decision. There has not been any report that
the differences in the tasks have an impact on the deliberation-without-attention
effect even though they require slightly different cognitive processes to solve. To
solve a word search puzzle, one must hunt through a matrix of letters to find a
target word. Priming the category of a target word-withina-word search has no
significant impact on performance, suggesting that word search puzzles are visual
perceptual tasks that do not require any semantic activation of the target word
(Karlin & Bower, 1976). However, anagram puzzles consist of a list of scrambled
words that participants must identify and spell correctly (i.e., the correct answer to
lisa is sail). There is some evidence that solving anagram puzzles activates a
deeper level of processing (Rajaram & Roediger, 1993; Srinivas & Roediger, 1990).
These two puzzles can both successfully occupy conscious resources, but it is un-
clear if the difference in their cognitive demands would influence the concurrent
unconscious processes.
In addition to a lack of research into the previously used distracter tasks, there
has not been any work demonstrating that the deliberation-without-attention ef-
fect will hold for more ecologically valid distracters. One of the most ubiquitous
distracters in daily life are portable music players. As twentieth-century English
conductor Sir Thomas Beecham astutely claimed, The function of music is to re-
lease us from the tyranny of conscious thought. Rauscher, Shaw, and Ky (1993)
came across an interesting finding in their studies that suggested classical music
(specifically Mozart pieces) temporarily enhance spatial reasoning performance.
This was dubbed the Mozart Effect and sparked new interest in how listening
to music can improve the performance of various cognitive processes (Fairfield &
Cornoldi, 2007; Lesiuk & Teresa, 2010; Mammarella, Fairfield, & Cornoldi, 2007;
Rauscher et al., 1993; Rideout, Dougherty, & Wernert, 1998). An interesting pilot
study by Caldwell and Riby (2007) explored how listening to a subjects preferred
music genre affects the processing of novel information. Rock and classical musi-
cians listened to either classical or rock music while completing an oddball task, an
attention exercise in which subjects have to pick out when a new stimulus appears
on a computer screen. Their event-related potentials suggested that conscious pro-
cesses like working memory were not as strongly deployed to the oddball task if
the subject listened to their preferred music genre. If listening to music has such
a notable influence on cognitive abilities and attention, it is worth exploring how
well it can apply to an unconscious decision-making paradigm, especially since it
is such a popular form of distraction.
In these experiments, we compared the efficacies of the different distracter tasks
to see if the type or difficulty of the distraction has any influence on unconscious
decision making. We predicted that we would replicate Dijksterhuis results in
the overall comparison of conscious thought and unconscious thought conditions
for difficult decisions. Furthermore, we examined the nuances of different uncon-
scious thought conditions in Experiment 1. Taking into account the previously
mentioned literature on task difficulty and attentional conflict, we predicted that
undemanding tasks such as listening to music and solving word search puzzles
would lead to higher performance on the complex decision-making task than the
more challenging anagram puzzles. In Experiment 2, we attempted to replicate the
deliberation-without-without attention effect with music, except within a social

50
686 MCMAHON ET AL.

decision-making task. We also explored the influence of different levels of diffi-


culty, or cognitive load, within the same type of distracter task. We predicted that
an easier and less challenging distracter task would lead to better unconscious
decision-making performance.

EXPERIMENT 1

METHOD

Participants and Design. Seventy-four Columbia University students were re-


cruited through the introductory psychology courses or through flyers posted
around campus and compensated with course credit or $5. All participants were
randomly assigned one of five conditions: an immediate decision condition (n =
15), a conscious thought condition (n = 15), a word search condition (n = 14), a
portable music or iPod condition (n = 15), and an anagram condition (n = 15). The
latter three conditions all fall under the unconscious thought category, but use dif-
ferent types of distraction.
Distracter Task Difficulty. Subjective ratings of cognitive load and working mem-
ory tasks have been used as a reliable measure of difficulty (Bratfisch, Borg, & Dor-
nic, 1972; Paas, Van Merrinboer & Adam, 1994). The distracter tasks were judged
for their relative difficulty with 17 separate participants from the same pool of
Columbia University students. The word search, anagram, and iPod tasks were
completed by each participant in random order and then rated for level of diffi-
culty on a scale of 1 (not difficult) to 7 (very difficult). A repeated measures ANOVA
determined a significant difference in difficulty among the distracter tasks, F(2,17)
= 42.08, p < .01. Post hoc pair-wise comparisons using Bonferroni correction re-
vealed that anagram puzzles (M = 5.18, SE = 1.43) were rated as significantly more
difficult than the word search task (M = 3.94, SE = 1.64) at p< .04 and the iPod-lis-
tening task (M = 1.35, SE = .86) at p < .01. The word search task was also considered
significantly more difficult than the iPod-listening at p < .001

PROCEDURE

Participants were seated at a lab computer that used the experimental program-
ming software Medialab (Jarvis, 2004). All participants were informed that they
would be presented with descriptions of four different cars and would have to
choose the car that is the best overall. As in Dijksterhuis (2004) study 2, partici-
pants were then presented with the description of the cars for 10 seconds each.
Each car had the same 12 attributes: handling, gas mileage, impact on the environ-
ment, legroom, quality of sound system, quality of service, difficulty to shift gears,
trunk size, age of car, choice in color, sunroof, and cup holders. These attributes
could either be positive (e.g.,The Hatsdun has good gas mileage) or negative
(e.g., The Nabusi has bad gas mileage). The best car, the Hatsdun, had 75% posi-
tive attributes, the medium cars had 50% positive attributes, and the worst car had
25% positive attributes. Even though this proportion of positive and negative at-
tributes makes the Hatsdun the overall best choice, we took into account the pos-
sibility that some car attributes are more important than others. The distribution of

51
DRIVEN TO DISTRACTION 687

TABLE 1. The Valence of the Attributes for Each Car


The Hatsdun The Nabusi The Kaiwa The Dasuka
Gas Mileage + - - -
Handling - - - +
Environment + + + -
Sound System + - - +
Service + - + -
Shifting Gears + - + +
Trunk Size + - + -
Legroom - + + +
New/Old + + - -
Colors + - + -
Sunroof - - - +
Cup Holders + - - +

positive and negative attributes of each car was based on Rey, Goldstein, and Per-
ruchets (2009) attribute weighting. His survey results provide a mean value of in-
fluence for each type attribute according to how important it is. When an attribute
is positive (e.g., The Hatsdun has good mileage) the mean value of influence
score is positive, and when the attribute is negative (e.g., The Nabusi has poor
handling) the mean value of influence is negative. A mean evaluation score for
each car was calculated from averaging the values of all the attributes. The higher
the mean evaluation score, the better the car is. Based on Reys attribute values,
our version of the Hatsdun had a mean evaluation score of 5.9, the Kaiwa had a
score of .5, the Dasuka had a score of -1.1, and the Nabusi had the lowest score of
-5.4. The positive and negative attribute distribution is displayed in Table 1.
After participants read the descriptions of the four cars, they were assigned to
the immediate, the conscious thought, the iPod, the anagram, or the word search
condition. In the immediate condition, participants chose the best car right after
they read the four descriptions. In the conscious thought condition, participants
were told to spend 3 minutes carefully considering the four different cars before
making their decision. The iPod, anagram, and word search conditions are all dif-
ferent forms of unconscious thought conditions. For the iPod condition, they were
told to listen to any song they liked on their portable music player with head-
phones for 3 minutes (all participants were asked to bring in a portable music
player in case they were randomized to this condition). In the word search and
anagram conditions, participants spent 3 minutes solving the assigned puzzle.
Once the 3-minute conscious thinking or distraction period was over, partici-
pants indicated which car they thought was the best overall out of all four in mul-
tiple choice format. They were then debriefed, compensated, and dismissed.

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

The percentages of participants in each condition who chose the best car, the Hats-
dun, are displayed in Figure 1. We used a generalized linear model to analyze
this binary data. As we expected, we found an overall significant main effect of
thought, Wald chi-square = 13.29, df = 4, p < .02, indicating that some types of

52
688 MCMAHON ET AL.

FIGURE 1. Percentages shown are based on the proportion of subjects who chose the Hatsdun,
the best choice, to the total number of subjects in each condition.

thought condition performed better than others. Pair-wise comparisons revealed


that the word search condition significantly outperformed the conscious thought
condition (M = -.47, SE = .161, Z score= -2.9, df = 1, p < .01), as did the iPod con-
dition (M = -.45, SE = .166, Z score = -2.7, df = 1, p < .01). The participants in the
anagram condition did poorly and did not differ significantly from the conscious
condition (M = .07, SE = .154, Z score = .45, df =1, p =.67).
We also noted that participants in the immediate choice condition did signifi-
cantly worse than the word search condition (M = -.33, SE = .170, Z score = -1.9, df
= 1, p < .05), and marginally worse than the iPod condition (M = -.31, SE = .175, Z
score = -1.77, df = 1, p = .07). The immediate choice condition did not significantly
differ from the conscious thought (M = .13, SE = .170, Z score = .76, df = 1, p = .43) or
the anagram condition (M = .20, SE = .163, Z score = 1.23, df = 1, p = .22). This com-
parison provides additional evidence for the claim that the improved performance
of unconscious thought conditions is not due to using an on-line judgment made
during the acquisition phase. Since the immediate condition performance lies in
between the performances of unconscious and conscious thought, we are lead to
believe that it demonstrates the relative effectiveness of each kind of thought.
These results suggest that listening to music is an effective type of distraction
that can lead to improved decision making. Both the iPod and word search con-
ditions allowed subjects to take a break from the decision-making task with an
activity stimulating enough to capture their attention. We were surprised at how
poorly the anagram condition performed considering its success in previous work
(e.g., Dijksterhuis, 2004). While running the experiment, we noticed a difference
in students reactions to the distracter tasks. People in the word search and iPod
conditions looked engaged in their tasks and nothing seemed out of the ordinary.
After the incubation period in the anagram condition, however, most of the par-
ticipants appeared flustered and discouraged. Some of them hesitantly handed in
their anagram sheets, apologizing for not doing that well.
An interesting finding emerged when we explored the type of errors that con-
scious thinkers were making instead of choosing the Hatsdun. Further analyses
revealed that those in the underperforming conditions tended to select the first

53
DRIVEN TO DISTRACTION 689

FIGURE 2. Percentage of participants who chose the Kaiwa, the first car, out of all the
participants in each thought condition.

car presented, the Kaiwa. Percentages of car one (the Kaiwa) choices are shown in
Figure 2. The generalized linear model reveals no overall significance of thought
condition, Wald chi-square = 3.78, df =4, p = .44, but pair-wise comparisons reveal
that the conscious (M = -.40, SE = .126, Z score = -3.17, df = 1, p < .01), immediate
(M= -.33, SE = .122, Z score = 2.7, df = 1, p < .01), and anagram (M= -.33, SE = .122, Z
score = 2.7, df = 1, p < .01) conditions chose Kaiwa significantly more than the iPod
condition. The conscious condition also chose Kaiwa significantly more than the
word search condition (M= -.33, SE = .142, Z score = 2.32, df = 1, p < .01).
The Kaiwa does not have anything that would make it stand out from the other
cars, especially since it has negative attributes within the most important charac-
teristics. The only aspect worth noting of the Kaiwa is that it is the first car pre-
sented in the acquisition phase. Its popularity may be attributed to the primacy
effect, a serial order effect that biases judgment toward the first item presented in
a sequence (Deese & Kaufman, 1957). Heuristics reduce complex decision-making
and reasoning tasks into fast, automatic judgments based on previous experience
(Tversky & Kahneman, 1974), and streamline complex judgment and reasoning
processes by making use of a fewer, more select number of available cues (Shah &
Oppenheimer, 2008). In this way, deliberative conscious thought could exacerbate
the tendency to give some attributes too much weight within the decision, as Di-
jksterhuis and Nordgren (2006) describe in the unconscious thought theory. In Ex-
periment 2, we explored these heuristic-consistent errors as a secondary concern.

EXPERIMENT 2

The second experiment further replicates the deliberation-without-attention ef-


fect with music, but within a social decision-making task. Listening to portable
music proved to be an effective distracter that is not only ecologically valid, but
also leaves flexibility for different engagement and attention manipulations. We
compared conscious thought to both high and low cognitive load music listening
conditions to see if the levels of difficulty of the distracter task had any influence

54
690 MCMAHON ET AL.

on the deliberation-without-attention effect. In addition, we also investigated if


there were any consistent mistakes within incorrect responses.

METHOD

Participants and Design. Seventy-two Columbia University students participated


in this experiment for course credit or $5. All participants were taken from the
Columbia undergraduate population to increase the influence of in-group bias for
other Columbia students. Participants were randomly assigned to one of three
conditions: conscious thought condition (n = 24), easy iPod condition (n = 24), and
difficult iPod condition (n = 24).
Distracter Task Difficulty. As in Experiment 1, these two iPod (or unconscious
thought) tasks were rated on a 7-point difficulty scale by the same participants
who rated the other distracters, plus an additional Columbia University student
(for a total of 18 raters). A repeated measures ANOVA determined a significant
difference between among the tasks, F(1, 18) = 29.97, p < 0.01 (Mdifficult = 3.72, SE
= 1.87; Measy = 1.33, SE = .08). Subjects found that an added listening exercise in
the hard iPod condition provided a more difficult task than the condition in which
they could listen to music with no other demands in the easy iPod condition.
Procedure. Participants were seated at a computer that used the experimental
programming software Medialab (Jarvis, 2004). Participants were told that they
are in charge of admissions to a hypothetical graduate school that judges appli-
cants based on 12 attributes: Graduate Record Examination (GRE) percentile, un-
dergraduate university, grade point average (GPA), quality of recommendations,
prestige of the recommenders, interest in your particular institution, ability to meet
deadlines, honors, relevance of undergraduate major, ability to contribute ideas to
the program, email address, and amount of work experience. Participants were
then given full descriptions for four different applicants with all the attributes of
each applicant presented on the screen for 14 seconds each in randomized order.
Much like in the car context, these attributes could either be positive (e.g., Ra-
chel is good at meeting deadlines) or negative (e.g., Victoria is bad at meeting
deadlines). Rachel, the best applicant, had 75% positive attributes, Victoria was
considered the worst with 25% positive attributes, and the two medium applicants
both had 50% positive attributes. We based the distribution and relative attribute
weighting on the results of pretesting. These initial rankings were confirmed by
ranking scores the participants gave to each attribute (see Table 2).
Once the four applicant descriptions were presented, participants were random-
ly assigned to one of three conditions. In the conscious thought condition, they
were told to spend the following 3 minutes carefully considering the applicants.
The easy iPod condition was a kind of low load task in which the participants were
distracted with no performance or cognitive demands. The instructions they were
given were: you will take a break of three minutes. Please choose a song with
lyrics on your iPod by any band or artist you like. In the difficult iPod condition,
we wanted to use the same type of distraction except with an added motivation
to increase attention to the music. The participants were told, You will spend the
next three minutes listening carefully to a song with lyrics on your iPod by any
band or artist you like. First, write down the name of the song in the box provided

55
DRIVEN TO DISTRACTION 691

TABLE 2. The Valence of the Attributes for Each Applicant


Rachel Victoria James Peter
Grade Point Average + - - +
Recommendations + - + -
University - + + -
Graduate Record Examination - + + -
Work Experience + - - +
Ideas + + + -
Interest in Program - - + +
Honors + - - +
Prestige of Recommenders + - - +
Deadlines + - + -
Major + - - -
Email Address + - - +

and as you listen, remember and write down every word that begins or ends with
the letter T. Please list as many words as you can since your performance will be
scored.
After the 3-minute incubation period, participants were shown the applicant
descriptions for 1 second each as a quick review to prevent the participants from
forgetting which applicant was which. Then they had to choose the overall best
applicant in a randomized multiple-choice format. Afterward they filled out an
affect questionnaire and rated on a 7-point scale the levels of enthusiasm, stress,
interest, boredom, and alertness they felt during the experiment. They then ranked
the 12 application attributes in descending order of relative importance. Partici-
pants were then debriefed, compensated, and dismissed.

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

For each condition, the percentages of participants who chose the best applicant
(Rachel) are displayed in Figure 3. We used a generalized linear model to assess
the probability of selecting Rachel as a function of the thought condition partici-
pants were randomly assigned to. We found a marginally significant main effect
of thought, Wald chi-square = 5.521, df = 2, p = .063, but pair-wise comparisons
revealed that the easy iPod condition performed significantly better than the con-
scious thought condition (M = -.33, SE = .136, Z score = -2.43, df =1, p < .01). The
difficult iPod condition marginally outperformed the conscious condition (M =
-.25, SE = .138, Z score = -1.81, df = 1, p = .06). As we hypothesized, both kinds of
distraction, easy and difficult, outperformed the conscious thought condition, but
only the easy iPod condition performed well enough to yield a significant differ-
ence. A comparison between the two iPod conditions did not yield a significant
difference (M = .08, SE = .142, Z score = .56, df = 1, p = .56). When participants take
a break with their own choice of music, their decisions reflect the performance of
unconscious thought when making complex decisions. This suggests that the de-
liberation-without-attention effect could be replicated in more ecologically valid

56
692 MCMAHON ET AL.

FIGURE 3. Percentage of participants who chose Rachel, the best applicant, of all the
participants in each thought condition.

methods of distraction. Although participants in both iPod conditions made better


decisions than conscious thinkers, a demand for more concentration on the dis-
tracter task in the hard iPod condition impaired the unconscious decision making.
The effect was not as strong as with the anagram condition in the previous experi-
ment, but the difficulty ratings suggest that the listening task was not as challeng-
ing as an anagram puzzle. However, as in Experiment 1, the participants who were
distracted with the easier task made the best decision most often.
Taking into consideration the pattern of incorrect decisions in Experiment 1, we
looked at which erroneous choice was the most popular in Experiment 2 and if
there was any implication of heuristic use. James was the less qualified applicant
and was only distinguished by his status as an alumnus of Columbia University.
Since all of the participants were members of the Columbia University community,
it can be argued that the decision making of the conscious thinkers were biased by
the in-group bias; that is, the tendency to favor members within ones own social
categories (Tajfel, 1970). An analysis of the number of people who chose James
as a function of type of thought condition revealed that there was a marginally
significant overall main effect of thought, Wald chi-square = 5.388, df = 2, p = .07,
but participants in the conscious thought condition chose James significantly more
often than those in the easy iPod condition (M = .33, SE = .131, Z score = 2.52, df =
1, p < .01). The percentages of participants who chose James and the percentages
of the participants who chose either of the other two incorrect responses within
each thought condition are displayed in Figure 4. These results are hard to inter-
pret considering the three conditions had unequal numbers of participants who
made an incorrect decision. However, within the incorrect decisions, there was a
trend toward choosing James more prominently in the conscious and difficult iPod
conditions. More exploration on heuristics and the deliberation-without-attention
effect would be necessary to draw any definite conclusions.

57
DRIVEN TO DISTRACTION 693

FIGURE 4. Percentage of participants who chose James, the Columbia applicant, compared
to the percentage who chose either Peter or Victoria, of all participants in each thought
condition.

AFFECT AND ATTRIBUTE RATINGS

The post-study questionnaires measured explicit reports of emotion and value


placed on the respective 12 applicant attributes. We ran one-way ANOVA analy-
ses to compare the effect of thought condition on each affect rating and only the
Alertness rating was significant, F (2)= 4.94, p < .02. A pair-wise comparison re-
vealed that the mean rating of Alertness in the difficult iPod condition (M = 1.75,
SE = .193) was significantly lower than in the conscious thought condition (M =
2.54, SE = .193). The participants in the easy iPod condition lay in between (M =
2.17, SE = .193) the two other conditions, feeling slightly less alert than conscious
thinkers but more than the difficult iPod participants. Although this is only based
on self-report, these results suggest that alertness during the experiment does not
definitively account for the differences in decision-making performance among
the conditions. It is possible that too much or too little attentiveness can impair
decision making, but the effect is too small to be determined as the mechanism of
deliberation-without-attention effect.
Each participant ranked each of the 12 attributes from 1 (most important) to 12
(least important) based on how influential the attributes were for assessing the
overall quality of the applicants. The means for university ratings are displayed
in Table 3; the lower the mean ranking score, the more important the attribute is
considered to be within an application. There was no significant effect of thought
condition on any of the rankings, suggesting that there are no major differences
in the explicit decision-making strategies the participants used to compare the ap-
plicants. However, we used these mean ranking scores to calculate the individual
applicants mean evaluation score. Rachel had a mean evaluation score of 2.84,
Victoria had a score of -2.74, Peter had a score of 1.01, and James had a score of .21.
Therefore, Rachel was confirmed as the best choice even taking into account that
some attributes of an application are considered more important than others.

58
694 MCMAHON ET AL.

TABLE 3. The Mean Ranking Scores of Each Application Attribute


Attribute Mean Standard Deviation
Grade Point Average 3.06 2.28
Recommendations 4.65 2.72
University 5.24 3.37
Graduate Record Examination 5.24 2.83
Work Experience 5.46 3.17
Ideas to Contribute 5.85 3.16
Interest in Program 6.44 3.08
Honors 7.01 2.19
Prestige of Recommenders 7.19 2.75
Ability to Meet Deadlines 7.46 2.82
Relevance of Major 8.75 2.73
Email Address 11.56 1.63

GENERAL DISCUSSION

We were interested in refining the idea of a distracter task within a deliberation-


without-attention context and determining which distractions are the most useful
in making complex decisions unconsciously. The first experiment was intended to
compare different types of distracters, especially to see if a popular form of distrac-
tion, listening to portable music, can yield the same unconscious thought perfor-
mance as other known tasks. The results of the experiment suggest that distracters
such as word searches and iPods lead to making better complex decisions than
thinking consciously about the decision. However, it was surprising how poorly
people in the anagram condition performed, as if the pressure the subjects felt to
do the anagram was enough to dampen unconscious thoughts abilities. We iso-
lated this factor of task difficulty in the second experiment by providing different
levels of difficulty to the participants while keeping the distracter task constant.
As we predicted, all the subjects who were distracted with music performed well
on the decision-making task, but only the subjects who listened to music with no
added cognitive demands made the best choice significantly more than conscious
thinkers. The type or level of difficulty that drives these effects is still unclear,
but the consistent high performance of participants with easy tasks suggests that
distractions with the least amount of cognitive demands are the optimal choice for
the deliberation-without-attention effect. Further work would have to determine
what type of distracter or the threshold of distracter difficulty would completely
obstruct unconscious processing.
If you recall a time when you took a break from a difficult situation, work as-
signment, or decision, most likely you engaged yourself in your favorite mindless
form of procrastination. In these studies, all the unconscious thought conditions
included an incubation period, but only the easy tasks of word searches and pas-
sive music listening really simulated how people take a real break. The feeling of
relaxation and low vigilance during these easy tasks could account for the high
decision-making performance. On the other hand, Yerkes & Dodson (1908) and
Zajoncs (1965) work on social facilitation both provide evidence that arousal over
a certain threshold can impede performance on challenging tasks. Therefore, the

59
DRIVEN TO DISTRACTION 695

added pressure that the anagram and difficult iPod conditions could have elevat-
ed subjects to a level of arousal that impaired their performance on the difficult
decision-making task. Conscious thought also has that added pressure of concen-
trating on the decision-making task, which would further impede performance.
The issue of affect in general has been raised as a confounding factor since people
listening to music may feel happier during the experiment. There is some evidence
that positive affect can improve performances on cognitive tasks such a decision
making and creative problem solving (Isen, Daubman, & Nowicki, 1987; Isen &
Means, 1983). To check for this, we had subjects report their levels of enthusiasm,
alertness, boredom, stress, and interest during the experiment. None of the mea-
sures showed a significant effect of positive affect among the thought conditions,
so we ruled this out as a possibility. The participants subjective ratings of alert-
ness do not support the argument that high arousal or attentiveness leads to lower
performance. Perhaps physiological measures can more clearly point to an effect
of arousal, but our lack of evidence leads us to believe that our data is less affected
by arousal and more a result of the different cognitive demands.
An exploratory look into the incorrect decisions revealed a trend toward the
heuristic-consistent choice. The Kaiwa car and James were choices that were oper-
ationally not the best option, but were appealing to those making decisions based
on shortcuts like the order of item presentation or the preference toward members
of your own social group. There is evidence that when people feel pressured to
complete a task and if the task is difficult, their attention resources are depleted
and they default to biased responses (Chaiken & Maheswaran, 1994; Kunda, 1990).
A series of studies by Pelham and Neter (1995) found an interaction with task dif-
ficulty and motivation on making heuristic-consistent errors. When subjects were
motivated to answer difficult versions of decision-making tasks, they were more
likely to rely on the heuristics and make inaccurate judgments than if they were
unmotivated within easy versions of the task. Although the heuristics data from
our studies are far from conclusive, it is interesting to note that the incorrect deci-
sions of the participants in the difficult distracter and conscious conditions tend to
be biased, a typical result of restricted attentional resources. Thinking consciously
about a complex decision or engaging in difficult distracters may tax attention
resources enough to reduce decision-making accuracy. These conditions lead to in-
formation overload, which negatively impacts task performance according to the
aforementioned capacity interference (Kahneman, 1973) and Distraction-Conflict
Theory (Baron, 1986). It questions the perspective that distractions activate only
unconscious thought and deliberation activates only conscious thought. Instead,
there is enough overlap within the two systems that the difficulty of the task dur-
ing incubation periods can influence the concurrent decision-making process.
Growing evidence for implicit processes within the field of working memory
further supports the interaction of unconscious and conscious processes. Given
that conscious attention is so limited, current working memory models claim that
consciousness is useful for processing stimuli input and activating the unconscious
networks to process this information (Baars, 2002, 2003; Baars & Ave, 1997). Recent
dual-process theories have proposed that we can make automatic judgments and
decisions, but conscious thought can monitor and modify their effect (Chaiken,
Liberman, & Eagly, 1989; Evans, 2006). These theories support the idea that ex-
plicit and implicit processes often work together instead of exist as two distinct
systems. The successful unconscious thought paradigms first require subjects to

60
696 MCMAHON ET AL.

attend to the necessary information on the four items before distraction can allow
for the unconscious working memory to process the decision. Attending to yet
another task might overwhelm the working memory and deplete both conscious
and unconscious resources. Our results suggest that the best way to facilitate the
deliberation-without-attention effect is to consciously attend to all of the available
information, then engage in a distracter task that is low in cognitive load and does
not require a lot of working memory or attention resources.

CONCLUSION AND IMPLICATIONS FOR FURTHER RESEARCH

Distracter tasks that are low in difficulty are shown to be the most successful in
demonstrating the deliberation-without-attention effect. Further replications of
this effect should take the quality of distracters into account, especially partici-
pants assessment of the tasks difficulty. Listening to portable music is an effec-
tive and accessible way to distract subjects without added pressure or too much
cognitive demand. Since it just provides auditory stimulation, it leaves room for
new unconscious manipulations that would need to present visual stimuli. For
example, subjects can listen to music while new applicant information is present-
ed or subliminally primed on the screen. Further studies can also more clearly
determine the mechanism for what makes the more difficult distracters like ana-
grams impede unconscious decision making. The performance of the hard iPod
participants showed that a more difficult distracter task can reduce unconscious
thoughts efficacy, but perhaps a task that requires a deeper level of processing
(complex memory tasks or other puzzles that require associative thinking) or adds
a higher level of cognitive load would eliminate unconscious thoughts advan-
tages in complex decision making.

REFERENCES

Baars, B. J. (2002). The conscious access hy- need a break right now? Creativity Re-
pothesis: origins and recent evidence. search Journal, 20, 358-364.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(1), 4752. Bratfisch, O., Borg, G., & Dornic, S. (1972).
Baars, B. J. (2003). Working memory requires Perceived item-difficulty in three tests of
conscious processes, not vice versa: A intellectual performance capacity (Rep. No.
global workspace account. In N. Osaka 29). Stockholm: Institute of Applied Psy-
(Ed.), Neural basis of consciousness. Phila- chology.
delphia: John Benjamins. Caldwell, G. N., & Riby, L. M. (2007). The ef-
Baars, B. J., & Ave, D. (1997). Global workspace fects of music exposure and own genre
theory, a rigorous scientific theory of preference on conscious and uncon-
consciousness. Journal of Consciousness scious cognitive processes: A pilot ERP
Studies, 4, 292309. study. Consciousness and Cognition, 16,
Baron, R. S. (1986). Distraction-conflict theory: 992996.
Progress and problems. Advances in Ex- Chaiken, S., Liberman, A., & Eagly, A. H.
perimental Social Psychology, 19, 139. (1989). Heuristic and systematic infor-
Beeftink, F., Van Eerdea, W., & Rutteb, C.G. mation processing within and beyond
(2008). The effect of interruptions and the persuasion context. In J. S. Uleman
breaks on insight and impasses: Do you & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought

61
DRIVEN TO DISTRACTION 697

(Vol. 16, pp. 212252). New York: Guil- Gilovich, D. Griffin, & D. Kahneman
ford. (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psycholo-
Chaiken, S., & Maheswaran, D. (1994). Heuris- gy of intuitive judgment (pp. 4981). Cam-
tic processing can bias systematic pro- bridge: Cambridge University Press.
cessing: Effects of source credibility, ar- Karlin, M. B., & Bower, G. H. (1976). Semantic
gument ambiguity, and task importance category effects in visual word search.
on attitude judgment. Journal of Persona- Perception Psychophysics, 19, 417424.
lity and Social Psychology, 66, 460473. Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated
Chewning, E. G., Jr., & Harrell, A. M. (1990). reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 108,
The effect of information load on deci- 480498.
sion makers cue utilization levels and Lesiuk, T. (2010). The effect of preferred music
decision quality in a financial distress on mood and performance in a high-
decision task. Accounting, Organizations, cognitive demand occupation. Journal of
and Society, 15, 527542. Music Therapy, 47, 137154.
Deese, J., & Kaufman, R. A. (1957). Serial effects Mammarella, N., Fairfield, B., & Cornoldi, C.
in recall of unorganized and sequential- (2007). Does music enhance cognitive
ly organized verbal material. Journal of performance in healthy older adults?
Experimental Psychology, 54, 180187. The Vivaldi effect. Aging Clinical and Ex-
Dijksterhuis, A. (2004). Think different: The perimental Research, 19, 394399.
merits of unconscious thought in prefer- Norman, D. A., & Bobrow, D. G. (1975). On
ence development and decision making. data-limited and resource-limited pro-
Journal of Personality and Social Psycholo- cesses. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 44-64.
gy, 87, 586598. Paas, F., Van Merrinboer, J.J.G., & Adam, J. J.
Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M. W., Nordgren, L. F., (1994). Measurement of cognitive-load
& Van Baaren, R. B. (2006). On making in instructional research. Perceptual and
the right choice: The deliberation-with- Motor Skills, 79, 419430.
out-attention effect. Science, 311(5763), Paulus, P. B., Nakui, T., Putman, V. L., & Brown,
10051007. V. R. (2006). Effects of task instructions
Dijksterhuis, A., & Nordgren, L. F. (2006). A and brief breaks on brainstorming.
theory of unconscious thought. Perspec- Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and
tives on Psychological Science, 1, 95109. Practice, 10, 206219.
Evans, J. S. (2006). The heuristic-analytic theo- Pelham, B. W., & Neter, E. (1995). The effect
ry of reasoning: Extension and evalua- of motivation of judgment depends on
tion. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, the difficulty of the judgment. Journal
378395. of Personality and Social Psychology, 68,
Huber, V. L. (1985). Effects of task difficulty, 581594.
goal setting, and strategy on perfor- Rajaram, S., & Roediger III, H. L. (1993). Direct
mance of a heuristic task. Journal of Ap- comparison of four implicit memory
plied Psychology, 70, 492504. tests. Learning, Memory, and Cognition,
Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Nowicki, G. P. 19, 765776.
(1987). Positive affect facilitates creative Rauscher, F. H., Shaw, G. L., & Ky, K. N. (1993).
problem solving. Journal of Personality Music and spatial task performance. Na-
and Social Psychology, 52, 11221131. ture, 365(6447), 611.
Isen, A. M., & Means, B. (1983). The influence Reber, A. S. (1993). Implicit learning and tacit
of positive affect on decision-making knowledge. Oxford: Oxford University
strategy. Social Cognition, 2, 1831. Press.
Jarvis, B.G. (2004). MediaLab Research Soft- Rey, A., Goldstein, R. M., & Perruchet, P. (2009).
ware, Version 2004 [Computer Pro- Does unconscious thought improve
gram]. New York: Empirisoft. (www. complex decision making? Psychological
empirisoft.com) Research, 73, 372379.
Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. En- Rideout, B.E., Dougherty, S., & Wernert, L.
glewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. (1998). Effect of music on spatial per-
Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S. (2002). Repre- formance: A test of generality. Perceptual
sentativeness revisited: Attribute sub- Motor Skills, 86, 512514.
stitution in intuitive judgement. In T.

62
698 MCMAHON ET AL.

Shah, A. K., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2008). Tajfel, H. (1970). Experiments in intergroup


Heuristics made easy: An effort-reduc- discrimination. Scientific American, 223,
tion framework. Psychological Bulletin, 96102.
134, 207222. Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment
Smith, S. M. (1995). Fixation, incubation, and under uncertainty: Heuristics and bias-
insight in memory and creative think- es. Science, 185(4157), 11241131.
ing. In S. M. Smith, T. B. Ward, & R. A. Van Der Weiden, R.M.F. (2001). The Mozart ef-
Finke (Eds.), The creative cognition ap- fect. Journal of the Royal Society of Medici-
proach (pp. 135156). Cambridge, MA: ne, 94, 553.
The MIT Press. Wallas, G. (1926). The art of thought. New York:
Speier, C., Valacich, J. S., & Vessey, I. (1999). Harcourt.
The influence of task interruption on Yerkes, R. M., & Dodson, J. D. (1908). The re-
individual decision making: An infor- lation of strength of stimulus to rapid-
mation overload perspective. Decision ity of habit-formation. Journal of Com-
Sciences, 30, 337360. parative Neurology and Psychology, 18,
Srinivas, K., & Roediger, H. L. (1990). Classify- 459-482.
ing implicit memory tests: Category as- Zajonc, R. B. (1965). Social facilitation. Science,
sociation and anagram solution. Journal 149, 269-274.
of Memory and Language, 29, 389412.

63
Psychological Science http://pss.sagepub.com/

Inspired by Distraction : Mind Wandering Facilitates Creative Incubation


Benjamin Baird, Jonathan Smallwood, Michael D. Mrazek, Julia W. Y. Kam, Michael S. Franklin and Jonathan W. Schooler
Psychological Science published online 31 August 2012
DOI: 10.1177/0956797612446024

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://pss.sagepub.com/content/early/2012/08/31/0956797612446024

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

On behalf of:

Association for Psychological Science

Additional services and information for Psychological Science can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://pss.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

Subscriptions: http://pss.sagepub.com/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

>> OnlineFirst Version of Record - Aug 31, 2012

What is This?

Downloaded from pss.sagepub.com at UNIV CALIFORNIA SANTA BARBARA on September 26, 2012

64
Psychological Science OnlineFirst, published on August 31, 2012 as doi:10.1177/0956797612446024

Research Report
Psychological Science

Inspired by Distraction: Mind Wandering XX(X) 16


The Author(s) 2012
Reprints and permission:
Facilitates Creative Incubation sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0956797612446024
http://pss.sagepub.com

Benjamin Baird1, Jonathan Smallwood2 , Michael D. Mrazek1,


Julia W. Y. Kam3, Michael S. Franklin1, and
Jonathan W. Schooler1
1
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara; 2Department
for Social Neuroscience, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany;
and 3Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia

Abstract
Although anecdotes that creative thoughts often arise when one is engaged in an unrelated train of thought date back
thousands of years, empirical research has not yet investigated this potentially critical source of inspiration. We used an
incubation paradigm to assess whether performance on validated creativity problems (the Unusual Uses Task, or UUT) can
be facilitated by engaging in either a demanding task or an undemanding task that maximizes mind wandering. Compared
with engaging in a demanding task, rest, or no break, engaging in an undemanding task during an incubation period led
to substantial improvements in performance on previously encountered problems. Critically, the context that improved
performance after the incubation period was associated with higher levels of mind wandering but not with a greater number
of explicitly directed thoughts about the UUT. These data suggest that engaging in simple external tasks that allow the mind
to wander may facilitate creative problem solving.

Keywords
creativity, consciousness, insight
Received 12/26/11; Revision accepted 3/29/12

Anecdotes of individuals solving problems after relinquishing Second, focused deliberation on problems can undermine
the effort to solve them date back millennia. Indeed, many creativity, whereas distraction can enhance creativity
influential scientific thinkersincluding Newton, Poincar, (Dijksterhuis & Meurs, 2006). Third, a recent meta-analysis
and Einsteinclaim to have had their moments of inspiration of the conditions that maximize incubation effects (i.e.,
while engaged in thoughts or activities not deliberately aimed enhanced creative problem solving following a break) found
at solving the problem they were trying to solve. A key ques- that the benefits of incubation intervals are greater when indi-
tion that arises from such examples is whether engaging in any viduals are occupied by an undemanding task than when they
type of unrelated cognition increases the frequency of creative engage in either a demanding task or no task at all (Sio &
solutions, or whether the thoughts that yield such insights have Ormerod, 2009). Given that mind wandering is more frequent in
specific features. undemanding tasks than in demanding tasks (e.g., Mason et al.,
One common example of thinking that is unrelated to an 2007; Smallwood, Nind, & OConnor, 2009), this finding sug-
overt goal is the internally generated thought that occupies gests that one feature that may characterize successful incuba-
ones attention during mind wandering (Smallwood & Schooler, tion intervals could be the opportunity for mind wandering.
2006). Several lines of research suggest that mind wandering Finally, a recent investigation found that when individuals
could be linked to enhanced creativity, particularly for prob- engaged in REM sleep during an incubation interval, they
lems that have been previously encountered. First, individuals showed enhanced integration of unassociated information
with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (which is known in the service of creative problem solving (Cai, Mednick,
to be associated with mind wandering; e.g., Shaw & Giambra,
1993) tend to score higher than individuals without ADHD on
Corresponding Author:
laboratory measures of creativity (White & Shah, 2006) and on Benjamin Baird, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Building
questionnaire-based assessments of achievement in creative 429, Room 102, University of California, Santa Barbara, CA 93106-9660
areas (e.g., music, visual arts; White & Shah, 2011). E-mail: baird@psych.ucsb.edu

Downloaded from pss.sagepub.com at UNIV CALIFORNIA SANTA BARBARA on September 26, 2012

65
2 Baird et al.

Harrison, Kanady, & Mednick, 2009). Although REM sleep is incubation would be greater for participants who engaged in
very different from mind wandering, the fact that the formation the undemanding task than for participants who engaged in the
of associative networks during dreaming can lead to incubation demanding task and, third, that this effect would not be attrib-
effects is certainly consistent with the prospect that the loose utable to a greater number of explicit thoughts about the previ-
associative processes of mind wandering (e.g., Smallwood, ously encountered problems. Finally, we hypothesized that
Obonsawin, & Heim, 2003) might have similar effects. performance would selectively improve on repeated-exposure
However, caution must be taken in drawing firm conclu- problems (i.e., not on new problems) following the undemand-
sions from the results of these studies; to date, no published ing task, which would indicate that the performance improve-
study has directly compared the effects of incubation intervals ments resulted from an incubation process rather than a general
of systematically varying difficulty within a single experi- increase in creative problem solving.
ment, nor has any study directly assessed the occurrence of
mind wandering during incubation. Furthermore, there are at
least two competing interpretations of the beneficial effects of Method
tasks with a light cognitive load: Easy tasks may simply allow Participants
individuals a greater opportunity to explicitly think about pre- One hundred forty-five participants (35 males, 110 females)
vious problems, or easy tasks may encourage a global mental completed the experiment (age range: 1932 years) as partial
set (e.g., Frster, Friedman, & Liberman, 2004) that might fulfillment of a course requirement. Informed consent was
facilitate creativity independently of any specific benefit of obtained from all participants, and ethical approval for the
incubation. study was obtained from the University of California, Santa
The study reported here used an incubation paradigm to Barbara, institutional review board.
compare the effects of interpolated tasks that systematically
varied in their levels of attentional demand and thus in their
conduciveness to mind wandering. The tasks were interpo- Procedure
lated into the Unusual Uses Task (UUT), a classic and widely Baseline UUT. Participants were randomly assigned to work
used measure of divergent thinking (Guilford, 1967). The on two UUT problems (2 min per problem) in which they were
UUT was selected because it yields particularly consistent and instructed to list as many unusual uses as possible for each
robust incubation effects (Ellwood, Pallier, Snyder, & Gallate, stimulus. Participants typed their responses on a computer,
2009; Sio & Ormerod, 2009), unlike convergent-thinking directly into a text box that automatically expired after 2 min.
tasks (such as the Remote Associates Task), which have been
more prone to empirical inconsistencies (Vul & Pashler, 2007). Incubation. After completing the baseline UUT, participants
The UUT requires participants to generate as many unusual were assigned to one of four between-subjects conditions, using
uses as possible for a common object, such as a brick, in a set a counterbalanced design. In three of these conditions (demand-
amount of time. The originality of the responses is taken as an ing task, undemanding task, and rest), the baseline UUT was
index of creative thinking (e.g., Milgram & Milgram, 1976; followed by an incubation period that lasted 12 min. Partici-
Torrance, 2008; Wallach & Kogan, 1965). pants in the demanding-task condition performed a 1-back
Following the procedure in Cai et al. (2009), we assessed working memory task that places a strong constraint on top-
participants performance on UUT problems that were pre- down attention, whereas those in the undemanding-task condi-
sented both before and after the incubation interval (repeated tion performed a choice reaction time task (0-back) requiring
exposure) and on UUT problems that were presented for the infrequent responses. Studies have shown that tasks without a
first time after the incubation interval (new exposure). These working memory load elicit more mind wandering than tasks
exposure conditions allowed us to distinguish between two with a working memory load (e.g., Smallwood et al., 2009). In
different types of improvements in problem solving: incuba- the rest condition, participants were asked to sit quietly during
tion effects (repeated-exposure condition), which correspond the incubation interval. Participants in the fourth condition (no
to enhanced processing of previously encountered informa- break) did not receive a break from the UUT.
tion, and general increases in creative problem solving (new- Immediately following the incubation interval in the
exposure condition), which could correspond to general demanding-task, undemanding-task, and rest conditions, we
improvements in creative thinking or to other general facilita- administered a commonly used self-report measure of mind
tive effects (e.g., arousal or fatigue). wandering (e.g., Barron, Riby, Greer, & Smallwood, 2011;
We had four hypotheses for this study. First, we expected Matthews et al., 1999) in order to confirm differences in mind-
that participants would exhibit more mind wandering in wandering frequency between the two task conditions. (The
an interpolated undemanding task than in an interpolated questionnaire was administered following the rest interval in
demanding task, which would replicate previous findings that the rest condition in order to maintain consistency across incu-
attentional demand reduces mind wandering (Smallwood bation conditions.) This questionnaire asks participants to rate
et al., 2009). Given these anticipated differences in mind wan- how often they engaged in different types of task-unrelated
dering, we hypothesized, second, that the creative benefits of thought, such as considering personal worries or future or past

Downloaded from pss.sagepub.com at UNIV CALIFORNIA SANTA BARBARA on September 26, 2012

66
Mind Wandering Facilitates Creative Incubation 3

events (rating scale from 1 to 5, with higher scores indicating high creativity scores simply by virtue of generating a large
higher levels of mind wandering). To assess explicit thoughts number of responses). Therefore, to assess fluency, we had
about the creativity task, we had participants in these three two independent raters blind to condition tabulate the number
conditions complete a separate questionnaire on the frequency of nonredundant responses each participant generated for each
of their thoughts about the creativity problems during the incu- UUT stimulus. The interrater classification of nonredundant
bation interval. responses was highly reliable ( = .95). For each individual,
the two raters scores were averaged to yield a measure of
Postincubation UUT. After the incubation interval (or fol- fluency.
lowing the baseline UUT, in the case of the no-break condi-
tion), participants were informed that they would work on the
UUT again. Four UUT problems (2 min per problem) were Results
presented in a random order: two repeat problems (repeated- Mind wandering
exposure condition) that were identical to the problems pre-
sented at baseline and two randomly assigned new problems Participants in the undemanding-task condition reported sig-
(new-exposure condition). nificantly greater mind wandering (M = 2.47, SD = 0.66) in the
retrospective questionnaire than did participants in the
Assessing propensity to mind-wander. At the end of the demanding-task condition (M = 2.15, SD = 0.67), F(1, 72) =
experiment, all participants completed the Daydreaming Fre- 4.04, p < .05, 2 = .05. This result replicates previous findings
quency subscale of the Imaginal Process Inventory (IPI), that working memory load decreases the frequency of mind
which assesses individuals general propensity to mind- wandering.2 An analysis of the demanding-task, undemanding-
wander (Singer & Antrobus, 1972). task, and rest conditions revealed no group differences in par-
ticipants retrospective reports about the degree to which they
had been explicitly thinking about the previous creativity task,
Tasks F(2, 106) = 0.09, p = .90, 2 = .002.
Interpolated tasks. Stimuli for the demanding and unde-
manding tasks were the digits from 1 through 9, which were
presented serially (in quasirandom order) in the center of a Incubation-task performance measures
computer screen for 1,000 ms each; each digit was followed No significant difference in accuracy was observed between
by a 1,500-ms fixation cross. In both of these tasks, nontargets the undemanding task (M = .87, SD = .10) and the demanding
were black numbers that required no response, and nontargets task (M = .88, SD = .20), F(1, 72) = 0.06, p = .80, 2 = .001.
occurred frequently, whereas targets were infrequent. In the Response time to targets was significantly faster in the
undemanding task, targets were colored numbers, and partici- demanding task (M = 518.39 ms, SD = 117.55 ms) than in
pants had to determine whether each target stimulus was even the undemanding task (M = 648.97 ms, SD = 48.21 ms), F(1,
or odd. In the demanding task, targets were colored question 72) = 38.93, p < .001, 2 = .35. Faster response times were
marks, and participants had to determine whether the stimulus expected in the demanding task because responses were based
immediately preceding each target was even or odd. Partici- on the previous (already-encoded) digit, whereas the unde-
pants in both conditions received a short practice session with manding task required participants to first encode the target
feedback. digit and then respond. This difference in response times
reflects the key difference in the structure of the two tasks: The
UUT. Following the procedure used by Wallach and Kogan demanding task required that the identity of nontarget stimuli
(1965), we pooled responses to each UUT stimulus across the be encoded, whereas the undemanding task did not require
sample, and points were assigned for statistically unique that participants attend to nontarget stimuli.
responses.1 Percentage improvement on the UUT was calcu-
lated separately for each problem type (repeated exposure,
new exposure) and was compared across conditions (unde- UUT uniqueness scores
manding task, demanding task, rest, no break). This was calcu- We first analyzed the UUT uniqueness scores using a mixed-
lated as [(postincubation UUT score baseline UUT score)/ model analysis of variance (ANOVA) with exposure condition
(baseline UUT score)] 100 (see Cai et al., 2009, for a similar (repeated exposure, new exposure) as a repeated measures fac-
analytic method). Percentage improvement was calculated at tor and incubation condition (undemanding task, demanding
the individual level and then averaged for each condition. task, rest, no break) as a between-subjects factor. An Exposure
Although uniqueness scoring is the most standard method Condition Incubation Condition interaction emerged, F(1,
of scoring divergent-thinking tasks (e.g., Milgram & Milgram, 141) = 4.98, p < .01, 2 = .10. To further explore this effect, we
1976; Torrance, 2008; Wallach & Kogan, 1965), it has been used univariate ANOVAs to analyze incubation-condition dif-
criticized (Silvia et al., 2008) on the grounds that it may con- ferences in repeated-exposure and new-exposure UUT unique-
found creativity with fluency (e.g., participants may receive ness scores.

Downloaded from pss.sagepub.com at UNIV CALIFORNIA SANTA BARBARA on September 26, 2012

67
4 Baird et al.

60 UUT fluency
Improvement on UUT (%)

40 Fluency scores for the repeated-exposure problems did not dif-


fer significantly between incubation conditions, F(1, 144) =
20 1.15, p = .39, 2 = .02. This result rules out the possibility that
0 between-condition differences in creativity as indexed by
uniqueness scores were a result of confounding fluency and
20 creativity.
40
Undemanding Demanding Rest No Break
Incubation Condition
Individual differences in mind wandering and
UUT uniqueness scores
Fig. 1. Improvement in Unusual Uses Task (UUT) uniqueness scores (post-
Scores on the Daydreaming Frequency subscale of the IPI
incubation performance relative to baseline performance) for repeated-
exposure problems as a function of incubation condition. Error bars indicate positively correlated with UUT uniqueness scores for both
standard errors of the mean. repeated-exposure problems, r = .22, p < .05, and new-
exposure problems, r = .20, p < .05. This result provides pre-
liminary evidence that individuals who mind-wander more
frequently in their daily lives may be more creative in
Repeated-exposure condition. There was a significant general.
effect of incubation condition in the repeated-exposure condi-
tion, F(1, 144) = 4.99, p < .01, 2 = .10. Participants who
engaged in an undemanding task during the incubation inter- Discussion
val displayed significantly greater improvement in UUT Although research has suggested that taking a break can facili-
uniqueness scores for repeated-exposure problems compared tate creativity, the mechanism of this incubation effect has
with participants who engaged in a demanding task (p < .01), remained unclear and has been the source of considerable
a period of rest (p < .01), or no break (p < .01). No significant empirical research and theoretical debate (e.g., Dijksterhuis &
difference in improvement was observed between participants Meurs, 2006; Smith & Blankenship, 1989; Yaniv & Meyer,
who received no break and those who engaged in either a 1987). The study reported here demonstrated that taking a
demanding task (p = .35) or a period of rest (p = .30); thus, no break involving an undemanding task improved performance
incubation effect was observed in the latter two conditions on a classic creativity task (the UUT) far more than did taking
(see Fig. 1). a break involving a demanding task, resting, or taking no
break. Notably, this improvement was observed only for
New-exposure condition. No incubation-condition differ- repeated-exposure problems, which demonstrates that it
ences were observed for improvement in uniqueness scores resulted from an incubation process rather than a general
for new problems, F(1, 144) = 1.01, p = .39, 2 = .02 (Fig. 2). increase in creative problem solving. Together, these data cor-
No significant difference was observed between participants roborate, within a single experiment, the conclusion of a recent
who received no break and those who engaged in an unde- meta-analysis (Sio & Ormerod, 2009) showing that incubation
manding task (p = .21), a demanding task (p = .70), or rest effects were larger in studies in which individuals engaged in
(p = .95). Thus, there was no significant incubation effect in an undemanding interpolated task than in studies that included
any incubation condition for the new-exposure problems. a demanding interpolated task or a rest period.
Our data support the notion that specific types of unrelated
thought facilitate creative problem solving. Even though the
act of encoding information in working memory was unrelated
Improvement on UUT (%)

60
to the solutions of the creativity problems, no incubation effect
40 was observed in the demanding-task condition. Moreover, the
20 undemanding-task condition was not associated with increased
0
frequency of thoughts explicitly about the creativity problems,
but was characterized by high levels of mind wandering. Thus,
20 our data indicate that creative problem solutions may be facili-
40 tated specifically by simple external tasks (i.e., tasks not
Undemanding Demanding Rest No Break related to the primary task) that maximize mind wandering.
Incubation Condition The observation that performance selectively improved for
repeated-exposure problems (and not for new problems) indi-
Fig. 2. Improvement in Unusual Uses Task (UUT) uniqueness scores
cates that engaging in a task conducive to mind wandering
(postincubation performance relative to baseline performance) for new-
exposure problems as a function of incubation condition. Error bars indicate does not lead to general increases in creative problem-solving
standard errors of the mean. ability. However, performance on both repeated-exposure and

Downloaded from pss.sagepub.com at UNIV CALIFORNIA SANTA BARBARA on September 26, 2012

68
Mind Wandering Facilitates Creative Incubation 5

new problems positively correlated with individuals general Funding


propensity to mind-wander in everyday life (as assessed by the This research was supported by the John Templeton Foundation
IPI). This observation provides preliminary evidence that under Grant No. 24329, awarded to Jonathan W. Schooler. Benjamin
there may be a relationship between individual differences in Baird is supported by a National Science Foundation Graduate
mind wandering and creativity. Although this observation is Research Fellowship under Grant No. DGE-0707430. Michael D.
intriguing, it should be noted that this study lacked assess- Mrazek and Michael S. Franklin are supported through Office of
ments for a variety of other individual differences measures Education Grant No. R305H030235, awarded to Jonathan W.
(most notably, measures of inhibition) that could in principle Schooler and Jonathan Smallwood.
account for the association between propensity to mind- Notes
wander and performance on the creativity task. An important
direction for future research will be to conduct a more thor- 1. Following the procedure used by Wallach and Kogan (1965), we
ough assessment of the relationship between individual differ- categorically assigned statistically unique responses a score of 1.
ences in mind wandering and creativity while controlling for Because problems were repeated in our incubation design, responses
other factors that could contribute to this relationship. appearing up to two times across the sample received points. An
Further research is needed to determine precisely why the alternative scoring method using a graded scale (from 1 to 5;
unrelated thoughts that occur during mind wandering uniquely S. Fiore, personal communication, January 25, 2011) yielded nearly
facilitate incubation. One possibility is that mind wandering identical results.
enhances creativity by increasing unconscious associative pro- 2. As noted, we also administered the mind-wandering questionnaire
cessing, as predicted by the spreading-activation account of following the rest interval. The score on the retrospective mind-
incubation (e.g., Yaniv & Meyer, 1987; see also Dijksterhuis wandering scale in the rest condition (M = 2.35, SD = 0.57) was not
& Meurs, 2006). A second possibility derives from recent neu- significantly different from the score on this scale in either the
roimaging work indicating that executive and default networks undemanding-task condition (p = .44) or the demanding-task condi-
interact during mind wandering (Christoff, Gordon, Small- tion (p = .19), although this comparison is difficult to interpret
wood, Smith, & Schooler, 2009). Interactions between these because the rest condition included no primary task to which internal
networks are observed relatively rarely in cognitive neurosci- thoughts could fail to pertain.
ence (although see Baird, Smallwood, & Schooler, 2011;
Gerlach, Spreng, Gilmore, & Schacter, 2011); considering that References
activations in both networks are observed prior to successful Baird, B., Smallwood, S., & Schooler, J. W. (2011). Back to the
solution of insight problems (Kounios et al., 2008; Kounios future: Autobiographical planning and the functionality of mind-
et al., 2006), engaging in tasks conducive to mind wandering wandering. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 16041611.
could contribute to incubation by creating a situation in which Barron, E., Riby, L. M., Greer, J., & Smallwood, J. (2011). Absorbed
default and executive systems mutually contribute to associa- in thought: The effect of mind wandering on the processing of
tive processing. Neurocognitive investigations of the brain relevant and irrelevant events. Psychological Science, 22, 596
activations that occur during successful incubation intervals 601.
might profitably explore this issue. Cai, D. J., Mednick, S. A., Harrison, E. M., Kanady, J. C., & Med-
Anecdotal accounts of the inception of creative ideas have nick, S. C. (2009). REM, not incubation, improves creativity by
long implicated mind wandering in the creative process. The priming associative networks. Proceedings of the National Acad-
findings reported here provide arguably the most direct evi- emy of Sciences, USA, 106, 10130.
dence to date that conditions that favor mind wandering also Christoff, K., Gordon, A. M., Smallwood, J., Smith, R., & Schooler,
enhance creativity. From a theoretical perspective, this J. W. (2009). Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default
research also helps to establish at least one benefit from network and executive system contributions to mind wandering.
engaging in this otherwise seemingly dysfunctional mental Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 106,
state. Although mind wandering may be linked to compro- 87198724.
mised performance on an external task (Barron et al., 2011; Dijksterhuis, A., & Meurs, T. (2006). Where creativity resides: The
McVay & Kane, 2009) and may be a signature of unhappiness generative power of unconscious thought. Consciousness and
(Killingsworth & Gilbert, 2010), it may also serve as a founda- Cognition, 15, 135146.
tion for creative inspiration. Ellwood, S., Pallier, G., Snyder, A., & Gallate, J. (2009). The incu-
bation effect: Hatching a solution? Creativity Research Journal,
Acknowledgments 21, 614.
We thank Steve Fiore for helpful discussion and James Schlegel, Frster, J., Friedman, R. S., & Liberman, N. (2004). Temporal con-
Alex Weis, and Adam Haik for assistance in conducting the research. strual effects on abstract and concrete thinking: Consequences for
insight and creative cognition. Journal of Personality and Social
Declaration of Conflicting Interests Psychology, 87, 177189.
The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with Gerlach, K. D., Spreng, R. N., Gilmore, A. W., & Schacter, D. L.
respect to their authorship or the publication of this article. (2011). Solving future problems: Default network and executive

Downloaded from pss.sagepub.com at UNIV CALIFORNIA SANTA BARBARA on September 26, 2012

69
6 Baird et al.

activity associated with goal-directed mental simulations. Neuro- new subjective scoring methods. Psychology of Aesthetics, Cre-
Image, 55, 18161824. ativity, and the Arts, 2, 6885.
Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York, Singer, J. L., & Antrobus, J. S. (1972). Daydreaming, imaginal pro-
NY: McGraw-Hill. cesses, and personality: A normative study. In P. W. Sheehan
Killingsworth, M. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is (Ed.), The function and nature of imagery (pp. 175202). New
an unhappy mind. Science, 330, 932. York, NY: Academic Press.
Kounios, J., Fleck, J. I., Green, D. L., Payne, L., Stevenson, J. L., Sio, U. N., & Ormerod, T. C. (2009). Does incubation enhance prob-
Bowden, E. M., & Jung-Beeman, M. (2008). The origins of lem solving? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin,
insight in resting-state brain activity. Neuropsychologia, 46, 281 135, 94120.
291. Smallwood, J., Nind, L., & OConnor, R. C. (2009). When is your
Kounios, J., Frymiare, J. L., Bowden, E. M., Fleck, J. I., Subra- head at? An exploration of the factors associated with the tempo-
maniam, K., Parrish, T. B., & Jung-Beeman, M. (2006). The ral focus of the wandering mind. Consciousness and Cognition,
prepared mind: Neural activity prior to problem presentation pre- 18, 118125.
dicts subsequent solution by sudden insight. Psychological Sci- Smallwood, J., Obonsawin, M. C., & Heim, D. (2003). Task unre-
ence, 17, 882890. lated thought: The role of distributed processing. Consciousness
Mason, M. F., Norton, M. I., Van, H. J. D., Wegner, D. M., Grafton, and Cognition, 12, 169189.
S. T., & Macrae, C. N. (2007). Wandering minds: The default Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psycho-
network and stimulus-independent thought. Science, 315, 393 logical Bulletin, 132, 946958.
395. Smith, S. M., & Blankenship, S. E. (1989). Incubation effects. Bul-
Matthews, G., Joyner, L., Gilliland, K., Campbell, S., Huggins, J., & letin of the Psychonomic Society, 27, 311314.
Falconer, S. (1999). Validation of a comprehensive stress state Torrance, E. P. (2008). Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking: Norms-
questionnaire: Towards a state big three? In I. Mervielde, I. J. technical manual, verbal forms A and B. Bensenville, IL: Scho-
Deary, F. DeFruyt, & F. Ostendorf (Eds.), Personality psychol- lastic Testing Service.
ogy in Europe (Vol. 7, pp. 335350). Tilburg, The Netherlands: Vul, E., & Pashler, H. (2007). Incubation benefits only after people
Tilburg University Press. have been misdirected. Memory & Cognition, 35, 701710.
McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2009). Conducting the train of thought: Wallach, M. A., & Kogan, N. (1965). Modes of thinking in young
Working memory capacity, goal neglect, and mind wandering in children: A study of the creativityintelligence distinction. New
an executive-control task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: York, NY: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 196204. White, H. A., & Shah, P. (2006). Uninhibited imaginations: Creativ-
Milgram, R. M., & Milgram, N. A. (1976). Creative thinking and ity in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Person-
creative performance in Israeli students. Journal of Educational ality and Individual Differences, 40, 11211131.
Psychology, 68, 255259. White, H. A., & Shah, P. (2011). Creative style and achievement in
Shaw, G. A., & Giambra, L. M. (1993). Task unrelated thoughts of adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Personality
college students diagnosed as hyperactive in childhood. Develop- and Individual Differences, 50, 673677.
mental Neuropsychology, 9, 1730. Yaniv, I., & Meyer, D. E. (1987). Activation and metacognition of
Silvia, P. J., Barona, C. M., Cram, J. T., Hess, K. I., Martinez, J. L., inaccessible stored information: Potential bases for incubation
Richard, C. A., . . . Willse, J. T. (2008). Assessing creativity with effects in problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
divergent thinking tasks: Exploring the reliability and validity of Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 187205.

Downloaded from pss.sagepub.com at UNIV CALIFORNIA SANTA BARBARA on September 26, 2012

70
Cerebral Cortex August 2015;25:21602169
doi:10.1093/cercor/bhu022
Advance Access publication February 20, 2014

Neural Correlates of Subliminal Language Processing


Vadim Axelrod1,2, Moshe Bar1,4, Geraint Rees2,3 and Galit Yovel5,6
1
The Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Bar Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel, 2UCL Institute of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 3Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London, London, UK, 4Athinoula A. Martinos Center
for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Charlestown, MA, USA, 5School of
Psychological Sciences and 6Sagol School of Neuroscience, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel

Address correspondence to Vadim Axelrod. Email: vadim.axelrod@gmail.com

Language is a high-level cognitive function, so exploring the neural see Vigneau et al. 2006; Price 2012), the role of the frontal
correlates of unconscious language processing is essential for un- lobes in unconscious language processing remains elusive. In
derstanding the limits of unconscious processing in general. The particular, only one study reports activations in the inferior
results of several functional magnetic resonance imaging studies frontal gyrus for subliminally presented words (Diaz and
have suggested that unconscious lexical and semantic processing is McCarthy 2007). However, this study did not implement sub-
conned to the posterior temporal lobe, without involvement of the jective/objective awareness reports after each trial (Seth et al.
frontal lobethe regions that are indispensable for conscious 2008) and it is therefore difcult to condently determine
language processing. However, previous studies employed a similarly whether the activations indeed reect unconscious language
designed masked priming paradigm with briey presented single and processing. It is noteworthy that while such limited empirical
contextually unrelated words. It is thus possible, that the stimulation evidence for unconscious activations in frontal lobes (e.g., Lau
level was insufciently strong to be detected in the high-level frontal and Passingham 2007; van Gaal et al. 2010) is in line with
regions. Here, in a high-resolution fMRI and multivariate pattern some prominent theoretical models, such as Global Workspace
analysis study we explored the neural correlates of subliminal model (Dehaene et al. 1998), it is also possible that subliminal
language processing using a novel paradigm, where written mean- sensory stimulation in the previous studies was too weak to ac-
ingful sentences were suppressed from awareness for extended dur- tivate the frontal lobes (Haynes 2009). Specically, the earlier
ation using continuous ash suppression. We found that subjectively imaging studies used subliminal priming masking paradigm
and objectively invisible meaningful sentences and unpronounceable with a brief stimulus exposure, which could have resulted in
nonwords could be discriminated not only in the left posterior insufcient brain stimulation. In addition, a more general limit-
superior temporal sulcus (STS), but critically, also in the left middle ation of previous studies was that none of them measured
frontal gyrus. We conclude that frontal lobes play a role in uncon- awareness on each individual trial during the neuroimaging
scious language processing and that activation of the frontal lobes experiment (Seth et al. 2008). Consequently, if on some trials
per se might not be sufcient for achieving conscious awareness. the primes were visible or at least partially visible (Kouider
et al. 2010) they would still be considered unaware and there-
Keywords: continuous ash suppression (CFS), decoding subliminal fore their neural correlates may not reect only unconsciously
content, fMRI imaging of unconscious processing, multivoxel pattern processed material.
classication analysis (MVPA), subliminal language processing In the current functional magnetic resonance imaging
(MRI) study we explored the neural correlates of subliminal
language processing, with a novel design that addresses the
concerns reviewed above. Observers were presented with
series of consecutively presented textual stimuli: meaningful
Introduction sentences or unpronounceable nonwords (Fedorenko et al.
What are the limits of unconscious language processing? This 2010), which were rendered invisible using continuous ash
question has been intensively researched during last 50 years suppression (CFS) (Tsuchiya and Koch 2005) for extended
(for reviews see Kouider and Dehaene 2007; Lin and He 2009; period of time (10 s) (Fig. 1). Critically, as we sought for evi-
Van den Bussche et al. 2009). Though no consensus has been dence of unconscious language processing of any type, we
reached, many behavioral experiments show that subliminally decided to use meaningful sentences that required not only
presented text can be processed not only at a relatively low semantic, but also syntactic and structural processingthe
orthographic level (e.g., Dehaene et al. 2001; Devlin et al. design which permitted to increase potential differences
2004), but also at a higher semantic level (e.g., Marcel 1983; between meaningful (sentences) and meaningless (non-
Jiang et al. 2007; Costello et al. 2009; Sklar et al. 2012; but see words) conditions. After each block of either sentences or
Holender 1986; Abrams and Greenwald 2000). Neuroimaging nonwords participants reported whether they had been
studies show that the visual word form area (VWFA) (Cohen aware of even a single worda procedure which ensured that
et al. 2000) is involved in unconscious orthographic word pro- data analyses were conducted only on blocks judged invisible
cessing (e.g., Dehaene et al. 2001; Kouider et al. 2007), by participants. To discriminate between neural activity eli-
whereas unconscious semantic language processing is most cited by the 2 conditions we used multivoxel pattern classi-
consistently observed along the left posterior STS (Devlin et al. cation analyses (MVPA) focusing on the language network
2004; Nakamura et al. 2007; see also Nakamura et al. 2005). (Fedorenko et al. 2010), which was localized on a per-
Whereas the central role of frontal lobes in various aspects of participant basis using the same stimuli while they were fully
conscious language processing is unquestionable (for reviews visible. The principal goal of our research was to test whether

The Author 2014. Published by Oxford University Press.


This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/), which permits unrestricted
reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
71
Materials and Methods

Apparatus
MRI data were collected using a 3T GE MRI scanner with an 8-channel
head coil. Echo planar imaging used a T2*-weighted sequence to
measure changes in blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) signal. The
scanning parameters were as follows: repetition time (TR) = 2.5 s, time
echo (TE) = 30 ms, ip angle: 90, slice thickness: 3.4 mm no gap, eld
of view (FOV) 200 mm, 32 slices; data were acquired using 96 96
matrix (in plane resolution 2.08 2.08 mm), reconstruct into 128 128
matrix (in plane resolution 1.56 1.56 mm). Slice orientation was par-
allel to temporal lobe with full coverage of the cerebral cortex. An ana-
tomical SPRG scan with full brain coverage was collected with
1 1 1 mm resolution (TE = 3.52 ms, TR = 9.104 ms).

Participants
Seventeen healthy volunteers (age: 2343, 9 females, all right-handed)
with normal or corrected-to-normal vision participated in the exper-
iment. The mother-tongue of all participants was Russian (they were
born in the Soviet Union and lived there or in an ex-Soviet Union
country at least till the age of 14). The study was approved by the
ethics committee of the Tel Aviv Sourasky Medical Center. All partici-
pants signed informed consent to participate in the study. Data of 2
participants were excluded from the analysis due to inability to follow
the instructions (one participant) and excessive movements (>1 cm) in
the scanner (another participant).

Experimental Setup

Stimuli
Experiment textual material included series of words presented one
word at a time (Fig. 1). The stimuli were of 2 types: meaningful sen-
tences or series of unpronounceable nonwords sentences (random
permutation of the letters) (e.g., Fedorenko et al. 2010, 2011). The
meaningful sentences described neutral situations (e.g., weather,
nature description etc.). The textual material was in Russian (Cyrillic al-
phabet) written with the Arial font. The letters were presented in lower
case (including the rst letter of the sentence) and there was no period
sign at the end of the sentence. The words were shown at the center of
the screen; visual angle size varied between 8 5 (horizontal vertical)
and 30 5. The sentences in both conditions were 6 words in length.
In total, there were 40 different meaningful and 40 nonword sen-
tences.

Continuous Flash Suppression


A standard CFS procedure was used (Tsuchiya and Koch 2005). In the
scanner participants wore MR-compatible cardboard anaglyph cyan/
red glasses. Stimuli were projected with an LCD projector (NEC,
VT660K), positioned ahead of the participant and viewed through a
Figure 1. Schematic ow of one block with meaningful sentence in CFS invisible tilted mirror mounted on the MR head coil. Stimuli were projected
experiment. The words are translated to English for illustrative purpose only while in using the red RGB color channel (visible using red lter) and for the
the experiment all the materials were in Russian. The structure of the blocks with Mondrian mask blue/green RGB channels were used. The red (target)
unpronounceable nonwords was the same as the blocks with meaningful sentences, glass lter was always placed over the nondominant eye of each partici-
but substituting nonwords (random letter permutations) for words. In the functional pant. Eye-dominance was assessed prior to the experiment by asking
localizer (visible experiment) the block ow was similar to that depicted in this gure the participants to view a distant object through a hole made by the
while the words were visible via both eyes (no CFS mask) and the task was 1-back ngers of their 2 hands (Miles test) (Miles 1930; Mendola and Conner
word repetition (instead of awareness report at the end of the block). 2007). The screen luminance of the text was set to 40% ( percent of the
maximal screen luminance; dark gray) and of the background was 61%
the frontal lobes were involved in any unconscious proces- (light gray). The Mondrian pattern was projected on the center of the
sing of language. The secondary goal was to reveal whether screen and its size was 34 6 of visual angle (to cover the longest
using a paradigm that is different from previous studies and word). The pattern of the CFS mask consisted of unlled ellipses and
rectangles, which were similar in image pattern structure to letters (see
by measuring awareness report after each block, the neural
Fig. 1 for the examples of the pattern). The frequency rate at which the
correlates of subliminal processing could still be found in the Mondrian patterns were changed was 10 Hz (100 ms for each image).
left posterior temporal lobe. Given that different subliminal
paradigms do not always yield similar effects (Almeida et al.
Experimental Design
2008, 2013; Kanai et al. 2010; Faivre et al. 2012), such a repli- Participants underwent 2 separate fMRI experiments: the main exper-
cation is important for establishing this general cognitive iment using CFS masking and a functional localizer with fully visible
phenomenon. text. The sessions with visible text were always the last in order not to

Cerebral Cortex August 2015, V 25 N 8 2161


72
provide additional cues about the appearance of invisible stimuli. The nonwords, detecting one of the stimuli (single word or nonword)
design and ow of both experiments were identical except for the visi- during the block would allow you to indicate the correct response.
bility level of the stimuli and the behavioral task (described below). In Even if you did not see anything, we ask you to guess. The second
the functional localizer experiment, no Mondrian mask was used and response asks you to indicate whether your rst response was based
the text was projected to both eyes using all 3 RGB channels (to elimin- on seeing the text or guessing. It should be noted that as the condition
ate potential head movements in the scanner between 2 experiments, of sentences is comprised of various types of language processing
participants still wore anaglyph glasses during this experiment as (sentence syntax and structure, words semantics etc.), it was crucial to
well). Screen luminance of the text in the functional localizer exper- ensure that any part of language processing remained unconscious.
iment was 0% (black) and the background was 61% (light gray). This was the reason that we made it clear to participants, that detecting
The experiment used a block-design with each experimental block a single word in the block is sufcient for a correct answer. Participants
lasting 10 s and interleaved with a xation block of 7.5-s duration. Fix- underwent a short training session outside the scanner as well as a
ation block was an empty screen with + sign at the middle (0.2 0.2 short training session inside the scanner at the beginning of the exper-
of visual angle; background luminosity: 61% [light gray color], fore- iment to ensure that they understood the instructions. Prior to starting
ground luminosity: 0% [black color]). Each session consisted of 12 the experimental sessions all participants conrmed that the instruc-
experimental blocks (6 blocks of meaningful sentences and unpro- tions are clear for them. The instructions were also repeated during the
nounceable nonwords, respectively). Each session started with a 10 s experiment, between the sessions. At the end of the experiment,
xation cross. Total session duration was 3 min and 40 s. Number of during the informal debrieng, none of the participants indicated any
sessions per participant varied between participants: for the main difculty with performing the task according to the instructions.
experiment (invisibility experiment) it was between 7 and 11 and for
the functional localizer experiment it was between 3 and 5. The use of
a larger number of short experimental sessions compared with a
smaller number of long experimental sessions improves classication Data Analysis
performance (Coutanche and Thompson-Schill 2012).
The schematic ow of one block (invisible CFS experiment) consist- Preprocessing
ing of meaningful sentences is shown in Figure 1. The ow of the con- Data analysis used SPM5 (Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging,
dition with unpronounceable nonwords was similar. For illustrative London, UK; http://www.l.ion.ucl.ac.uk). The rst 4 volumes (4 TRs,
purposes only, in the gure the words have been translated to English, 10 s) of each session were discarded to allow for T1 equilibration
while all the experiment materials were in Russian. Duration of a effects. Preprocessing steps applied for functional (EPI) scans in-
single word was 0.4 s; duration of a single sentence (6 words) was 2.4 s cluded: realignment, slice-time correction, motion correction, normali-
(words appeared one after another without interstimulus interval). zation to 2 2 3 voxel resolution using Montreal Neurological
Each experimental block consisted of 4 sentences (or 4 lists of non- Institute (MNI) template and spatial smoothing with a full-width at
words). The sentences appeared back to back without delay between half-maximum = 6 mm kernel. For the normalization we used a unied
the last word of the previous sentence and the rst word of a next sen- segmentation procedure (Ashburner and Friston 2005).
tence. For some sentences of the block the last word of the previous
sentence and the rst word of the consecutive sentence was identical
(e.g., the queue was extremely long today, today the weather is very
nice). The number of repetitions of the last and rst word varied ran- Region of Interest Localization
domly between blocks (minimum 0, maximum 2). The rst word of For the language functional localizer (visible text) we estimated a GLM
each sentence appeared with a random horizontal position jitter (one model (HRF boxcar function) with 2 regressors: meaningful sentences
or 2 letters from centered position). This ensured that when the rst and nonwords. We used the contrast meaningful sentences >
and last word of the sentence repeated, there was no effect of word nonwords to identify a network of language processing regions for
freezing on the screen. The blocks of unpronounceable nonwords each participant (Fedorenko et al. 2010, 2011, 2012). To constraint
were similar to blocks of meaningful sentences (Fig. 1) but substituting individual GLM-dened functional activations we used probabilistic
nonwords (random letter permutations) for words. We decided to use group-level functional masks (Fedorenko et al. 2010; http://web.mit.
the random letter permutation and not the letter permutation of the edu/evelina9/www/funcloc/funcloc_parcels.html). Thus, for each
real words, since the later can be still occasionally recognized as real mask region/participant based on individual meaningful sentences >
words (e.g., Wentura et al. 2005). The 6-word sentence-like structure nonwords GLM contrast we selected a contiguous cluster of most se-
and the repetition of the last and rst word were preserved for non- lective voxels (number of voxels is specied below). The regions
words as well. dened by the masks are shown in Figure 2. There were 11 regions in
The behavioral tasks in the functional localizer and in the CFS sub- total: 5 regions in the left parieto-temporal lobe (angular gyrus, supra-
liminal experiment were different. In the functional localizer partici- marginal gyrus, posterior STS, middle anterior temporal gyrus, and
pants were asked to press any button on the response box when they anterior temporal gyrus), 2 regions in the right hemisphere of the tem-
detected a consecutive repetition of a word (1-back task). The repeated poral lobe ( posterior STS, middle anterior temporal gyrus) and 4
words could only be the last and rst words of a sentence (see above). regions in the left hemisphere of the frontal lobe (orbital inferior
This task ensured that participants were attentive to the stimuli. frontal gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, superior
In the CFS main experiment, participants were required to make 2 frontal gyrus). Critically, as multivariate prediction is inuenced by
separate responses after every block (either 4 sentences or 4 nonword region of interest (ROI) size (e.g., Eger et al. 2008; Walther et al. 2009;
lists, see Fig. 1). These responses were made during the xation block Said et al. 2010) we ensured the ROIs of different regions were of an
(no instructions were presented). The participants were rst required equal size of 100 voxels (1200 mm3). In additional analyses we also ex-
(objective response) whether they thought that a block consisted of plored a range of different ROI sizes of 50 and 150 voxels. The ROI
sentences (comprising meaningful words) or of nonwords. Partici- size could not be increased further since the size of probabilistic
pants were then required to make a second response (subjective group-level functional masks (Fedorenko et al. 2010) of some of the
response) indicating whether their rst response was based on seeing regions (e.g., left superior frontal gyrus) was <200 voxels. Dening
the stimuli or on a guess. Below are the instructions, which were ROIs of equal size was undertaken using custom MATLAB code, where
given to participants: You will be presented with blocks of either sen- for each region/participant the code selected the contiguous cluster of
tences composed of meaningful words or series of nonwords. The voxels with the highest z-score values relating to the meaningful
words or nonwords will be presented sequentially. The stimuli are pre- sentences > nonwords contrast in the independent localizer with
sented in a way that makes it very hard and probably impossible to see visible stimuli (similar procedure had been previously applied for face-
them. At the end of each block you need to make 2 responses. The rst selective voxels here [Axelrod and Yovel 2012]). The list of the ROIs
response asks you to indicate whether it was a block of sentences (100 voxels size) with their coordinates and average z-score values can
(words) or nonwords. Because in each block we present only words or be found in Table 3.

2162 Neural Correlates of Subliminal Language Processing Axelrod et al.



73
Figure 2. Probabilistic group-level masks of language network (Fedorenko et al. 2010; http://web.mit.edu/evelina9/www/funcloc/funcloc_parcels.html) projected on a SPM
template T1 image. The names of the regions are: 1left angular gyrus, 2left supramarginal gyrus, 3left posterior STS, 4left middle anterior temporal, 5left anterior
temporal, 6left orbital inferior frontal gyrus, 7left inferior frontal gyrus, 8left middle frontal gyrus, 9left superior frontal gyrus, 10right posterior STS, 11right middle
anterior temporal. Regions IDs correspond to the IDs in Table 3.

Main Experiment: Multivariate Pattern Analysis participants was guessed were used. To ensure that for each partici-
A multivariate pattern classication analysis was performed on raw pant the equal number of data points (blocks) per condition (otherwise
BOLD signal values. After a preprocessing stage (the step which was the use of imbalanced data set might bias classication performance
identical for all EPI data), the data of the main (CFS) experiment were [Japkowicz and Stephen 2002]) we randomly discarded the data points
detrended and normalized using the z-score MATLAB function. This from the condition with largest amount of data.
procedure was applied to the full scan voxel time course. The time
course was also shifted 3 TRs to account for hemodynamic lag. In the
multivariate analysis the global signal average for each of the 2 con- Results
ditions was subtracted (e.g., Serences et al. 2009; Misaki et al. 2010),
while this procedure was performed separately for each session in
order to prevent information leakage in course of cross-validation Behavioral Results
procedure. Global signal average subtraction, which was applied as To establish the level of stimulus awareness (during fMRI scan-
part of per-condition normalization procedure, increases classication ning) after each block (duration of 10 s) participants were
performance (Raizada et al. 2010; see also Aksoy and Haralick 2001) asked to make 2 separate judgments: an objective response
and may be benecial, as it prevents voxels with higher values and (meaningful sentences/nonwords) and a subjective (con-
range to dominate the classiers weights (Coutanche 2013). In
addition, subtraction of the global signal average from each condition
dence) response (knew/guessed). Most blocks of both con-
permits a more straightforward interpretation of the results, as even ditions were judged to be invisible: the percentage of blocks
minimal differences in global average between conditions might be a on which participants responded with guessed for meaning-
result of different level of arousal or attention (Coutanche 2013). We ful sentences was 80.3% (standard error of mean [SEM] = 4.5%)
obtained qualitatively similar results when the analyses were con- and for nonwords was 82.6% (SEM = 3.4%). Critically, objective
ducted without subtracting global signal average. The TRs within each responses for these invisible (guessed) blocks were at
block were averaged, resulting in a single average data point value per
chance level: meaningful sentences = 51.7% (SEM = 2.1%;
block that was used as the input to pattern classication analysis. The
LibSVM MATLAB implementation of a linear support vector machine P-value = 0.22, t < 1, one-tailed t-test vs. 50%) and nonwords =
was used for classication (http://www.csie.ntu.edu.tw/~cjlin/libsvm/ 51.7% (SEM = 2.4%; P-value = 0.24, t < 1, one-tailed t-test vs.
) using a leave-one-session out cross-validation procedure, which was 50%). The distribution of all subjective and objective responses
repeated according to number of scans available for each participant. is shown in Tables 1 and 2. In addition, during the informal de-
Signicance of prediction rate was established using a one-tailed t-test brieng after the experiment, we asked the participants what
above the chance for the group classication rates (Eger et al. 2008;
exactly they saw when they choose to answer guessed. Criti-
Meyer et al. 2010; Nestor et al. 2011). Multiple comparison Bonferroni
correction was made based on the number of ROIs in posterior tem- cally, all the participants indicated that when they responded
poral lobes (7 ROIs, P-value signicance threshold = 0.0071) and guessed then they could not see even a single letter within a
frontal lobe: 4 ROIs, P-value signicance threshold = 0.0125). Of note, presented stimulus. Thus, taken together, we conclude that
the signicance of the results persists when the correction is made invisibility manipulation was effective and that the trials,
based on total number of ROIs (11 ROIs). Supplementary analyses which were reported as guessed, were genuinely invisible.
tested whether successful prediction could also be achieved based on To explore unconscious processing, we restricted all sub-
signal global level and this analysis differed from the main analysis in 2
ways: 1) the average signal was not subtracted; 2) the timecourses of all
sequent analyses to blocks ranked by participants as guessed
voxels in a ROI were averaged resulting in one timecourse (i.e., classi- in their subjective response. The average number of blocks per
cation using one dimension). In both main and supplementary ana- participant/condition was 38.53 (mean squared error [MSE] =
lyses only the blocks where the second (subjective) report of the 2.49). It was not feasible to investigate neural correlates of

Cerebral Cortex August 2015, V 25 N 8 2163


74
Table 1
Distribution of subjective responses (condence rating, second response)

Knew Guessed
Meaningful sentences 19.7% (SEM: 4.5%) 80.3% (SEM: 4.5%)
Nonwords 17.4% (SEM: 3.4%) 82.6% (SEM: 3.4%)

Table 2
Percent of correct objective (rst answer) responses per each category (numbers in the table
cells) binned for corresponding subjective (second answer) response (table columns)

Knew Guessed
Meaningful sentences 62.4% (SEM: 9.9%) 51.7% (SEM: 2.1%)]
Nonwords 86.5% (SEM: 4.9%) 51.7% (SEM: 2.4%)

Table 3
Average z-scores and average MNI coordinates (center of mass) of ROI used in the decoding
analysis of invisible stimuli

ID Region of interest Average z-score MNI coordinates


X Y Z
1 Left angular gyrus 1.46 (SEM: 0.47) 43 73 30
2 Left supramarginal gyrus 3.35 (SEM: 0.64) 54 59 15
3 Left posterior superior temporal sulcus (STS) 5.85 (SEM: 0.47) 57 42 4
4 Left middle anterior temporal 4.67 (SEM: 0.55) 57 18 9
5 Left anterior temporal 3.9 (SEM: 0.44) 54 2 17
6 Left orbital inferior frontal gyrus 3.5 (SEM: 0.51) 48 28 3 Figure 3. Prediction rate of discrimination between subliminal meaningful sentences
7 Left inferior frontal gyrus 4.94 (SEM: 0. 64) 50 17 23 and nonwords in language network regions in the temporal lobe (A) and the frontal
8 Left middle frontal gyrus 3.72 (SEM: 0.62) 43 1 52 lobe (B). Black line is a chance level = 50%; error bars denote standard error of the
9 Left superior frontal gyrus 0.13 (SEM: 0.47) 7 54 38 mean.
10 Right posterior superior temporal sulcus (STS) 3.24 (SEM: 0.31) 59 45 8
11 Right middle anterior temporal 3.08 (SEM: 0.47) 54 14 13

ID numbers in the rst column correspond to the numbers of anatomical masks in Figure 2. The
The performance of the support vector machine in dis-
Z-score values are based on meaningful sentence > nonwords contrast in visible stimuli localizer. tinguishing subliminal meaningful sentences from nonwords
Volume of all ROIs was 100 voxels (1200 mm3). Details of how the ROIs were generated are in the parieto-temporal ROIs is shown in Figure 3A. Group-
described in the Materials and Methods section. level statistical signicance was assessed using one-tailed
t-tests against chance level of 50% (Bonferroni multiple com-
parison correction, see Materials and Methods). The only
conscious processing (knew subjective, second response) parieto-temporal region, which showed prediction signi-
since there was not sufcient data: 45% of the participants had cantly above chance was left posterior STS: 56.2% (MSE: 2%,
<5 blocks per condition of this type and the average number of t (14) = 3.01, P = 0.004). Prediction rate in the right posterior STS
blocks per participant/condition was 7.93 (MSE = 1.94). was greater than chance (53.1% [MSE: 1.6%]), but it did not
reach statistical signicance after multiple comparison correc-
tion (t (14) = 1.93, P = 0.036)]. Performance in the other ROIs
Imaging Results did not differ from chance: left supramarginal gyrus: 53.1%
The goal of our analyses was to identify brain regions where (MSE: 2.8%, t (14) = 1.1, P = 0.22), left angular gyrus: 51.6%
pattern signals were sufcient to discriminate subliminal (MSE: 2.5%, t (14) < 1), left middle anterior temporal: 49.9%
meaningful sentences from nonwords. Our multivoxel pattern (MSE: 2%, t (14) < 1), left anterior temporal: 50.7% (MSE: 2.7%,
classication analysis (MVPA) (Norman et al. 2006) approach t (14) < 1) and right middle anterior temporal: 49.1% (MSE:
focused on nodes in the language network (Fedorenko et al. 2.8%, t (14) < 1). To compare the prediction rates between hemi-
2010) identied by an independent localizer using visible spheres, for 2 regions which were localized in both hemi-
stimuli. Summary statistics (average selectivity z-score and co- spheres (the posterior STS and the middle anterior temporal
ordinates) of the ROIs is presented in the Table 3. It can be region) we ran a 2-way repeated measures ANOVA with a
seen that all the regions except for the left superior frontal region and a hemisphere as factors. The results showed signi-
gyrus showed higher activation for meaningful sentences com- cant main effect of region [F1,14 = 7.781, P = 0.014], but no sig-
pared with nonwords. Notably, the left superior frontal gyrus nicant effect of hemisphere [F1,14 < 1] and no signicant
was also among the less selective regions in the study of Fedor- interaction [F1,14 < 1] suggesting that higher prediction rate in
enko et al. (2010). It is noteworthy, that statistical contrast of the posterior STS comparing to the middle anterior temporal
visible meaningful sentences versus nonwords identies only was a property of both hemispheres.
the high-level language processing network and does not Decoding performance comparing subliminal meaningful
include inferior temporal cortex (e.g., VWFA [Cohen et al. sentences and nonwords in frontal regions is shown in Figure 3B.
2000]), which is implicated in more low-level orthographical The only region which showed prediction rate signicantly
processing (Dehaene and Cohen 2011). above chance was left middle frontal gyrus: 54.7% (MSE: 1.5%,

2164 Neural Correlates of Subliminal Language Processing Axelrod et al.



75
t(14) = 2.98, P < 0.004). In 3 other regions the prediction rate did prediction rate: 56.7%, MSE: 1.9%, t (14) = 3.6, P = 0.001)], left
not differ from chance level: left orbital inferior frontal gyrus: middle frontal gyrus [50 voxels: prediction rate: 54.2%, MSE:
53.8% (MSE: 2.6%, t(14) = 1.47, P = 0.081), left inferior frontal 1.4%, t (14) = 2.99, P = 0.004; 150 voxels: prediction rate: 56.4%,
gyrus: 49.1% (MSE: 2.2%, t(14) < 1), left superior frontal gyrus: MSE: 1.6%, t (14) = 3.97, P < 0.001]. Consistent with our main
51.4% (MSE: 2.3%, t(14) < 1]. To test whether the amount of analysis the prediction rate was also above chance in the right
unconscious information differed between the highest classi- posterior STS, but statistical signicance was not reached after
cation rate region in the temporal and frontal lobes we compared multiple comparison correction [50 voxels: prediction rate:
the prediction rates in the left posterior STS and left middle 53.7%, MSE: 1.9%, t (14) = 1.94, P = 0.04; 150 voxels: prediction
frontal gyrus. No signicant difference was found (paired t-test, rate: 55.3%, MSE: 2.2%, t (14) = 2.4, P = 0.01]. Performance did
t(14) < 1), suggesting that there is no evidence that one of the not differ from chance in left angular gyrus: [50 voxels: predic-
regions contained more information than the other. tion rate: 52.6%, MSE: 2.8%, t (14) < 1; 150 voxels: prediction
So far we have shown that it was possible to discriminate rate: 53.5%, MSE: 2.9%, t (14) = 1.19, P < 0.12], left supramargi-
between unconscious meaningful sentences and nonwords nal gyrus: [50 voxels: prediction rate: 55%, MSE: 2.2%,
based on multidimensional patterns of BOLD signals in the left t (14) = 2.29, P = 0.018; 150 voxels: prediction rate: 52%, MSE:
posterior STS and left middle frontal gyrus. Now, we asked 3.1%, t (14) < 1], left orbital inferior frontal gyrus [50 voxels: pre-
whether the 2 subliminal conditions could also be discrimi- diction rate: 53%, MSE: 2.5%, t (14) = 1.23, P = 0.12; 150 voxels:
nated based on global signal level alonethe univariate ap- prediction rate: 53.5%, MSE: 2.6%, t (14) = 1.37, P = 0.09], left
proach, which is extensively used in fMRI research. We superior frontal gyrus [50 voxels: prediction rate: 53.8%, MSE:
therefore conducted additional analyses where the classi- 2.2%, t (14) = 1.73, P = 0.05; 150 voxels: prediction rate: 50.2%,
cation was done for only one dimension, which was the MSE: 2.5%, t (14) < 1]. In left middle anterior temporal, left
average across all the voxels in the ROI. This analysis revealed anterior temporal, left inferior frontal gyrus and right middle
that across both parieto-temporal and frontal lobe regions only anterior temporal the prediction rate also did not differ from
the angular gyrus exhibited above chance prediction rate chance and was <52.5% [t (14) < 1]. The results of this analysis
(53.7%, MSE: 1.8%), but it did not reach signicance level after suggest that both the left posterior STS and the left middle
multiple comparison correction [t (14) = 2.08, P = 0.028]. In all frontal gyrus contained the information, which permitted
other regions the prediction rate did not exceed 51.5% and did reliable discrimination between meaningful sentences and
not differ from chance [t (14) < 1]. We conclude that average nonwords across different ROI sizes.
signal did not contain sufcient information for successful dis-
crimination between 2 conditions.
Finally, to ensure that the reported result was not idiosyn- Discussion
cratic for a specic ROI size, we repeated the multivariate ana- The principal nding of this study was that subliminal meaning-
lyses for the ROI size of 50 and 150 voxels. As in the main ful sentences and nonwords could be discriminated above
analysis, the signicance was assessed based on Bonferroni chance level based on BOLD signals in 2 regions: the left pos-
multiple comparison correction for each ROI size (see terior STS and the middle frontal gyrus. This supports the idea
Materials and Methods). The results of this analysis are shown that high-level language information can be processed in the
in Figure 4. Critically, for both left posterior STS and the left absence of awareness and critically provides important evidence
middle frontal gyrus the prediction rate was always signi- that unconscious language processing is not conned to
cantly above chance: left posterior STS [50 voxels: prediction occipito-temporal lobes, but also involves the human frontal
rate: 56.4%, MSE: 2.1%, t (14) = 3.02, P = 0.004; 150 voxels: lobes.

Figure 4. Discrimination between subliminal meaningful sentences and nonwords in language network regions for ROI size of 50, 100, and 150 voxels. Grey line represents a
chance level of performance (50%); error bars denote standard error of the mean.

Cerebral Cortex August 2015, V 25 N 8 2165


76
The language processing system is a widely distributed processing in general (Broca 1861). This result is apparently at
brain network, which spans large regions of the left temporal odds with the results of Diaz and McCarthy (2007) study, who
and frontal lobes (for reviews see Vigneau et al. 2006; Price did report extended activations in the left inferior frontal gyrus
2012) as well as regions in the right temporal lobe (for reviews during a subliminal semantic task. Yet, as this study did not
see Jung-Beeman 2005; Vigneau et al. 2011). Strong activation assess awareness after each trial, it is possible that invisibility
of the language network was observed in our functional locali- was not complete and the participants were aware of the sub-
zer experiment with visible stimuli, where all the regions liminal stimuli during some of the trials.
except for the superior frontal gyrus showed higher activation From a broader conscious awareness theoretical perspec-
to meaningful sentences compared with unpronounceable tive, the fact that unconscious information was successfully
nonwords (Table 3). Notably, the level of activations and the decoded from a region in the frontal lobes suggests that infor-
amount of information is drastically attenuated with uncon- mation can be processed by frontal lobes without automati-
scious stimulation. Even more relevant for the current discus- cally triggering conscious awareness. This result is consistent
sion, is that some regions like frontal lobes are usually not with rst order theories (Block 2005) and recurrent processing
activated at all during unconscious processing (for review see view (Lamme 2006; van Gaal and Lamme 2011) which do not
Dehaene and Changeux 2011)evidence that is taken to attribute special role to the frontal lobes in achieving conscious
support theoretical models, such as Global Workspace Theory awareness. The present result does not support the original
(Dehaene et al. 1998). Yet, alternatively, it can be suggested formulation of Global Workspace Theory (Dehaene et al.
that weak sensory stimulation, which is an inevitable conse- 1998), which does not expect the frontal lobes to be activated
quence of rendering stimuli invisible, is simply not sufciently by unconscious stimulation. Yet, based on the recent elabor-
strong to lead to activation in areas processing high-level infor- ation of this theory (Dehaene and Changeux 2011), our result
mation such as the frontal lobes (Haynes 2009). In the current might not contradict it either since the activity we report was
study, to increase the probability of detecting the signals in the localized in a specic region and did not span large portions of
frontal lobes we: 1) increased sensory stimulation by projecting the frontal lobe, as is the case for many conscious experiences
invisible stimuli for an extended period of time (10 s); 2) en- (Dehaene and Changeux 2011). Indeed, localized fMRI acti-
hanced linguistic processing by showing meaningful sen- vations of the frontal lobes were previously shown in uncon-
tences, which in addition to semantics also contained syntax scious cognitive control tasks (Lau and Passingham 2007; van
and structure; 3) used multivariate ROI analysis approaches Gaal et al. 2008; see also EEG ndings: van Gaal et al. 2010,
that focused on language-selective regions, which were loca- 2011). Yet, the neural systems responsible for cognitive control
lized using independent experiment with visible stimuli. Criti- and language processing are very different. Thus, the present
cally, by implementing the awareness report procedure after ndings complement current knowledge by showing that
each block we ensured that the only blocks used in our analysis unconscious language processing can also elicit localized
were subjectively and objectively invisible on a per-participant activity in the frontal lobes.
and per-block basis. We found that invisible meaningful sen- Interestingly, many previous studies using various stimuli,
tences and unpronounceable nonwords could be discriminated such as faces (e.g., Sterzer et al. 2008; Schurger et al. 2010; Fah-
beyond chance level in the left middle frontal gyrus (Fig. 3B). renfort et al. 2012) or words (Devlin et al. 2004; Nakamura et al.
The classication result was stable and did not depend on the 2005, 2007) failed to nd unconscious information in the frontal
exact size of the ROI used (Fig. 4). lobes. In the present study, we used a combination of exper-
It is noteworthy, that because none of the previous studies imental design (long stimulation duration using CFS and mean-
that have examined unconscious language processing could ingful sentences) and data analysis (MVPA) procedures, which
reliably demonstrate unconscious language activity in the has not been previously applied together. There was no way to
frontal lobes, we deliberately designed the study to maximize estimate a contribution of each one of these procedures, as our
the difference between 2 conditions (meaningful sentences vs. experiment did not include direct, within experiment compari-
nonwords). Accordingly, the current design was not intended son, between different parameters (e.g., long stimulation dur-
to answer the question what type of language information (se- ation using CFS vs. short backward masking stimulation).
mantic, syntactic, structural, and semantic context etc.) contrib- Having said that, we were able to establish that the use of multi-
uted to successful unconscious discrimination. Yet, the fact variate analysis (MVPA) played an important role in decoding
that in the conscious language experiments the left middle unconscious activity in the frontal lobes, as the successful de-
frontal gyrus has been shown to be most active in studies with coding could not be achieved using univariate analysis. Similar
sentences (Bottini et al. 1994; Baumgaertner et al. 2002), text observation was made by another study, where invisible face/
processing (Vingerhoets et al. 2003), and complex language scene stimuli could be decoded in the temporal lobe only by
material in general (for review see Vigneau et al. 2006) makes using multivariate, but not using univariate approach (Sterzer
it plausible that subliminal syntactic/structural information et al. 2008). Thus, future studies are needed to examine whether
might have contributed to successful discrimination between the use of MVPA will permit to nd unconscious information in
meaningful sentences and nonwords. This interpretation is the frontal lobes, also for the backward masking paradigms
also in line with a recent event-related potentials (ERP) study (Devlin et al. 2004; Nakamura et al. 2005, 2007).
(Batterink and Neville 2013), where using auditoryvisual at- Additional nding of the current study was that subliminal
tentional blink paradigm (Raymond et al. 1992) the authors meaningful sentences and nonwords could be discriminated
showed that consciously undetected violations in written sen- beyond chance level from signals in the left posterior STS. This
tence syntax processing elicited early frontal negativity 100 result is consistent with previous studies that also reported
400 ms. Interestingly, we found no successful decoding in the signals associated with unconscious processing of words in
orbital inferior frontal gyrus and inferior frontal gyrus regions this area (Devlin et al. 2004; Nakamura et al. 2005, 2007). Criti-
(including Brocas area)the key regions of language cally, only the present study implemented awareness report

2166 Neural Correlates of Subliminal Language Processing Axelrod et al.



77
after each block allowing characterization of subjective and ob- words sentence on one screen (Sklar et al. 2012) here we
jective invisibility. Compared with previous studies we used a propose a method of presenting invisible sentences of the un-
different methodological approach: experimental paradigm limited length. The presentation of invisible sentences for pro-
(CFS vs. backward/forward mask priming), language stimuli longed duration (e.g., 10 s in our case) gives an opportunity to
(sentences vs. words) and data analysis approach (MVPA) present not only subliminal text with much richer semantic
(Norman et al. 2006) vs. fMR-adaptation (Grill-Spector et al. information, but also permits exploration of unconscious
1999)]. Importantly, despite the differences between the neural correlates of complex language processing (e.g., syntax
studies the result in the left posterior STS was successfully re- processing). We suggest that this paradigm can be useful for
plicated, providing converging evidence and making a strong future studies in the eld.
case for the involvement of the left posterior STS in subliminal In conclusion, in the current study we demonstrated that
text processing. It should be also noted, that discrimination based on the activity in the human left posterior STS and left
rate from signals in the right posterior STS was also relatively middle frontal gyrus it was possible to discriminate between
high (Fig. 3B) and stable across ROIs of different sizes (Fig. 4), subliminally presented meaningful sentences and nonwords.
thought it did not reach statistical signicance after correction This result supports the notion that high-level language func-
for multiple comparisons. An important role of bilateral pos- tions might be processed subliminally and provides important
terior STS in semantic processing with visible stimuli has been evidence that frontal regions might be involved in unconscious
shown using various tasks and paradigms (for reviews see language processing.
Jung-Beeman 2005; Vigneau et al. 2006, 2011). As this region
was also shown to be involved in syntactic processing (e.g.,
Ben-Shachar et al. 2004) it remains to be established what type Funding
of information contributed the most to successful discrimi-
This study was supported by the Israeli Center of Research Ex-
nation of the invisible stimuli. Interestingly, in our functional
cellence in Cognitive Sciences (V.A., M.B., and G.Y.), Daniel
localizer task with visible stimuli, while the activation of the
Turnberg Travel Fellowship (V.A.) and the Wellcome Trust
left posterior STS was the strongest among all the regions
(G.R.). Funding to pay the Open Access publication charges
(Table 3, average z-score column), the activation of the right
for this article was provided by Wellcome Trust grant 100227/
posterior STS was weaker than most other regions. Thus, suc-
Z/12/Z and Wellcome Trust strategic award 091593/Z/10/Z.
cessful discrimination of invisible stimuli was not just a direct
consequence of a strong level of activation for visible stimuli
(Smith et al. 2011; Tong et al. 2012), but might also reect
regional specialization in the absence of aware processing. Notes
Interestingly, while fMRI studies including the current We thank Ev Fedorenko for assistance with establishing experimental
(Devlin et al. 2004; Nakamura et al. 2005, 2007; Diaz and paradigm and for the permission to use anatomical masks. We would
also like to thank 2 anonymous reviewers for constructive comments.
McCarthy 2007) consistently nd neural correlates of sublim-
Conict of Interest: None declared
inal meaningful text processing in the left posterior temporal
lobe, one recent ERP study, which also used CFS (Tsuchiya
and Koch 2005) failed to nd any modulation of the N400 se-
mantic component to invisible text content (Kang et al. 2011; References
see also: Vogel et al. 1998; Heyman and Moors 2012). Though Abrams RL, Greenwald AG. 2000. Parts outweigh the whole (word) in
differences in the signal measured by the 2 neuroimaging unconscious analysis of meaning. Psychol Sci. 11:118124.
Aksoy S, Haralick RM. 2001. Feature normalization and likelihood-
methods can potentially explain these different results, based
based similarity measures for image retrieval. Pattern Recognit Lett.
on our current ndings we propose an additional interpret- 22:563582.
ation. The N400 is thought to originate from multiple sources Almeida J, Mahon BZ, Nakayama K, Caramazza A. 2008. Unconscious
in the left temporal lobe (Kutas and Federmeier 2011). In the processing dissociates along categorical lines. Proc Natl Acad Sci
present study we identied 5 regions in the left temporal lobe USA. 105:1521415218.
(Fig. 2) that were all selective to meaningful text processing Almeida J, Pajtas P, Mahon B, Nakayama K, Caramazza A. 2013. Affect
of the unconscious: Visually suppressed angry faces modulate our
when stimuli were visible (Table 3). Yet, when the text was
decisions. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 13:94101.
presented subliminally out of those 5 temporal regions only Ashburner J, Friston KJ. 2005. Unied segmentation. Neuroimage.
one of them (left posterior STS) afforded successful discrimi- 26:839851.
nation of meaningful sentences from nonwords, while in the Axelrod V, Yovel G. 2012. Hierarchical processing of face viewpoint in
other temporal ROIs performance did not differ from chance human visual cortex. J Neurosci. 32:24422452.
level. Given that the N400 ERP component reects these mul- Batterink L, Neville HJ. 2013. The human brain processes syntax in the
absence of conscious awareness. J Neurosci. 33:85288533.
tiple sources (Kutas and Federmeier 2011), it is possible that
Baumgaertner A, Weiller C, Bchel C. 2002. Event-related fMRI reveals
the neural activity that originated in discriminative left pos- cortical sites involved in contextual sentence integration. Neuro-
terior STS was intermixed with the activity which originated in image. 16:736745.
neighboring, nondiscriminative regions (e.g., left middle Ben-Shachar M, Palti D, Grodzinsky Y. 2004. Neural correlates of syn-
anterior temporal, left anterior temporal). As a result, the N400 tactic movement: converging evidence from two fMRI experiments.
component recorded on the scalp was not sensitive enough to Neuroimage. 21:13201336.
reect the subliminal semantic processing. Block N. 2005. Two neural correlates of consciousness. Trends Cogn
Sci. 9:4652.
Finally, a novel methodological aspect of our work was the
Bottini G, Corcoran R, Sterzi R, Paulesu E, Schenone P, Scarpa P, Frack-
presentation of long invisible sentences. While the CFS para- owiak RS, Frith D. 1994. The role of the right hemisphere in the
digm was previously used for presenting subliminal words interpretation of gurative aspects of language A positron emission
(Costello et al. 2009; Kang et al. 2011; Yang and Yeh 2011) or 3 tomography activation study. Brain. 117:12411253.

Cerebral Cortex August 2015, V 25 N 8 2167


78
Broca P. 1861. Remarques sur le sige de la facult du langage articul, Kang M-S, Blake R, Woodman GF. 2011. Semantic analysis does not
suivies dune observation daphmie ( perte de la parole). Bull Soc occur in the absence of awareness induced by interocular suppres-
Anat. 6:330357. sion. J Neurosci. 31:1353513545.
Cohen L, Dehaene S, Naccache L, Lehricy S, Dehaene-Lambertz G, Kouider S, de Gardelle V, Sackur J, Dupoux E. 2010. How rich is con-
Hnaff M-A, Michel F. 2000. The visual word form area. Brain. sciousness? The partial awareness hypothesis. Trends Cogn Sci.
123:291307. 14:301307.
Costello P, Jiang Y, Baartman B, McGlennen K, He S. 2009. Semantic Kouider S, Dehaene S. 2007. Levels of processing during non-
and subword priming during binocular suppression. Conscious conscious perception: a critical review of visual masking. Philos
Cogn. 18:375382. Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 362:857875.
Coutanche MN. 2013. Distinguishing multi-voxel patterns and mean Kouider S, Dehaene S, Jobert A, Le Bihan D. 2007. Cerebral bases of
activation: why, how, and what does it tell us? Cogn Affect Behav subliminal and supraliminal priming during reading. Cereb Cortex.
Neurosci. 13:667673. 17:20192029.
Coutanche MN, Thompson-Schill SL. 2012. The advantage of brief Kutas M, Federmeier KD. 2011. Thirty years and counting: nding
fMRI acquisition runs for multi-voxel pattern detection across runs. meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain poten-
Neuroimage. 61:11131119. tial (ERP). Annu Rev Psychol. 62:621647.
Dehaene S, Changeux J-P. 2011. Experimental and theoretical ap- Lamme VAF. 2006. Towards a true neural stance on consciousness.
proaches to conscious processing. Neuron. 70:200227. Trends Cogn Sci. 10:494501.
Dehaene S, Cohen L. 2011. The unique role of the visual word form Lau HC, Passingham RE. 2007. Unconscious activation of the cognitive
area in reading. Trends Cogn Sci. 15:254262. control system in the human prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci.
Dehaene S, Kerszberg M, Changeux J-P. 1998. A neuronal model of a 27:58055811.
global workspace in effortful cognitive tasks. Proc Natl Acad Sci Lin Z, He S. 2009. Seeing the invisible: the scope and limits of uncon-
USA. 95:1452914534. scious processing in binocular rivalry. Prog Neurobiol. 87:195211.
Dehaene S, Naccache L, Cohen L, Bihan DL, Mangin J-F, Poline J-B, Marcel AJ. 1983. Conscious and unconscious perception: experiments
Riviere D. 2001. Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and uncon- on visual masking and word recognition. Cogn Psychol. 15:197237.
scious repetition priming. Nat Neurosci. 4:752758. Mendola JD, Conner IP. 2007. Eye dominance predicts fMRI signals in
Devlin JT, Jamison HL, Matthews PM, Gonnerman LM. 2004. Mor- human retinotopic cortex. Neurosci Lett. 414:3034.
phology and the internal structure of words. Proc Natl Acad Sci Meyer K, Kaplan JT, Essex R, Webber C, Damasio H, Damasio A. 2010.
USA. 101:1498414988. Predicting visual stimuli on the basis of activity in auditory cortices.
Diaz MT, McCarthy G. 2007. Unconscious word processing engages a Nat Neurosci. 13:667668.
distributed network of brain regions. J Cogn Neurosci. 19: Miles WR. 1930. Ocular dominance in human adults. J Gen Psychol.
17681775. 3:412430.
Eger E, Ashburner J, Haynes JD, Dolan RJ, Rees G. 2008. fMRI activity Misaki M, Kim Y, Bandettini PA, Kriegeskorte N. 2010. Comparison of
patterns in human LOC carry information about object exemplars multivariate classiers and response normalizations for pattern-
within category. J Cogn Neurosci. 20:356370. information fMRI. Neuroimage. 53:103118.
Fahrenfort JJ, Snijders TM, Heinen K, van Gaal S, Scholte HS, Lamme Nakamura K, Dehaene S, Jobert A, Bihan DL, Kouider S. 2005. Sublim-
VAF. 2012. Neuronal integration in visual cortex elevates face cat- inal convergence of Kanji and Kana words: further evidence for
egory tuning to conscious face perception. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. functional parcellation of the posterior temporal cortex in visual
109:2150421509. word perception. J Cogn Neurosci. 17:954968.
Faivre N, Berthet V, Kouider S. 2012. Nonconscious inuences from Nakamura K, Dehaene S, Jobert A, Le Bihan D, Kouider S. 2007. Task-
emotional faces: a comparison of visual crowding, masking, and specic change of unconscious neural priming in the cerebral
continuous ash suppression. Front Psychol. 3:129. language network. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 104:1964319648.
Fedorenko E, Behr MK, Kanwisher N. 2011. Functional specicity for Nestor A, Plaut DC, Behrmann M. 2011. Unraveling the distributed
high-level linguistic processing in the human brain. Proc Natl Acad neural code of facial identity through spatiotemporal pattern analy-
Sci USA. 108:1642816433. sis. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 108:999810003.
Fedorenko E, Hsieh P-J, Nieto-Castan A, Whiteld-Gabrieli S, Kanw- Norman KA, Polyn SM, Detre GJ, Haxby JV. 2006. Beyond mind-
isher N. 2010. New method for fMRI investigations of language: de- reading: multi-voxel pattern analysis of fMRI data. Trends Cogn Sci.
ning ROIs functionally in individual subjects. J Neurophysiol. 10:424430.
104:11771194. Price CJ. 2012. A review and synthesis of the rst 20 years of PET and
Fedorenko E, Nieto-Castaon A, Kanwisher N. 2012. Lexical and syn- fMRI studies of heard speech, spoken language and reading. Neu-
tactic representations in the brain: an fMRI investigation with multi- roimage. 62:816847.
voxel pattern analyses. Neuropsychologia. 50:499513. Raizada RD, Tsao F-M, Liu H-M, Holloway ID, Ansari D, Kuhl PK.
Grill-Spector K, Kushnir T, Edelman S, Avidan G, Itzchak Y, Malach R. 2010. Linking brain-wide multivoxel activation patterns to behav-
1999. Differential processing of objects under various viewing con- iour: examples from language and math. Neuroimage. 51:462471.
ditions in the human lateral occipital complex. Neuron. 24:187203. Raymond JE, Shapiro KL, Arnell KM. 1992. Temporary suppression of
Haynes J-D. 2009. Decoding visual consciousness from human brain visual processing in an RSVP task: an attentional blink. J Exp
signals. Trends Cogn Sci. 13:194202. Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 18:849860.
Heyman T, Moors P. 2012. Using interocular suppression and EEG to Said CP, Moore CD, Engell AD, Todorov A, Haxby JV. 2010. Distributed
study semantic processing. J Neurosci. 32:15151516. representations of dynamic facial expressions in the superior tem-
Holender D. 1986. Semantic activation without conscious identication poral sulcus. J Vis. 10:112.
in dichotic listening, parafoveal vision, and visual masking: a Schurger A, Pereira F, Treisman A, Cohen JD. 2010. Reproducibility dis-
survey and appraisal. Behav Brain Sci. 9:123. tinguishes conscious from nonconscious neural representations.
Japkowicz N, Stephen S. 2002. The class imbalance problem: a sys- Science. 327:9799.
tematic study. Intell Data Anal. 6:429449. Serences JT, Saproo S, Scolari M, Ho T, Muftuler LT. 2009. Estimating the
Jiang Y, Costello P, He S. 2007. Processing of invisible stimuli: advan- inuence of attention on population codes in human visual cortex
tage of upright faces and recognizable words in overcoming intero- using voxel-based tuning functions. Neuroimage. 44:223231.
cular suppression. Psychol Sci. 18:349355. Seth AK, Dienes Z, Cleeremans A, Overgaard M, Pessoa L. 2008.
Jung-Beeman M. 2005. Bilateral brain processes for comprehending Measuring consciousness: relating behavioural and neurophysiolo-
natural language. Trends Cogn Sci. 9:512518. gical approaches. Trends Cogn Sci. 12:314321.
Kanai R, Walsh V, Tseng C-h. 2010. Subjective discriminability of invisi- Sklar AY, Levy N, Goldstein A, Mandel R, Maril A, Hassin RR. 2012.
bility: a framework for distinguishing perceptual and attentional Reading and doing arithmetic nonconsciously. Proc Natl Acad Sci
failures of awareness. Conscious Cogn. 19:10451057. USA. 109:1961419619.

2168 Neural Correlates of Subliminal Language Processing Axelrod et al.



79
Smith AT, Kosillo P, Williams AL. 2011. The confounding effect of Vigneau M, Beaucousin V, Herv PY, Duffau H, Crivello F, Houd O,
response amplitude on MVPA performance measures. Neuroimage. Mazoyer B, Tzourio-Mazoyer N. 2006. Meta-analyzing left hemi-
56:525530. sphere language areas: Phonology, semantics, and sentence proces-
Sterzer P, Haynes J-D, Rees G. 2008. Fine-scale activity patterns in high- sing. Neuroimage. 30:14141432.
level visual areas encode the category of invisible objects. J Vis. 8:112. Vigneau M, Beaucousin V, Herv P-Y, Jobard G, Petit L, Crivello F,
Tong F, Harrison SA, Dewey JA, Kamitani Y. 2012. Relationship between Mellet E, Zago L, Mazoyer B, Tzourio-Mazoyer N. 2011. What is
BOLD amplitude and pattern classication of orientation-selective right-hemisphere contribution to phonological, lexico-semantic,
activity in the human visual cortex. Neuroimage. 63:12121222. and sentence processing? Insights from a meta-analysis. Neuro-
Tsuchiya N, Koch C. 2005. Continuous ash suppression reduces nega- image. 54:577593.
tive afterimages. Nat Neurosci. 8:10961101. Vingerhoets G, Borsel JV, Tesink C, van den Noort M, Deblaere K, Seur-
Van den Bussche E, Van den Noortgate W, Reynvoet B. 2009. Mechan- inck R, Vandemaele P, Achten E. 2003. Multilingualism: an fMRI
isms of masked priming: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 135:452477. study. Neuroimage. 20:21812196.
van Gaal S, Lamme VA, Fahrenfort JJ, Ridderinkhof KR. 2011. Dissoci- Vogel EK, Luck SJ, Shapiro KL. 1998. Electrophysiological evi-
able brain mechanisms underlying the conscious and unconscious dence for a postperceptual locus of suppression during the at-
control of behavior. J Cogn Neurosci. 23:91105. tentional blink. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform.
van Gaal S, Lamme VAF. 2011. Unconscious high-level information pro- 24:16561674.
cessing: implication for neurobiological theories of consciousness. Walther DB, Caddigan E, Fei-Fei L, Beck DM. 2009. Natural scene cat-
The Neuroscientist. 18:287301. egories revealed in distributed patterns of activity in the human
van Gaal S, Ridderinkhof KR, Fahrenfort JJ, Scholte HS, Lamme VA. brain. J Neurosci. 29:1057310581.
2008. Frontal cortex mediates unconsciously triggered inhibitory Wentura D, Kulfanek M, Greve W. 2005. Masked affective priming by
control. J Neurosci. 28:80538062. name letters: evidence for a correspondence of explicit and implicit
van Gaal S, Ridderinkhof KR, Scholte HS, Lamme VA. 2010. Uncon- self-esteem. J Exp Soc Psychol. 41:654663.
scious activation of the prefrontal no-go network. J Neurosci. Yang Y-H, Yeh S-L. 2011. Accessing the meaning of invisible words.
30:41434150. Conscious Cogn. 20:223233.

Cerebral Cortex August 2015, V 25 N 8 2169


80
On Making the Right Choice: The
Deliberation-Without-Attention Effect
Ap Dijksterhuis, et al.
Science 311, 1005 (2006);
DOI: 10.1126/science.1121629

The following resources related to this article are available online at


www.sciencemag.org (this information is current as of May 27, 2007 ):

Updated information and services, including high-resolution figures, can be found in the online
version of this article at:
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/311/5763/1005

Supporting Online Material can be found at:


http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/311/5763/1005/DC1

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on May 27, 2007


A list of selected additional articles on the Science Web sites related to this article can be
found at:
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/311/5763/1005#related-content
This article cites 13 articles, 2 of which can be accessed for free:
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/311/5763/1005#otherarticles

This article has been cited by 11 article(s) on the ISI Web of Science.

This article has been cited by 3 articles hosted by HighWire Press; see:
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/311/5763/1005#otherarticles

This article appears in the following subject collections:


Psychology
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/collection/psychology

Information about obtaining reprints of this article or about obtaining permission to reproduce
this article in whole or in part can be found at:
http://www.sciencemag.org/about/permissions.dtl

Science (print ISSN 0036-8075; online ISSN 1095-9203) is published weekly, except the last week in December, by the
American Association for the Advancement of Science, 1200 New York Avenue NW, Washington, DC 20005. Copyright
c 2006 by the American Association for the Advancement of Science; all rights reserved. The title SCIENCE is a
registered trademark of AAAS.
81
REPORTS
13. E. J. Eide, H. Kang, S. Crapo, M. Gallego, D. M. Virshup, 20. A. Balsalobre, F. Damiola, U. Schibler, Cell 93, 929 (1998). 30. We thank the University of Pennsylvania Diabetes and
Methods Enzymol. 393, 408 (2005). 21. F. Zhang, C. J. Phiel, L. Spece, N. Gurvich, P. S. Klein, Endocrinology Research Center Vector Core (NIH DK
14. S. Martinek, S. Inonog, A. S. Manoukian, M. W. Young, J. Biol. Chem. 278, 33067 (2003). 19525) and M. J. Birnbaum for GSK3b adenoviruses,
Cell 105, 769 (2001). 22. A. J. Gelenberg et al., J. Med. 321, 1489 (1989). and J. D. Alvarez and A. Sehgal for helpful discussions.
15. M. Abe, E. D. Herzog, G. D. Block, Neuroreport 11, 3261 23. J. A. Ripperger, L. P. Shearman, S. M. Reppert, Supported by NIH DK45586 (to M.A.L.) and NIH
(2000). U. Schibler, Genes Dev. 14, 679 (2000). MH058324 (to P.S.K.).
16. C. Iitaka, K. Miyazaki, T. Akaike, N. Ishida, J. Biol. Chem. 24. E. Iwahana et al., Eur. J. Neurosci. 19, 2281 (2004).
280, 29397 (2005). 25. F. Benedetti et al., Neurosci. Lett. 368, 123 (2004). Supporting Online Material
17. P. S. Klein, D. A. Melton, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93, 26. R. H. Lenox, T. D. Gould, H. K. Manji, Am. J. Med. Genet. www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/311/5763/1002/DC1
8455 (1996). 114, 391 (2002). Materials and Methods
18. G. Adelmant, A. Begue, D. Stehelin, V. Laudet, Proc. Natl. 27. M. Gottlicher et al., EMBO J. 20, 6969 (2001). Figs. S1 to S7
Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93, 3553 (1996). 28. C. J. Phiel et al., J. Biol. Chem. 276, 36734 (2001).
19. B. W. Doble, J. R. Woodgett, J. Cell Sci. 116, 1175 29. M. E. Dokucu, L. Yu, P. H. Taghert, Neuropsychopharma- 20 October 2005; accepted 14 January 2006
(2003). cology 30, 2216 (2005). 10.1126/science.1121613

On Making the Right Choice: The hardly developed beyond the status of Bfolk
wisdom.[ It has been postulated or investigated
by scientists infrequently Ebut see (1013)^. The
Deliberation-Without-Attention Effect question addressed here is whether this view is
justified. We hypothesize that it is not.

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on May 27, 2007


Ap Dijksterhuis,* Maarten W. Bos, Loran F. Nordgren, Rick B. van Baaren First, conscious thought does not always
lead to sound choices. For example, participants
Contrary to conventional wisdom, it is not always advantageous to engage in thorough conscious who chose their favorite poster among a set of
deliberation before choosing. On the basis of recent insights into the characteristics of conscious five after thorough contemplation showed less
and unconscious thought, we tested the hypothesis that simple choices (such as between different postchoice satisfaction than participants who
towels or different sets of oven mitts) indeed produce better results after conscious thought, but only looked at them briefly (14, 15). Further-
that choices in complex matters (such as between different houses or different cars) should be left more, conscious deliberation can make multiple
to unconscious thought. Named the deliberation-without-attention hypothesis, it was confirmed evaluations of the same object less consistent
in four studies on consumer choice, both in the laboratory as well as among actual shoppers, that over time (16). Two reasons why conscious
purchases of complex products were viewed more favorably when decisions had been made in the deliberation sometimes leads to poor judgments
absence of attentive deliberation. have been identified. First, consciousness has
a low capacity (17, 18), causing choosers to take

C
ommon knowledge holds that thor- after 48 hours of not thinking about it con- into account only a subset of the relevant
ough conscious thought leads to good sciously, suddenly the thought BIt_s going to be information when they decide (13, 19). Second,
decisions and satisfactory choices. Tuscany![ pops into consciousness. This thought conscious thought can lead to suboptimal weight-
Whether purchasing a new car, a desktop com- itself is conscious, but the transition from ing of the importance of attributes (1316):
puter, or a pair of shoes, people generally indecision to a preference 2 days later is the We tend to inflate the importance of some at-
believe that serious conscious deliberation in- result of unconscious thought, or of deliberation tributes at the expense of others, leading to
creases the probability that they will make the without attention. worse choices.
Bright[ choice. This idea applies especially to The scientific literature has emphasized the Conversely, unconscious thought, or thought
choices between products that are complex, benefits of conscious deliberation in decision without attention, can lead to good choices
multifaceted, and expensive. Whereas most making for hundreds of years (2, 3). The idea (13, 14). In a recent experiment, participants
people are willing to buy a new set of towels that conscious deliberation is the ideal (if not read information about four apartments of dif-
without much thought, they are unlikely to buy always attainable) way to approach a decision ferent desirability (20). They were either asked
a new car or outfit a new kitchen without forms the backbone of classic (4, 5) as well as to choose their favorite immediately, or given
deliberation. contemporary perspectives on decision making the opportunity to choose after a period of
A second pervasive idea is that the quality (6, 7) and attitude formation (8, 9). In contrast, conscious thought, or distracted for some time
of a choice benefits from Bsleeping on it.[ the notion that unconscious thought is fruitful
Rather than (or in addition to) thinking con-
sciously, people usually feel that Bunconscious
thought[ is useful for making sound decisions.
Whereas conscious thought refers to thought or
deliberation while conscious attention is di-
rected at the problem at hand, unconscious
thought can be defined as thought or delibera-
tion in the absence of conscious attention di-
rected at the problem (1). An example of
unconscious thought is the following: One
compares two holiday destinations (say the
Costa Brava and Tuscany) and does not know
what to decide. One puts the problem aside and
Fig. 1. Percentage of participants who chose the Fig. 2. Difference in attitude (on a scale of 25 to
Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, most desirable car as a function of complexity of 25) toward the desirable and undesirable car as a
Roetersstraat 15, 1018 WB, Amsterdam, the Netherlands. decision and of mode of thought (n 0 18 to 22 in function of complexity of decision and of mode of
*To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: each condition). Error bars represent the stan- thought (n 0 12 to 14 in each condition). Error
a.j.dijksterhuis@uva.nl dard error. bars represent the standard error.

www.sciencemag.org 82 VOL 311


SCIENCE 17 FEBRUARY 2006 1005
REPORTS
before they chose. In the third of these con- houses) is complex. Conscious thought is hy- ences between conditions (F G 1, not significant).
ditions, participants could only engage in pothesized, due to its precision, to lead to good Conscious thinkers generally made the proper
unconscious deliberation: They knew they choices in simple matters. However, because of choice under simple conditions, but performed
would have to choose later, but the distraction its low capacity, conscious thought leads to poorly under complex circumstances EF(1,40) 0
task prevented them from devoting conscious progressively worse choices with more com- 4.95, P G 0.04^.
attention to the choice. Interestingly, un- plex issues. Unconscious thought (i.e., deliber- Study 2. For the second study we made one
conscious thinkers made better decisions than ation without attention) is expected, because of change (25). Rather than asking for a choice,
conscious thinkers or than immediate choosers its relative lack of precision, to lead to choices we asked participants about their attitudes
(13, 14). of lower quality. However, the quality of choice toward each of the four cars. As the dependent
Recently, we formulated the Unconscious does not deteriorate with increased complexity, variable, we used the difference in attitude
Thought Theory (UTT) (21) about the strengths allowing unconscious thought to lead to better toward the best car and the worst car. Again,
and weaknesses of conscious thought and un- choices than conscious thought under com- conscious thinkers were better able to differen-
conscious thought, that is, of deliberation with plex circumstances, this latter idea being the tiate the quality of the cars under simple
and without attention. Two characteristics of kernel of the deliberation-without-attention conditions, whereas unconscious thinkers were
conscious and unconscious thought are impor- hypothesis. Quality of choice was operation- better able to differentiate the quality of the cars
tant in the current context. First, conscious alized both normatively (studies 1 and 2) as under complex conditions EF(1,47) 0 5.63, P G
thought is rule-based and very precise (22, 23). well as subjectively (as postchoice satisfaction, 0.03^. The means are shown in Fig. 2.
Unconscious thought can conform to rules in in studies 3 and 4). Study 3. In a pilot study, undergraduate
that it detects recurring patterns, as the literature Study 1. Participants were subjected to a 2 students were asked how many aspects of a

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on May 27, 2007


on implicit learning shows (24). However, in (mode of thought: conscious versus uncon- product they would take into account in the
order to actively follow strict rules, conscious scious) ! 2 (complexity of choice problem: sim- purchase of 40 different products. In this way,
attention is necessary. For example, one cannot ple versus complex) factorial design (25). All we obtained an average Bcomplexity score[
do arithmetic without conscious attention. This participants read information about four hypo- for 40 different products (supporting online
capacity to follow rules makes conscious thought thetical cars. Depending on the condition, each text).
more precise in decision making, because it car was characterized by 4 attributes (simple) or For the actual study, other students were
can strictly follow self-generated rules such as by 12 attributes (complex). The attributes were presented with this list of 40 products. From the
not exceeding a maximum price. Second, as either positive or negative. One car was char- list, they were asked to choose a product that
alluded to earlier, conscious thought suffers acterized by 75% positive attributes, two by they had recently purchased and were asked the
from the low capacity of consciousness, mak- 50% positive attributes, and one by 25% posi- following questions: Which product did you
ing it less suitable for very complex issues. tive attributes (supporting online text). After purchase? Did you know the product before
Unconscious thought does not suffer from low reading the information about the four cars, you went on the shopping trip? How much did
capacity. Indeed, it has been shown that during participants were assigned either to a conscious you think about the product between seeing it
unconscious thought, large amounts of infor- thought condition or to an unconscious thought for the first time and buying it? How satisfied
mation can be integrated into an evaluative condition. In the conscious thought condition, are you with the product?
summary judgment (13). participants were asked to think about the cars To test our hypothesis, we distinguished
These characteristics of conscious and un- for 4 min before they chose their favorite car. participants who thought (either consciously or
conscious thought led us to postulate the In the unconscious thought condition, partic- unconsciously) about their purchase from im-
Bdeliberation-without-attention[ hypothesis, on ipants were distracted for 4 min (they solved pulse buyers who did not think much at all.
the relation between mode of thought or anagrams) and were told that after the period of Hence, participants who indicated that they
deliberation (conscious versus unconscious) distraction they would be asked to choose the bought a product they had never come across
and the complexity and quality of choice. best car. before the shopping trip were not included,
Complexity is defined as the amount of in- The percentages of participants who chose leaving only participants who knew the product
formation a choice involves. A choice between the best car are shown in Fig. 1. The crucial two- beforehand (n 0 49).
objects for which one or two attributes are way interaction supporting the deliberation- It is impossible to know whether people are
important (such as oven mitts or toothpaste) is without-attention hypothesis was significant engaged in unconscious thought by asking
simple, whereas a choice between objects for EF(1,76) 0 4.85, P G 0.04^. Unconscious think- them, so strictly speaking, we can only test
which many attributes are important (cars or ers fared relatively well and showed no differ- the relationship between conscious thought,

Fig. 3. The relation between mode of


thought and postchoice satisfaction
(on a scale of 1 to 7) for the six
products most frequently chosen in
study 3. Higher bars indicate more
satisfaction. The more complex the
product (on a scale of 1 to 5), the fur-
ther to the right it is shown. The
complexity score is given in parenthe-
ses. Participants were divided into
conscious and unconscious thinkers
on the basis of a median-split for
each product individually. Each bar Fig. 4. Postchoice satisfaction of IKEA (n 0 27)
represents between two and five and Bijenkorf (n 0 27) shoppers as a function
participants. of mode of thought. Error bars represent the
standard error.

1006 17 FEBRUARY 2006 VOL 311 83


SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org
REPORTS
complexity, and quality. However, it follows products) EF(1,25) 0 6.52, P G 0.02^. The oppo- 9. R. E. Petty, J. T. Cacioppo, in Advances in Experimental
from our definition of conscious and uncon- site was true for the IKEA customers (complex Social Psychology, L. Berkowitz, Ed. (Academic Press,
New York, 1986), vol. 19, pp. 123205.
scious thought (according to which attention products), in which case unconscious thinkers 10. As far as we know, the possibility of unconscious thought
to the problem at hand is the crucial distin- showed more postchoice satisfaction than con- (as well as the term unconscious thought) was explicitly
guishing factor) that they are at least partly scious thinkers EF(1,25) 0 6.12, P G 0.02^ used for the first time by Schopenhauer, who remarked
dependent. At any one point in time, attention (Fig. 4). that One might almost believe that half of our thinking
takes place unconsciously (27).
is either directed at the decision under con- In sum, in four studies we demonstrated the 11. G. Claxton, Hare Brain, Tortoise Mind: How Intelligence
sideration, or it is not; that is, at any partic- deliberation-without-attention effect. Conscious Increases When You Think Less (Harper Collins, New York,
ular point in time, either you are attending to thinkers were better able to make the best 1997).
buying a car, or you are not. The more you choice among simple products, whereas un- 12. The idea of unconscious thought was also studied by
various researchers interested in incubation processes in
think about a decision consciously (that is, conscious thinkers were better able to make the creativity. An example is K. S. Bowers et al. (28).
with attention), the less time remains to think best choice among complex products. Among 13. A. Dijksterhuis, J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87, 586 (2004).
about the same decision unconsciously (that people who knew the product they purchased 14. A. Dijksterhuis, Z. van Olden, J. Exp. Soc. Psychol.,
is, without attention). before they went on a shopping trip, the amount in press.
15. T. D. Wilson et al., Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 19, 331
We regressed the amount of thought and the of conscious thought was positively related to
(1993).
average number of aspects on postchoice satis- postchoice satisfaction for simple products and 16. G. M. Levine, J. B. Halberstadt, R. L. Goldstone, J. Pers.
faction. As expected, thinking does not make negatively related to postchoice satisfaction for Soc. Psychol. 70, 230 (1996).
people more satisfied, nor does complexity complex products. 17. G. A. Miller, Psych. Rev. 63, 81 (1956).
(t_s G 1). However, the interaction of the two Our aim was to test the Bdeliberation- 18. T. Nrretranders, The User Illusion: Cutting Consciousness

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on May 27, 2007


Down to Size (Viking, New York, 1998).
parameters significantly predicted postchoice without-attention[ hypothesis both in the labo- 19. T. D. Wilson, J. W. Schooler, J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 60, 181
satisfaction Et(48) 0 2.13, P G 0.04^. Correla- ratory and among shoppers. In that sense, it is (1991).
tions were calculated between amount of important to view our set of studies as a whole 20. Quality of decision was operationalized from a
thought and postchoice satisfaction for three rather than as a series of individual studies. normative perspective. One of the choice options was
made more desirable than the others because it
categories of products: complex, medium, and Study 4 has unavoidable disadvantages such as had been assigned more positive aspects than the
simple. For products of medium complexity, no that the IKEA and Bijenkorf samples may have other apartments.
correlation was found Er(18) 0 0.03^; for differed (after all, different shops attract a 21. A. Dijksterhuis, L. F. Nordgren, Persp. Psychol. Sci.,
simple products, a positive correlation was different clientele), which naturally opens the in press.
22. The important distinction between following rules and
found Er(15) 0 0.57, P G 0.03^; and for complex potential for alternative explanations. There- merely conforming to them (and the need for conscious
products, a negative correlation was found fore, study 3 was done in order to Bbridge[ the attention in the former) was made by S. A. Sloman
Er(16) 0 0.56, P G 0.03^. As expected, the laboratory studies with study 4. It has many of (29).
more people thought consciously about simple the assets of study 4 (real choices between real 23. E. R. Smith, J. DeCoster, Dual-Process Theories in
Social Psychology, S. Chaiken, Y. Trope, Eds. (Guilford,
products, the more satisfied they were with products with satisfaction as the dependent
New York, 1999), pp. 323336.
their purchase. Conversely, the more people variable), except that all participants were 24. Unconscious thought is reminiscent of implicit learning,
thought consciously about complex products, students. but there is an important difference. Implicit learning
the less satisfied they were with their purchase. Although we investigated choices among refers to aspects of a task that are learned while
Figure 3 depicts satisfaction as a function of consumer products in our studies, there is no a working on the task (and that are inaccessible to
consciousness). Unconscious thought refers to thought
mode of thought for the six most frequently priori reason to assume that the deliberation- processes that take place after the encoding of relevant
chosen products (26). without-attention effect does not generalize to information. A good example of this definition of
Study 4. On the basis of the pilot study to other types of choicespolitical, managerial, unconscious thought is the groundbreaking work by
study 3, two shops were selected: one where or otherwise. In such cases, it should benefit Stickgold and colleagues on learning during sleep.
See, e.g., (30, 31).
people generally buy complex products (IKEA, the individual to think consciously about 25. Materials and methods are available as supporting
which sells mainly furniture) and one where simple matters and to delegate thinking about material on Science Online.
people generally buy simple products (Bijenkorf, more complex matters to the unconscious. 26. We found a correlation between number of aspects and
a department store like Macy_s that sells amount of thought (r 0 0.54, P G 0.001): The more
References and Notes complex a product is, the more people think consciously
clothes, clothing accessories, and kitchen ac- when deciding to purchase it. Understandable as this
1. It is important to note that attention to the problem at
cessories). At the exit, shoppers were asked the hand is the crucial distinction in our definitions of conscious may be, our analysis suggests that people should do the
following questions: What did you buy? How and unconscious thought. Thinking about buying a new opposite, i.e., think unconsciously when deciding to
expensive was it? Did you know the product car while attention is directed at possible new cars is purchase a complex product. The correlation between
conscious thought. Thinking about buying a new car number of aspects and price was also significant
before you went on the shopping trip? and (r 0 0.45, P G 0.001): Expensive products were more
while attention is temporarily directed elsewhere is
How much did you think about the product complex than inexpensive ones.
unconscious thought. This distinction does not mean that
between seeing it for the first time and buying conscious thought only comprises conscious processes. 27. A. Schopenhauer, in Essays and Aphorisms (Penguin,
it? A few weeks later, the shoppers were asked One can compare it to speech. Speech is a conscious London, 1851/1970), p. 123.
process (i.e., attention is directed at it while one speaks), 28. K. S. Bowers, G. Regehr, C. Balthazard, K. Parker,
(over the phone) how satisfied they were with
but it is in part dependent on accompanying unconscious Cogn. Psychol. 22, 72 (1990).
their purchases. As in study 3, participants who 29. S. A. Sloman, Psychol. Bull. 119, 3 (1996).
processes (such as processes responsible for syntax or
indicated that they bought a product they had word choice). 30. R. Stickgold, M. Walker, Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 191 (2004).
never come across before the shopping trip 2. R. Descartes, The Philosophical Writings of Descartes 31. R. Stickgold et al., Science 294, 1052 (2001).
were not included. (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, 1984). 32. We thank E. Neimeijer, L. Schreers, and R. Wassenberg
3. J. Locke, An Essay Concerning Human Understanding for help with conducting study 4. This research was
We divided participants (Bthinkers[) on the supported by a grant from Nederlandse Organisatie voor
(Penguin, London, 1689/1997).
basis of a median-split procedure into those 4. I. L. Janis, L. Mann, Decision Making: A Psychological Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek (016.025.030).
who engaged in much conscious thought Analysis of Conflict, Choice, and Commitment (Free
(conscious thinkers) and those who engaged Press, New York, 1977). Supporting Online Material
in little conscious thought (unconscious think- 5. H. A. Simon, Q. J. Econ. 69, 99 (1955). www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/311/5763/1005/DC1
6. J. R. Bettman, M. F. Luce, J. W. Payne, J. Consum. Res. 25, Materials and Methods
ers). As expected, conscious thinkers reported 187 (1998).
more postchoice satisfaction than uncon- 7. D. Kahneman, Am. Psychol. 58, 697 (2003). 20 October 2005; accepted 9 January 2006
scious thinkers for Bijenkorf products (simple 8. S. Chaiken, J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 39, 752 (1980). 10.1126/science.1121629

www.sciencemag.org 84 VOL 311


SCIENCE 17 FEBRUARY 2006 1007
ARTICLES

Probabilistic word pre-activation during language


2005 Nature Publishing Group http://www.nature.com/natureneuroscience

comprehension inferred from electrical brain activity


Katherine A DeLong1, Thomas P Urbach1 & Marta Kutas1,2

Despite the numerous examples of anticipatory cognitive processes at micro and macro levels in many animal species, the idea
that anticipation of specific words plays an integral role in real-time language processing has been contentious. Here we exploited
a phonological regularity of English indefinite articles (an precedes nouns beginning with vowel sounds, whereas a precedes
nouns beginning with consonant sounds) in combination with event-related brain potential recordings from the human scalp to
show that readers brains can pre-activate individual words in a graded fashion to a degree that can be estimated from the
probability that each word is given as a continuation for a sentence fragment offline. These findings are evidence that readers
use the words in a sentence (as cues to their world knowledge) to estimate relative likelihoods for upcoming words.

Despite the variety of real-time processing domains across the phylo- offline cloze probabilities18. An items cloze probability is the percen-
genetic spectrum in which anticipatory processing has been observed (at tage of individuals that continue a sentence fragment with that item in
both micro and macro levels), the concept of anticipation has played a an offline sentence completion task. Despite the sensitivity of the N400
relatively minor role in language processing theories. Human languages in response to offline semantic expectancy, it is impossible to determine
offer unlimited possibilities not only for saying new things but also for whether variation in N400 amplitude in response to eliciting words
saying old things in new waysfar too many ways, some have argued, during online, real-time sentence processing means that comprehen-
to make prediction of words a viable and effective strategy except when ders are using context to generate expectancies for upcoming items
contextual constraint is unusually high1. Accordingly, early language (prediction view) or whether they are forced by the words themselves to
processing models often included some form of memory buffer wherein devote more or fewer resources to integrating words into sentence
sentential elements were temporarily stored for later integration at representations (integration view).
phrasal, clausal or sentence boundaries26. Since the 1970s, however, the Tracking prediction in sentence processing requires a measure that
consensus view has been that sentence processing is continuous and has high temporal resolution (such as ERPs, magnetoencephalogram or
incremental, with provisional commitments made that at least tem- eye movements) and does not alter the comprehension process under
porarily resolve linguistic ambiguities as each word is processed upon its study. Additionally, it calls for a design that precludes interpretation in
occurrence and rapidly integrated into the sentence representation716. terms of integrative difficulty. A few recent ERP and eye-tracking studies
More recently, a few researchers have argued for the predictive power have demonstrated contextually generated expectancies for semantic or
of context in generating expectancies during sentence processing, but it syntactic features of upcoming words1927. None, however, has demon-
has proven difficult to distinguish prediction from integration. What strated contextual generation of expectancies for specific word forms in
some researchers take as evidence for neural pre-activation (prediction, semantically meaningful, syntactically well-formed sentences.
at a psychological level), others take as a sign of the ease or difficulty in To this end, we designed an experiment which capitalized on the
integrating words into message-level representations upon, but not phonological regularity in English whereby the singular indefinite
before, their occurrence. A case in point is the N400 component article meaning some one thing is phonologically realized as an
observed in event-related brain potential (ERP) studies, in which before words beginning with a vowel sound and a before words
cortical neuronal ensembles generate potentials measurable at the scalp. beginning with a consonant sound (for example, an airplane and a
The N400 (B200500 ms postitem onset) is the brains neural kite). To determine whether comprehenders pre-activate specific
response to any potentially meaningful item. Its amplitude is sensitive articles and nouns before their occurrence, we used sentences of varying
to word frequency, repetition and concreteness, among other factors. constraint that led to expectations for particular consonant- or vowel-
The N400 is especially large to nouns that do not meaningfully fit with initial nouns. Across sentences, target nouns ranged from highly
their preceding contexts17. However, N400s also characterize responses probable to unlikely, based on offline cloze probability norming. For
to all but the most highly expected nouns, even when they fit instance, given The day was breezy so the boy went outside to flyy,
contextually, with amplitudes inversely related (r E !0.9) to their the most likely continuation was a kite (cloze of a 86%, kite

1Department of Cognitive Science and 2Department of Neurosciences, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093, USA. Correspondence should be

addressed to K.A.D. (kadelong@cogsci.ucsd.edu).


Published online 10 July 2005; doi:10.1038/nn1504

NATURE NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 8 [ NUMBER 8 [ AUGUST 2005 1117


85
ARTICLES

89%). However, the sentence could continue with a plausible, though RESULTS
less likely, alternative such as an airplane. Based on previous studies we We obtained offline probabilities for all article and noun targets.
knew that the N400 in response to kite would be smaller than that to Participants were asked to provide the best continuations for sentences
airplane, and more generally that noun N400 amplitude would be truncated before the article or noun. Article cloze ranged from 096%;
highly inversely correlated with cloze probability. However, as pre- noun cloze ranged from 0100%. These broad ranges of expectancy
viously mentioned, the pattern of noun effects may be a consequence allowed for analysis of the correlations between the ERP effects and the
either of the brains surprise at encountering an item different than offline probabilities of the relevant continuations.
what it expects (prediction view), or greater difficulty integrating the In the ERP experiment, different participants read sentences of
received word into the sentence representation (integration view). varying contextual constraint that included target articles and nouns
2005 Nature Publishing Group http://www.nature.com/natureneuroscience

Indeed, on the basis of our experiences, kite may be easier to with large ranges of cloze. Across participants the same sentence
integrate into the developing sentence representation than airplane. context appeared with both higher- and lower-probability articles
Given the difference in their meanings, it is likely that kite and and nouns. Notably, although some continuations were more probable
airplane also differ in how well they fit with event schemas that the than others, none was nonsensical, barring participants from develop-
sentence brings to mind through semantic memory processes. How- ing a strategy (conscious or unconscious) whereby an improbable
ever, whereas kite and airplane differ in meaning, a and an do not, article was taken to signal an impending semantic anomaly.
being distinguished only by their phonological forms. Since their
semantics are identical and they differ only in frequency of usage and ERP recording and analyses
length, there is no reason for the articles to be differentially difficult to To measure brain activity associated with prediction effects, we
integrate into a given sentence representation unless (i) a is always recorded electroencephalograms at 26 scalp locations as 32 partici-
easier to integrate, because it is shorter and/or more frequent than an pants read sentences word-by-word from a CRT at a rate of 2 words/s
in everyday usage, or, as we will maintain, (ii) comprehenders have (200-ms duration each). ERPs were analyzed for target articles and
already (unconsciously) formed a higher, non-trivial expectation for nouns in 160 sentences, with 16 participants viewing each item. The
kite than for airplane. 160 articles and nouns were sorted into ten equal-width bins as a
If anticipation is an integral part of language processing, then it function of each items cloze probability, from highest (90100%) to
should be reflected in the brain activity probed by the more and less lowest (010%). ERPs for each 10% bin were averaged first within, then
expected indefinite articles. If the amount of pre-activation is driven across, participants. The average numerical cloze probability of each
strictly by word length or frequency, then whatever the ERP effect, it
would be context independent, with all examples of a (versus an)
Vertex ERPs by median split on cloze probability,
patterning together. Even if pre-activation is context dependent, the a e.g., 'The day was breezy so the boy went outside to fly ...'
brain may react to the anticipated article with one response and to
Articles Nouns
anything else with a different response, in a binary rather than a graded 5 V
fashion. Finally, we hypothesize that the language processor exploits
sentence context to probabilistically pre-activate possible continua- 5 V
'airplane'
tions, consistent with constraint-based models; if so, the N400 should
'an'
respond to a degree that can be estimated from the articles offline cloze
probability. In sum, no observable difference in the brains response to 0 500 ms 0 500 ms
more- versus less-expected articles would be a sharp blow to predictive 'a'
processing accounts, whereas a graded N400 effect correlated with the 'kite'
articles offline cloze probability would support incremental, predictive < 50% Article cloze < 50% Noun cloze
processing that is sensitive to meaning-based constraints. 50% Article cloze 50% Noun cloze

N400 x cloze probability correlations at vertex


b
Figure 1 ERP waveforms and correlations between N400 amplitude and 2.5 Articles 2 Nouns
cloze probability showing that specific words were predicted during language
2.0 1
200500 ms (V)

200500 ms (V)
Mean amplitude

Mean amplitude

comprehension. Articles and nouns were analyzed separately. (a) Illustrative


ERPs at the midline central (vertex) recording site according to median splits 1.5 0
on cloze probabilities. Negative amplitudes are plotted upwards. Both articles
and nouns with o50% cloze elicited greater negativity between 200500 ms 1.0 1

post-stimulus onset (N400) than those with Z50% cloze. Although the 0.5 r = 0.68 2 r = 0.79
ERPs include responses to both article types (a, an) and both noun types
(kite, airplane), a single sample sentence is provided for simplicity. 0 3
0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0
(b) Mean N400 amplitudes were inversely correlated with items cloze Article cloze probability Noun cloze probability
probabilities. Scatter diagrams show strong inverse relations between cloze
and N400 amplitude at the vertex for both articles (r 0.68, P o 0.05) c Articles
r-values at all recording sites
Nouns
and nouns (r 0.79, P o 0.01). Best-fitting regression lines are also 1.00
plotted. (c) The r-values for all 26 electrode sites plotted on an idealized 0.34 0.25
0.41 0.53 0.36 0.36
head, looking down, nose at the top. Darker shading indicates larger negative 0.34 0.54 0.67 0.32 0.41
correlations, with r-values between sites estimated by spherical spline 0.30 0.62 0.33 0.44 0.55
0.19 0.43 0.60 0.63 0.21 0.63 0.66 0.05
interpolation. The dotted circle demarcates the vertex. Although ERP
0.49 0.68 0.68 0.00 0.71 0.79 0.65
prediction effects were slightly larger over the right hemisphere for both 0.59 0.69 0.83 0.79
articles and nouns and over posterior sites for nouns, the correlations for 0.27 0.63 0.67 0.33 0.08 0.82 0.70
0.51 0.72 0.81 0.76
both articles and nouns showed maximal values over centroparietal sites,
0.53 0.61 0.67 0.77 0.75
with a right hemisphere bias only for the articles (see Supplementary Note 0.27 0.52
0.75 0.75
0.60 1.00 0.84
for detailed distributional analyses).

1118 VOLUME 8 [ NUMBER 8 [ AUGUST 2005 NATURE NEUROSCIENCE


86
ARTICLES

bin was then calculated and correlated with mean ERP amplitude in the scalp sites (Fig. 1c) where previous reading studies have shown the
N400 time window (200500 ms) for articles and nouns separately. largest N400 effects. This topographical pattern indicated that the
Correlation coefficients (r-values) and percentage of variance explained values were not simply the spurious outcome of multiple testing at the
by offline probability (r2) were then calculated separately for all 26 26 electrode sites.
electrode sites. In conducting the correlational analyses, we established the func-
tional relationship of the negativity between 200500 ms post-article
N400 effects and correlations onset to the canonical N400 typically elicited by nouns and verbs.
As expected, N400 amplitude decreased (became less negative) as noun Notably, we also demonstrated that this negativity indexed expectancy
cloze probabilities increased (Fig. 1a). We replicated the well-known for the eliciting article (and upcoming noun). Given that all articles
2005 Nature Publishing Group http://www.nature.com/natureneuroscience

correlation between N400 amplitude and offline cloze of the target were grammatically and semantically congruent within their contexts
nouns (Fig. 1b), with correlation coefficients ranging from r 0.36 and that a and an have identical semantics, there was no reason for
(not significant) to r 0.84 (P o 0.01) at various scalp sites (Fig. 1c). either article type to have been any more or less difficult to integrate
Noun cloze probability thus accounted for up to 71% of variance in into the sentence representation. Systematic variation in amplitude
brain activity between 200500 ms after a nouns appearance. More- of the ERP negativity in relation to offline article cloze probability
over, correlations peaked over posterior sites, where N400 amplitudes thus constitutes strong evidence that participants were indeed antici-
are typically largest, whereas anterior sites (where visual N400s are pating the phonological form of a particular noun and therefore had
usually less prevalent) showed little if any evidence of similarly formed expectations for one article type relative to the other and
correlated brain activity (Fig. 1c). These results were an important seemed to experience some processing difficulty when the less-expected
precondition for analysis of the articles because they demonstrated that article appeared.
the different degrees of constraint in these materials were reflected Articles are relatively short; highly frequent; highly predictable as a
in offline expectancies and N400 amplitude modulations in the usual word class; not as semantically rich as nouns, verbs, adjectives or
way. However, as previously noted, the noun correlation pattern does adverbs and are often skipped over during natural reading28. In
not settle the question of prediction because high correlations could addition to providing unequivocal evidence for lexically specific pre-
reflect either the degree of pre-activation or the variance in the diction, the article correlations are compelling evidence that articles,
integrability of the noun with the mental representation of the sentence too, are predicted and integrated with context in qualitatively similar
up to that moment. ways as nouns. For reasons not yet known, the correlations with offline
To address the question of prediction directly we turned to the probability were on average lower for articles than for nouns (although
correlation pattern of the target articles. Although article ERP wave- at some electrode sites, the two were statistically similar). Nonetheless,
forms were significantly smaller than those elicited by nouns, the the article correlations clearly demonstrated that prediction is not
amplitude of the negativity in the N400 time window did indeed limited to highly constraining contexts. We believe that this sort of
vary as a function of article expectancy (Fig. 1a). Just as for the nouns, anticipation is an integral (perhaps inevitable) part of real-time
the higher the articles cloze probability, the smaller the ERP negativity language processing and is likely to have a functional role, although
between 200500 ms post-onset (Fig. 1b), with correlation coefficients this has yet to be demonstrated.
ranging from r "0.19 (not significant) to r "0.72 (P o 0.05) at Our findings thus suggest that individuals can use linguistic input to
various recording sites (Fig. 1c). Moreover, maximum correlations pre-activate representations of upcoming words in advance of their
clustered over centroparietal sites, similar to nouns, albeit more appearance. Exactly what informs these predictions, as well as the
lateralized to the right (Fig. 1c and Supplementary Note online). So neural mechanism for predictive language processing, are matters for
at least over certain scalp areas, up to 52% of variance in article N400 empirical and computational investigations. An open question, for
amplitude was accounted for by the average probability that individuals instance, is how the human sentence comprehension system handles
would continue the sentence context with that particular article offline. variation in natural input rates (for example, 2 to 3.5 words/s), and in
particular, whether the same (or different) mechanisms are engaged. In
DISCUSSION line with most studies of comprehension that draw general conclusions
By constructing sentence contexts that led to varying offline expecta- without systematically varying input rate, we assume that basic
tions for nouns beginning with vowel or consonant sounds, we could language processing mechanisms do not vary fundamentally across
assess the extent to which such expectations were formed online by the range of normal input rates. This parsimonious assumption is
preceding the noun with the phonologically appropriate indefinite bolstered by results of N400 studies in which variation in presentation
article or the other semantically identical, sententially congruent but rate showed no evidence for the engagement of qualitatively different
phonologically inappropriate one. Similar to the nouns, the more neural mechanisms29,30. Although the current study demonstrated
contextually unexpected an indefinite article was, the more negative the graded prediction only at the slower end of natural input rates, we
ERP mean amplitude between 200500 ms post-word onset (N400). In suggest that this conclusion may generalize to faster rates, given the
other words, the brains response to the articles differed in a graded aforementioned arguments and ERP evidence for binary prediction
fashion as a function of contextual constraint. Our results thus (expected versus unexpected) in natural speech26,27. Subsequent
demonstrate not only that readers can rapidly, incrementally integrate experimentation will undoubtedly shed more light on these issues.
incoming words into evolving mental sentence representations, but We propose that single words and combinations of words in a
that they do so in part by exploiting various constraining forces to form sentence tap into and differentially activate information via semantic
probabilistic predictions of which specific words will come next. Here, memory, going beyond the immediate physical input. Semantic mem-
we clearly showed this for the target articles and nouns, although we ory is presumed to include information about individual words as well
have no reason to assume the same would not hold for every word in a as world knowledge built up from experience of people, places, things
sentence throughout the range of normal reading rates. Notably, and events. We maintain that probabilistic pre-activation of particular
maximum correlations for both nouns and articles were not randomly word forms follows the access of this experiential knowledge as a result
distributed across the scalp but rather clustered over centroparietal of linguistic input. Our observation of an ERP expectancy effect at the

NATURE NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 8 [ NUMBER 8 [ AUGUST 2005 1119


87
ARTICLES

article leads us to conclude that predictions can be for specific continuations for sentences in advance of the actual input. In this
phonological formswords beginning with either vowels or conso- regard, language comprehension appears to involve a special case of the
nants. In this sense, we propose that prediction can be highly specific, at anticipatory behavior observed in other biological systems.
least under some circumstances.
Our results are in line with a growing list of empirical studies METHODS
demonstrating that the brains language parser projects probabilistic Experimental design and materials. Stimuli consisted of 80 sentence contexts
expectations about various aspects of linguistic processing during with two possible target types: relatively expected and unexpected indefinite
online sentence comprehension tasks. Several studies suggest that the article/noun pairs. Each article/noun pair served as a more- and less-expected
target in different contexts. Targets were sentence medial and congruent (that
parser uses constraints accruing as a sentence is analyzed word-by-word
2005 Nature Publishing Group http://www.nature.com/natureneuroscience

is, no agreement violations such as a airplane). The 160 stimuli were divided
to (i) compute likely relationships among referents in linguistic and
into two lists of 80 sentences, each participant viewing one list. Sentence
visual contexts (for example, upon hearing the word eat, a person is contexts and article/noun targets were used only once per list. Each list
likely to scan the environment for something edible)19, (ii) pre-activate contained equal numbers of relatively expected and unexpected targets as well
semantic features of categories (for example, expecting a particular as a and an targets. One-quarter of sentences were followed by yes/no
kind of tree pre-activates features of trees, even when not all trees would comprehension questions, 94% of which were answered correctly by partici-
be plausible in the sentence context)24 or (iii) anticipate various pants, on average (range, 88100%).
syntactic aspects of to-be-presented material (for example, expecting Norming for articles and nouns was done with different groups of student
the grammatical gender of upcoming items in gender-marked lan- volunteers. Informed written consent was obtained from all norming (and
guages such as Spanish or Dutch) during word-by-word reading and ERP) participants. For articles, cloze ratings were obtained from 30 participants
for 80 sentence contexts truncated before the target article. For nouns,
natural speech2527.
sentences were truncated after target articles, with two versions of each context
In particular, our study expands on findings from the aforemen-
(160 sentences total): one with the more-probable article supplied, the other
tioned grammatical gender studies25,27 in which nouns were preceded with the less-probable article. Individual participants saw only one version,
by words whose syntactic gender marking was inconsistent with that of with each normed by 30 participants.
the expected noun. Although both the gender studies and our study
used the same general logic, the differences in experimental manipula- Participants. Thirty-two volunteers (23 women) participated in the ERP
tions, design and analyses lead to substantive differences in the experiment for course credit or for cash. All were right-handed, native English
justifiable conclusions. Whereas the Spanish and Dutch studies utilized speakers with normal or corrected-to-normal vision, between 1837 years
(mean, 21 years). Seven participants reported a left-handed parent or sibling.
pre-nominal gender marking on determiners and adjectives, respec-
tively, our study relied upon a purely phonological (sound representa- Procedure. Volunteers were tested in a single session, with visual sentences
tion) relation between probed articles and upcoming nouns. Thus, our presented centrally, one word at a time (200-ms duration, 500-ms stimulus
observation of article ERP variation provides a strong test of whether onset asynchrony). The instructions were to read sentences for comprehension
the language system predicts word forms with specific phonological and answer yes/no comprehension questions by pressing hand-held buttons.
content (lexemes), instead of simply representations specifying words The electroencephalogram (EEG) was recorded from 26 electrodes arranged
semantic and syntactic properties (lemmas). In addition, having tested geodesically in an Electro-cap, each referenced online to the left mastoid. Blinks
and eye movements were monitored from electrodes placed on the outer canthi
prediction with semantically identical a/an articles (function words)
and under each eye, referenced to the left mastoid. Electrode impedances were
instead of words richer in meaning (content words) such as adjectives, kept below 5 KO. The EEG was amplified with Grass amplifiers with a band-
we effectively counter the argument that the observed difference pass of 0.01 to 100 Hz, continuously digitized at a sampling rate of 250 Hz.
between more- versus less-predictable articles reflects difficulty inter-
preting them. And perhaps most notably, only our study compares Data analysis. Trials contaminated by eye movements, excessive muscle activity
brain activity elicited by a range of more- or less-predictable articles, or amplifier blocking were rejected offline before averaging; on average, 10.7%
not simply most- versus least-expected. The article correlation findings of article trials and 11.4% of noun trials were rejected. Data with excessive
blinks were corrected using a spatial filter algorithm. A digital band-pass filter
thus show for the first time that the language system does not simply
set from 0.2 to 15 Hz was used on all data to reduce high-frequency noise. Data
pre-activate a single word when its representation exceeds some thresh- were re-referenced offline to the algebraic sum of left and right mastoids and
old given a highly constraining context. Instead, a gradient of pre- averaged for each experimental condition, time-locked to the target article and
activation shows that the system makes graded predictions. noun onsets.
Our electrophysiological results extend previous prediction findings
in several other notable ways. First, they demonstrate that a candidate Note: Supplementary information is available on the Nature Neuroscience website.
entity (or its depiction) need not be physically present in order for the
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
brain to narrow the possibilities for likely continuations; rather, pre- Many thanks to E. De Ochoa and J. Cagle for help with data collection and to
dictions can emerge on the basis of associations that form as sentential J. Elman for helpful comments on this manuscript. This research was supported
context accrues. Second, our results illustrate that at least one subclass by US National Institute of Child Health and Human Development grant
of function words (which generally provide more grammatical struc- HD22614 and National Institute on Aging grant AG08313 to M.K. and by
a Center for Research in Language Training Fellowship to K.A.D.
ture than lexical meaning), indefinite articles, can be important in
building context and facilitating linguistic processing. This finding is COMPETING INTERESTS STATEMENT
particularly relevant given the paucity of evidence in the comprehen- The authors declare that they have no competing financial interests.
sion literature about semantic context effects on function words31,32.
Received 21 April; accepted 21 June 2005
And finally, our findings unambiguously show that anticipatory pro-
Published online at http://www.nature.com/natureneuroscience/
cessing can happen not only for conceptual or semantic features but
also for specific phonological word forms. In sum, although natural
language comprehension must occur over a range of input rates with a 1. Stanovich, K.E. & West, R.F. Mechanisms of sentence context effects in reading:
automatic activation and conscious attention. Mem. Cognit. 7, 7785 (1979).
nearly infinite number of possible word combinations, these factors do 2. Kintsch, W. & van Dijk, T.A. Towards a model of text comprehension and production.
not seem to prevent the brain from anticipating the most probable Psychol. Rev. 85, 363394 (1978).

1120 VOLUME 8 [ NUMBER 8 [ AUGUST 2005 NATURE NEUROSCIENCE


88
ARTICLES

3. Carroll, J. & Bever, T. Sentence comprehension: a case study in the relation of knowledge 19. Altmann, G.T.M. & Kamide, Y. Incremental interpretation at verbs: restricting the
and perception. in Handbook of Perception, Vol. 7 (eds. Carterette, E. & Friedman, M.) domain of subsequent reference. Cognition 73, 247264 (1999).
299317 (Academic, New York, 1978). 20. Tanenhaus, M.K., Magnuson, J.S., Dahan, D. & Chambers, C. Eye movements and
4. Just, M.A. & Carpenter, P.A. A theory of reading: From eye fixations to comprehension. lexical access in spoken-language comprehension: evaluating a linking hypothesis
Psychol. Rev. 87, 329354 (1980). between fixations and linguistic processing. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 29, 557580
5. Daneman, M. & Carpenter, P.A. Individual differences in integrating information between (2000).
and within sentences. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 9, 561584 (1983). 21. Kamide, Y., Scheepers, C. & Altmann, G.T.M. Integration of syntactic and semantic
6. Mitchell, D.C. & Green, D.W. The effects of context and content on immediate processing information in predictive processing: cross-linguistic evidence from German and
in reading. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 30, 609636 (1978). English. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 32, 3755 (2003).
7. Marslen-Wilson, W.D. Sentence perception as an interactive parallel process. Science 22. Chambers, C.G. & Smyth, R. Structural parallelism and discourse coherence: a test of
189, 226228 (1975). centering theory. J. Mem. Lang. 39, 593608 (1998).
8. Altmann, G.T.M. & Steedman, M.J. Interaction with context during human sentence 23. Sedivy, J.C., Tanenhaus, M.K., Chambers, C.G. & Carlson, G.N. Achieving incremental
2005 Nature Publishing Group http://www.nature.com/natureneuroscience

processing. Cognition 30, 191238 (1988). semantic interpretation through contextual representation. Cognition 71, 109148
9. Pickering, M.J. Processing local and unbounded dependencies: a unified account. (1999).
J. Psycholinguist. Res. 23, 323352 (1994). 24. Federmeier, K.D. & Kutas, M. A rose by any other name: long-term memory structure and
10. Tyler, L.K. & Marslen-Wilson, W.D. The on-line effects of semantic context on syntactic sentence processing. J. Mem. Lang. 41, 469495 (1999).
processing. J. Verb. Learn. Verb. Behav. 16, 683692 (1977). 25. Wicha, N.Y.Y., Moreno, E.M. & Kutas, M. Anticipating words and their gender: an
11. Traxler, M., Bybee, M. & Pickering, M. Influence of connectives on language comprehen- event-related brain potential study of semantic integration, gender expectancy, and
sion: eye-tracking evidence for incremental interpretation. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A 50A, gender agreement in Spanish sentence reading. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 12721288
481497 (1997). (2004).
12. Marslen-Wilson, W.D. & Tyler, L.K. The temporal structure of spoken language under- 26. Wicha, N.Y.Y., Bates, E.A., Moreno, E.M. & Kutas, M. Potato not Pope: human brain
standing. Cognition 8, 171 (1980). potentials to gender expectation and agreement in Spanish spoken sentences. Neurosci.
13. Steedman, M. Grammar, interpretation, and processing from the lexicon. in Lexical Lett. 346, 165168 (2003).
Representation and Process (ed. Marslen-Wilson, W.D.) 463504 (MIT Press, Cam- 27. Van Berkum, J.J.A., Brown, C.M., Zwitserlood, P., Kooijman, V. & Hagoort, P. Anticipat-
bridge, Massachusetts, 1989). ing upcoming words in discourse: evidence from ERPs and reading times. J. Exp.
14. Boland, J.E., Tanenhaus, M.K., Garnsey, S.M. & Carlson, G.N. Verb argument structure Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 31, 443467 (2005).
in parsing and interpretation: Evidence from wh-questions. J. Mem. Lang. 34, 774806 28. ORegan, K. Saccade size control in reading: evidence for the linguistic control
(1995). hypothesis. Percept. Psychophys. 25, 501509 (1979).
15. Tanenhaus, M., Spivey-Knowlton, M., Eberhard, K. & Sedivy, J. Integration of visual 29. Kutas, M. Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) elicited during rapid serial visual
and linguistic information during spoken language comprehension. Science 268, presentation of congruous and incongruous sentences. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neu-
16321634 (1995). rophysiol. 40 (suppl.), 406411 (1987).
16. Kutas, M. & Hillyard, S.A. Event-related brain potentials to grammatical errors and 30. Gunter, T.C., Jackson, J.L. & Mulder, G. An electrophysiological study of semantic
semantic anomalies. Mem. Cognit. 11, 539550 (1983). processing in young and middle-aged academics. Psychophysiology 29, 3854 (1992).
17. Kutas, M. & Hillyard, S.A. Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect 31. King, J.W. & Kutas, M. A brain potential whose latency indexes the length and frequency
semantic incongruity. Science 207, 203204 (1980). of words. CRL Newsletter 10, 19 (1995).
18. Kutas, M. & Hillyard, S.A. Brain potentials during reading reflect word expectancy and 32. Kluender, R. & Kutas, M. Subjacency as a processing phenomenon. Lang. Cognitive
semantic association. Nature 307, 161163 (1984). Proc. 8, 573633 (1993).

NATURE NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 8 [ NUMBER 8 [ AUGUST 2005 1121


89
90
91
92
93
94
95
96
97
98
99
100
101
102
103
104
105
106
107
108
109
110
111
112
113
114
115
116
117
118
119
120
121
122
123
124
125
126
127
128
129
130
131
132
133
134
135
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association
2004, Vol. 87, No. 5, 586 598 0022-3514/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.5.586

Think Different: The Merits of Unconscious Thought in Preference


Development and Decision Making
Ap Dijksterhuis
University of Amsterdam

The role of unconscious and conscious thought in decision making was investigated in 5 experiments.
Because of the low processing capacity of consciousness, conscious thought was hypothesized to be
maladaptive when making complex decisions. Conversely, unconscious thought was expected to be
highly effective. In Experiments 13, participants were presented with a complex decision problem in
which they had to choose between various alternatives, each with multiple attributes. Some participants
had to make a decision immediately after being presented with the options. In the conscious thought
condition, participants could think about the decision for a few minutes. In the unconscious thought
condition, participants were distracted for a few minutes and then indicated their decision. Throughout
the experiments, unconscious thinkers made the best decisions. Additional evidence obtained in Exper-
iments 4 and 5 suggests that unconscious thought leads to clearer, more polarized, and more integrated
representations in memory.

When making a decision of minor importance, I have always found it The problem is that it feels wrong to make such an important
advantageous to consider all the pros and cons. In vital matters decision so quickly. It was not necessarily a bad decisionI can
however . . . the decision should come from the unconscious, from now say it was a good one but it felt like it was poorly made.
somewhere within ourselves.
After all, psychologists, teachers, and parents have all taught us
Sigmund Freud
that we need to think hard before we make an important decision.
Their advice is both well meant and intuitively compelling. How-
A few years ago, I accepted my current job at the University of
ever, whether thorough conscious thinking always leads to good
Amsterdam and shortly afterward started my quest for a place to
decisions is not clear. Freud questioned it, as did his contemporary
live. My timing was awful. The housing market was incredibly
Whitehead (1911), who said, It is a profoundly erroneous truism,
tight. Apartments were very expensive and very hard to get.
During those days, brokers adopted a procedure that was rather repeated by all copybooks and by eminent people making
demanding for the blood pressure of buyers. A house on sale was speeches, that we should cultivate the habit of thinking about what
widely advertised, and all potential buyers were invited to visit at were doing. The precise opposite is the case (p. 143; also quoted
the same time. The first person who made a bid (provided it was in Bargh, 1997, p. 10, and in Claxton, 1997, p. 15).
reasonable) would get the house. When I finally visited an apart- It is interesting that these same parents also advised us to take
ment I thought I liked, I knew I had to make a bid quickly. Very our time when faced with an important decision by telling us to
quickly. Between the moment I first passed through the doorway sleep on it. This suggests another approach to making a decision.
of the apartment and the moment I essentially became the owner of Here, people intuitively sense that letting the unconscious mull
the place, all of five minutes passed. As far as I remember, I had over the problem may lead to better decisions. However, whether
glanced at the bathroom for about three seconds before I commit- what we may call unconscious thought contributes to good
ted myself to a mortgage of alarming proportions. I did not sleep decisions is also not clear. Some recent treatments of the uncon-
much that night. scious have been optimistic about its powers (Bargh & Chartrand,
1999; Claxton, 1997; Wegner & Smart, 1997; Wilson, 2002), but
it has become so fashionable to view the unconscious as a rela-
tively stupid system that many people will surely not expect
This research was supported by Nederlandse Organisatie voor Weten- unconscious thought to be beneficial at all.
schappelijk OnderzoekVernieuwingsimpuls Grant 016.025.030. I thank The phrase unconscious thought may raise eyebrows. In the
Teun Meurs for his great help throughout this project and Odin de Vries present context, conscious thought and unconscious thought are
and Bregje Zwaan for the collection of the data of Experiment 3. I defined in the following way. Conscious thought refers to the
benefited greatly from comments on an earlier version by Henk Aarts, cognitive and/or affective task-relevant processes one is con-
Jonathan Schooler, Pamela Smith, Tim Wilson, Helma van den Berg, Karin sciously aware of while attending to a task. For instance, one may
Bongers, Yael de Liver, Kirsten Ruys, and Daniel Wigboldus. Thanks.
compare two holiday destinations and consciously think, The
The title of this article was inspired by the well-known Think Differ-
Spanish coast is cheap but I do not want to go there because it is
ent Apple ad campaign.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Ap way too crowded. Unconscious thought, on the other hand, refers
Dijksterhuis, Social Psychology Program, University of Amsterdam, to cognitive and/or affective task-relevant processes that take place
Roetersstaat 15, 1018 WB, Amsterdam, the Netherlands. E-mail: outside conscious awareness. One may compare two holiday des-
a.j.dijksterhuis@uva.nl tinations and not know which one to choose. Subsequently, one
586

136
MERITS OF UNCONSCIOUS THOUGHT 587

does not consciously attend to the problem for a few days, and several dimensions. The low capacity of consciousness will likely
suddenly the thought, Its going to be Tuscany! pops into mind. prevent it from taking all this information into account simulta-
This thought itself is conscious, but the transition from indecision neously, the consequence being that consciousness will only deal
to a preference a few days later is the result of unconscious with a subset of information. This may come at the expense of the
thought. final decision.
In this article, conscious and unconscious thought are pitted Indeed, it has been shown that when people can devote only a
against each other to answer the question, Who is the better limited amount of information processing capacity to making a
decision maker of the two? This question is investigated with decision (e.g., when they are under time pressure), normative,
complex decision problems. When choosing between jobs, be- consciously driven processes can lead to worse decisions than
tween houses, between different holiday destinations, between more heuristic strategies (Payne, Bettman, & Johnson, 1988). This
roommates, people are faced with various alternatives, each char- is because more elaborate, normative strategies only work well
acterized by many pros and cons. Making sound decisions requires when all information is taken into account.
integration of large amounts of information into impressions and a The work by Wilson, Schooler, and colleagues provides direct
comparison between these impressions to arrive at a preference. To evidence for consciousness as a poor decision maker because of its
do this, two things are needed. One needs enough processing limited capacity. Wilson and Schooler (1991; see also Schooler &
capacity to deal with large amounts of information, and one needs Melcher, 1995; Schooler, Ohlsson, & Brooks, 1993; Wilson et al.,
skills sophisticated enough to integrate information in a meaning- 1993) had participants evaluate objects, such as different college
ful and accurate way. courses. In one condition, participants were simply asked to eval-
uate the different objects. They most likely engaged in little
Processing Capacity conscious thought. In another condition, participants were pressed
to carefully analyze the reasons for their evaluations before mak-
A little introspection reveals that the processing capacity of ing them and to write down these reasons. They engaged in more
consciousness is limited. People are not able to concentrate con- thorough conscious thinking. This did not help them. In fact, they
sciously on two different things simultaneously. About a half a did a poorer job than participants who thought less. As expected,
century ago, researchers started to try to quantify the processing additional evidence indicated that conscious thought led people to
capacity of consciousness and the unconscious. Miller (1956) focus on a limited number of attributes at the expense of taking
demonstrated that the maximum amount of information that can be into account other relevant attributes.
kept under conscious scrutiny at any given time is about seven Other evidence comes from Pelham and Neter (1995). They
units. That is not a lot. Others who investigated the processing asked participants to solve various problems. Some problems were
capacity of consciousness also drew rather sobering conclusions. transparent and easy to solve, whereas others were difficult to
Consciousness can process about 40 60 bits per second (for brief solve, and participants had to avoid pitfalls (they ran the risk of
reviews of this research, see Nrretranders, 1998; Wilson, 2002). heuristics leading them astray). Some people were simply asked to
Take reading, for example. Each letter of the alphabet requires solve the problems, whereas others were strongly motivated to
about 5 bits. This means that a random letter string of eight letters solve the problems accurately. This increased motivation helped
(noahlief) contains 40 bits. However, in real language, letters are participants to be more accurate on the easy problems, but it
not random (e.g., zzzgh never appears in English), so a real hindered solving the complex problems. If one is willing to assume
eight-letter word has far less than 40 bits. Taking this into account, that the motivated participants engaged in more conscious think-
when people read, they process about 45 bits per second, equiva- ing, the results support the notion of consciousness as a low-
lent to a fairly short sentence. Although the number of bits con- capacity system. Ironically, more conscious thought reduced the
sciousness can process is dependent on both the task and the chance that people took crucial information into account.
experience (a skilled reader can process more bits while reading
than a child who is still mastering it), the processing capacity of
consciousness is low. The Skills to Think
In contrast, the processing capacity of the entire human system,
or, in other words, of conscious and unconscious processes com- Another important requirement for making a good decision is
bined, is enormous. The capacity of the entire system is about the integration of information in a meaningful way. Simply put, it
11,200,000 bits. The visual system alone can deal with 10 million is important to be able to think. Are both consciousness and the
bits per second. It follows that the unconscious does not have a unconscious able to associate, integrate, elaborate, weigh?
capacity problem. If the unconscious is a modern computer, con- Consciousness may suffer from a power cut when too much
sciousness is nothing more than an old abacus. pressure is put on its limited capacity, but as long as its capacity is
The low capacity of consciousness suggests that it may not be enough to deal with a particular problem, it is likely to be a good
up to the task of making complex decisions: The sheer amount of thinker. For instance, consciousness can effectively moderate im-
information involved may be too much for it to tackle. Take the pressions that were initially made unconsciously (e.g., Gilbert,
example of choosing a graduate school. Different programs have 1989). When we see someone shout, we come to the impression
different professors with different research interests, teaching eval- that this person is aggressive, but conscious intervention can alter
uations, and reputations as advisors. Some programs have a higher this impression if warranted (wait, he was provoked). With
status than others, but this status is not always perfectly correlated decisions, such conscious interventions can be highly effective.
with the status of the university as a whole. Different schools are Faced with the opportunity to buy an apartment with many wildly
also located in different cities with their own pros and cons on positive attributes and a single critical negative one (it is much too

137
588 DIJKSTERHUIS

expensive), consciousness will be good at quickly deciding failed to take into account even a small portion of the 75 units of
against it. information, this would have been impossible.
The question is whether the unconscious is a good thinker. If it Other evidence for unconscious thought processes comes from
can devote a relatively small part of its enormous processing research by Bowers, Regehr, Balthazard, and Parker (1990). Their
capacity to thought, it can potentially be very useful. However, a participants were asked to identify words while from time to time
vast processing capacity does not necessarily imply useful work. If they were given a hint, such as an associated word. After each hint,
you shut down your computer, it cannot do anything. Or, to stick they were pressed to guess. When people solve such problems,
to the computer metaphor, some computers are incredibly power- they feel as if they suddenly know the answer. Indeed, the
ful but do only one simple thing, like sorting mail by zip code. So answer suddenly pops up in consciousness (red . . . bowl . . . fresh
what can the unconscious contribute to making decisions? . . . of course, they mean fruit!). However, peoples successive
An area where people have devoted time and effort to investi- guesses indicated that the process is not quite as sudden if seen
gating the fruits of the unconscious is the domain of creativity. from the perspective of the unconscious. Successive guesses con-
Researchers have long recognized the importance of incubation, verged, and the unconscious seemed to be closing in on the right
the process whereby a problem is consciously ignored for a while, answer quite a while before the answer was accessible to con-
after which the unconscious offers a solution. The amount of sciousness. Related findings come from research on tip-of-the-
anecdotal evidence for incubation is enormous. Nobel laureates tongue phenomena. Yaniv and Meyer (1987) offered participants
and famous artists often refer to this process as the true key to definitions of rare words they could not recall but felt they knew.
insight (see, e.g., Gardner, 1993; Ghiselin, 1952). George Spencer In a lexical decision task, the target tip-of-the-tongue words were
Brown has famously said about Sir Isaac Newton that highly accessible. Although the words were inaccessible to con-
sciousness, the unconscious had found and activated them.
to arrive at the simplest truth, as Newton knew and practiced, requires To summarize, there is enough evidence to at least assume that
years of contemplation. Not activity. Not reasoning. Not calculating.
the unconscious continues to think about pressing matters in the
Not busy behavior of any kind. Not reading. Not talking. Not making
an effort. Not thinking. Simply bearing in mind what it is that one
absence of any conscious attention.
needs to know. (Quoted in Claxton, 1997, p. 58)
Overview of Experiments
Many of us recognize processes of incubation in ourselves. We
think about a paper we want to write and mull over the order in Two hypotheses were tested in the current research. First, when
which we are going to develop our argument in the introduction. making complex decisions, a brief period of unconscious thought
We put things to rest for a while and then suddenly, BING, we will lead to a better decision relative to conditions under which
feel we know it. unconscious thought is prevented. Second, when making complex
Strong as the anecdotal evidence may be, for a long time proof decisions, conscious thought is inferior relative to unconscious
of incubation was notoriously hard to establish in a research lab thought. The first three experiments directly tested these two
(e.g., Olton, 1979). In addition, the empirical evidence for incu- hypotheses. The fourth and fifth experiments were designed to
bation available these days (e.g., Schooler & Melcher, 1995; shed light on the nature of unconscious thought.
Smith, 1995) is usually not explained by unconscious thought. In the first three experiments, the same paradigm was used.
Instead, incubation is seen as fruitful because one is distracted Participants were presented with information about various alter-
from the problem at hand. Not thinking about a problem for a natives (apartments in Experiments 1 and 2, roommates in Exper-
while may lead people to forget wrong heuristics or inappropriate iment 3). Alternatives were described by both positive and nega-
strategies in general. Distraction, then, allows people to give the tive attributes, and one was made rather desirable and another one
problem a fresh look. The distinction between distraction, whereby rather undesirable. Hence, the quality of the decision is judged
the role of the unconscious is passive, and true active, unconscious from a normative perspective. In the experiments, filler alterna-
thought, is discussed in the introduction to Experiments 4 and 5 tives were included in order to increase the complexity of the
below. decision problem. These fillers were constructed to be neutral.
Whereas the conclusions that can be drawn from the literature Participants indicated their preference either directly, by choos-
on incubation are rather sobering regarding unconscious thought, ing an alternative (Experiment 2), or indirectly, by indicating their
recent evidence by Betsch, Plessner, Schwieren, and Gutig (2001) evaluation of each alternative (Experiments 1 and 3). In Experi-
shows that the unconscious can integrate large amounts of infor- ments 1 and 3, the dependent variable was the difference in attitude
mation. Betsch et al.s participants looked at ads shown on a toward the desirable and the undesirable alternatives. There are
computer screen. They were asked to carefully look at the ads two reasons for this choice. First, when choosing between alter-
because their memory for the ads would be probed. At the same natives, recognizing the best alternative is obviously important.
time, the numerical increases and decreases of five hypothetical However, recognizing and rejecting a particularly unattractive
shares were shown. Participants were presented with 75 units of alternative is in many cases just as important. The second reason
information, all only briefly presented on the computer screen. is practical. A difference score between two opposite attitudes
Afterward, they were asked specific questions about each of five provides more statistical power than a single attitude score.
shares, such as what the average money returns were. They were In all experiments, there were three conditions. Some partici-
clueless. However, when they were only asked to give their atti- pants were provided with the relevant information and had to
tudes, they somehow knew what the best and worst shares were. decide (or evaluate) immediately afterward. This condition is the
Participants had developed a gut feeling toward the shares, indi- immediate decision condition. Participants in the conscious
cating that they had integrated the information. If participants had thought condition were given a few minutes to think about the

138
MERITS OF UNCONSCIOUS THOUGHT 589

information before they decided. Finally, participants in the un- After participants read all the information, they were randomly allocated
conscious thought condition were distracted for a few minutes to one of three conditions. In the immediate decision condition, they were
before they decided, thereby enabling them to think unconsciously immediately asked to give their attitude toward each of the four apartments.
while at the same time preventing conscious thought. The imme- The questions were phrased How would you judge Apartment . . . ?
Participants were asked to indicate their answer on a 10-point scale ranging
diate decision condition can be seen as a baseline where little or no
from 1 (extremely negative) to 10 (extremely positive). All participants
thought takes place after participants read the information about rated the apartments in numerical order, starting with Apartment 1.
the various alternatives. The unconscious thought condition is In the conscious thought condition, participants were first asked to very
devised in such a way as to prevent conscious thought. The first carefully think about what you think of each of the four apartments.1 They
hypothesis, that unconscious thought helps to make decisions, were given 3 min. During this time, the computer screen was blank except
should lead to better performance in the unconscious thought for a clock indicating how much time they had left. After 3 min, partici-
condition relative to the immediate decision condition. pants answered the attitude questions.
The conscious thought condition is a little more complex be- In the unconscious thought condition, participants performed a distractor
cause it is not purely a conscious thought condition. Making task aimed at preventing conscious thought: the n-back task (e.g., Jonides
people think consciously does not stop them from thinking uncon- et al., 1997). In this task, participants are presented with a series of digits,
and for each digit they have to decide whether it matches the digit that
sciously. The second hypothesis about the relative inferiority of
preceded it by n places. Here participants completed a 2-back task. This
conscious thought should not necessarily lead to worse perfor- demanding task affects executive functioning quite severely and can there-
mance under conscious thought conditions than under immediate fore be expected to successfully eliminate conscious thought. Participants
decision conditions, because the former group benefits from un- performed the 2-back task for 3 min (including a 20-s instruction screen).
conscious thought. Instead, the second hypothesis implies that the A number between 1 and 9 appeared on the screen every second, and
conscious thought condition should show inferior performance participants had to indicate a match by pressing the space bar. After 3 min,
relative to the unconscious thought condition. they were asked to complete the attitude questions. In general, participants
did well on the n-back task, except for 3 participants who erred on over
10% of the trials. These participants were not taken into account in further
Experiment 1 analyses. After completing the attitude questions, all participants were
debriefed, thanked, and dismissed.
Method
Participants and design. Sixty-three undergraduate students (48
Results
women and 15 men) from the University of Amsterdam were randomly It was first confirmed that overall, the attractive apartment was
assigned to one of three conditions: an immediate decision condition, a judged as more attractive than the unattractive apartment. Indeed,
conscious thought condition, and an unconscious thought condition. They
the overall attitude toward the attractive apartment was higher
either received course credits or money (5; approximately US$6) for their
(M ! 6.18) than the attitude toward the unattractive apartment
participation.
Procedure and materials. The experiment was the last in a longer (M ! 5.38), with the attitude toward the two fillers falling in
session with various unrelated experiments. Participants worked in separate between (Ms ! 5.74 and 5.69).
cubicles. The experiment was described as an experiment on decision The measure of interest is how well participants could differ-
making. Participants were told that they would be presented with informa- entiate between the attractive apartment and the unattractive apart-
tion about four hypothetical apartments in Amsterdam (labeled Apartments ment. Hence, difference scores were calculated by subtracting the
1 4). All apartments were described by positive (e.g., a very nice area) attitude toward the unattractive apartment from the attitude toward
and negative (e.g., rather noisy) attributes. Participants were asked to the attractive apartment. Both participants in the immediate deci-
form an impression of the apartments, and they were told that they would sion condition (M ! 0.47, SD ! 1.71) and in the conscious thought
be asked to choose one of the apartments at a later stage.
condition (M ! 0.44, SD ! 1.48) performed poorly. Their scores
Information was then presented about the four apartments. Each apart-
did not significantly differ from zero (ts " 1.15), indicating no
ment was described by 12 attributes, for a total of 48 pieces of information.
These 48 attributes were presented in random order. Each attribute was clear preference for the attractive apartment. Participants in the
presented for 4 s in the center of the screen, automatically followed by the unconscious thought condition did better (M ! 1.23, SD ! 2.05).
next attribute. It is important to note that Apartment 2 was the most Their score was higher than zero, t(21) ! 2.75, p " .02.
attractive apartment, with 8 positive (e.g., Apartment 2 is in the city It was expected that unconscious thought would outperform
center) and 4 negative (e.g., Apartment 2 has an unfriendly landlord) participants in the immediate decision condition. The second hy-
attributes. Apartment 4 was the worst apartment, with 4 positive and 8 pothesis was that conscious thought would be maladaptive, which
negative attributes. Apartments 1 and 3 were of medium attractiveness, should lead to underperformance of conscious thinkers relative to
with 6 positive and 6 negative attributes. The last two apartments can be unconscious thinkers. It became clear after an inspection of the cell
seen as fillers, used so participants were presented with a large amount of
means that female and male participants may have responded
information.
differentially to our manipulation, so this factor was included in
The stimulus information was assembled as follows. A large number of
attributes were pretested, and all extremely negative or extremely positive the analyses. A 3 (experimental condition) # 2 (sex of participant)
attributes were excluded. When students were asked which attributes they analysis of variance (ANOVA) yielded a significant effect of
found important, two stood out: size of the apartment and cost. These condition, F(2, 54) ! 3.40, p " .05, and a nonsignificant two-way
dimensions were used with care in that items were phrased to make them interaction, F(2, 54) ! 2.79, p " .08. The three conditions were
not too extreme (e.g., Apartment 2 is fairly large rather than Apartment compared via three separate ANOVAs. As expected, a comparison
2 is enormous). No attributes were used more than once, although some
attribute dimensions were used twice (e.g., one was in a nice area, another
1
in a troublesome area). Original instructions were in Dutch.

139
590 DIJKSTERHUIS

between the immediate decision condition and the conscious ments were described by both positive and negative attributes. They were
thought condition did not yield any significant results. Participants asked to form an impression of the four apartments, and they were told that
in the unconscious thought condition performed better than par- they would be asked to choose one of the apartments at a later stage.
ticipants in the immediate decision condition, F(1, 37) ! 4.96, p " Information was then presented about the four apartments. Each apart-
ment was described by 15 attributes, for a total of 60 pieces of information.
.04. This effect was qualified by a two-way interaction of equal
As opposed to Experiment 1, the attributes were not presented in random
magnitude, F(1, 37) ! 4.96, p " .04, showing that male partici-
order. Participants were first given the 15 attributes describing Apartment
pants were especially sensitive to our manipulation. They scored 1. All 15 attributes appeared at once and were presented as a list. After 12 s,
very poorly in the immediate decision condition (M ! #1.00, the list for Apartment 2 appeared to the right of the list for Apartment 1.
SD ! 2.16) and exceptionally well in the unconscious thought Again after 12 s, a third list describing Apartment 3 was added, and after
condition (M ! 2.00, SD ! 1.83). This difference was absent for a further 12 s, the list for Apartment 4 was added. The four lists remained
female participants (both Ms ! 0.87). Finally, a comparison be- on the screen for another 12 s, after which all the information disappeared.
tween the conscious and unconscious thought conditions showed Apartment 3 was the most attractive apartment, with 8 positive, 4 negative,
the predicted main effect of condition. Participants in the uncon- and 3 neutral attributes. Because participants were asked to choose an
scious thought condition outperformed those in the conscious apartment rather than indicating their attitude toward each apartment, no
particularly unattractive apartment was included. The three remaining
thought condition, although this effect was not significant, F(1,
apartments (i.e., Apartments 1, 2, and 4) were all characterized by 5
37) ! 3.47, p " .08. No other effects were significant (Fs " 1).
positive, 6 negative, and 4 neutral attributes.
To summarize, participants who could only engage in uncon- After participants read all the information, they were randomly allocated
scious thought were able to differentiate between the attractive to one of three conditions. The conditions were the same as in Experiment 1.
apartment and the unattractive apartment, whereas participants Afterward, participants answered two questions. They were first asked,
who either were allowed to think consciously or were not allowed If you had to choose one of the apartments, which one would you
to think at all could not, thereby supporting the two hypotheses. choose? They indicated their answer by typing the corresponding number.
The second question pertained to the way they reached their choice. They
were asked, Is your choice based on a more global judgment, or is your
Experiment 2 choice based on only one or two specific attributes? Participants answered
Experiment 2 served various purposes. One goal was to replicate by clicking on one of two boxes, labeled global and specific. After
completing the attitude questions, participants were debriefed and
the effects of Experiment 1. However, rather than asking partici-
dismissed.
pants to evaluate each apartment separately, they were now asked
to choose one of the apartments. In addition, it is shown above that
conscious thought can be maladaptive because of the low capacity Results
of consciousness. Therefore, consciousness is not able to deal with Because of the extremely low number of male participants (only
a large amount of information and must resort to focusing on a 3 in the unconscious thought condition), sex of participant was not
limited subset. In concrete terms, when choosing between four included in the analyses. The percentages of participants choosing
apartments, each described with 12 attributes, consciousness the attractive apartment were compared. As expected, participants
would not be able to take all the information into account and in the unconscious thought condition most often made the right
would, by necessity, focus on only a few attributes. Unconscious choice (59.3%). Participants in the conscious thought condition
thought, however, was expected not to suffer from capacity prob- and the immediate decision condition did not perform as well
lems. It should therefore be easier for unconscious thought to form (47.1% and 36.4% made correct choices, respectively). Chi-square
a more global judgment based on all (or almost all) information. tests demonstrated that the goal to make the decision problem
This possibility was investigated by asking participants, after they easier was successful. In all conditions, participants performed
chose an apartment, to indicate whether their choice was based on better than chance, all !2s(92, N ! 93) $ 5.19, ps " .03. Further-
a global impression or on only one or two specific attributes. more, the difference between the unconscious thought condition
The paradigm changed a little from Experiment 1, in which the and the immediate decision condition was significant, !2(59, N !
decision-making task was very taxing, because participants in two 60) ! 3.13, p " .04, one-tailed.
out of three conditions could not discriminate between the best and The way participants reached their choice differed between
worst apartments. One reason for this experienced difficulty is that conditions. In the immediate decision condition, 42.4% of the
the information about the apartments was presented in random participants indicated they made a global judgment. This percent-
order. It was decided to present the information in Experiment 2 age was lower for participants in the conscious thought condition
about each apartment individually in a fixed order. (26.5%) and higher in the unconscious thought condition (55.6%).
These latter two percentages differed significantly, !2(60, N !
Method 61) ! 6.69, p " .01, one-tailed. The way participants reached their
choice was related to its quality. Across participants, the correla-
Participants and design. Ninety-four undergraduate students (80
tion between the answers on the two questions was .22 ( p " .04).
women and 14 men) from the University of Amsterdam were randomly
assigned to one of three conditions: an immediate decision condition, a
Participants who made global judgments more often chose the
conscious thought condition, and an unconscious thought condition. They attractive apartment.
either received course credits or money (5) for their participation.
Procedure and materials. Participants were seated in a cubicle. The Experiment 3
experiment was described as an experiment on decision making. Partici-
pants were told that they would be presented with information about four Experiment 3 was designed to replicate the finding of superior
hypothetical apartments in Amsterdam (called Apartments 1 4). All apart- unconscious thought with different stimulus materials. In Experi-

140
MERITS OF UNCONSCIOUS THOUGHT 591

ments 1 and 2, apartments were chosen as objects in the hope that They were asked to indicate their liking for each face, either after
students would find them both relevant and, to some extent, receiving the instruction to merely rate each face or after being
appealing. In Experiment 3, however, potential roommates were instructed to carefully think about the reasons for their liking. As
used as objects. This is as relevant, but possibly more appealing, one might expect, peoples weighing of the different dimensions in
than apartments because participants would be thinking about determining their liking of the faces was more variable after
people. thinking. However, they were also decidedly more inconsistent. In
In the first two experiments, quality of judgment was evaluated light of these findings, it is not appropriate to conclude that
from a normative perspective. One apartment was better than conscious thought is necessarily good at weighing the importance
another because it was characterized by more positive attributes of attributes.
and fewer negative attributes. However, people differ as to which The second widely held assumption is that the unconscious is
attributes they find most important. Some people may be willing to not good at weighing different attributes. However, intuitive as this
live in a very small apartment as long as it is in the city center, assumption is, there is no evidence that the unconscious is not able
whereas others need more space and prefer to move to the suburbs. to deal with subtleties such as weighing the importance of at-
Likewise, it is easier for most people to list the appealing aspects tributes. Conversely, there are reasons to believe it can. The
of a holiday in Tuscany (great cities and towns, lots of art, a evidence by Betsch et al. (2001) discussed above strongly suggests
beautiful countryside, great food, good wine, etc.) than a holiday that the unconscious can weigh information. In addition, there is
on a Spanish costa (one can swim and choose between dozens of more general evidence that the unconscious can deal with many
bars each night to get a drink). Still, many people prefer the subtle processes quite wellindeed, often better than conscious-
Spanish costas because they are not interested in the highlights of ness can. The unconscious can sense minor differences between
Tuscany. In other words, to reach a sound decision people should stimuli that consciousness cannot (Dijksterhuis & Aarts, 2003;
give different attributes different, idiosyncratic weights. Marcel, 1983; Pierce & Jastrow, 1884). The unconscious can
One may argue that this weighing of attributes of unequal develop preferences for people and objects, whereas consciousness
importance is a task at which consciousness excels. Although is not even aware of these people or objects, as shown by research
intuitively logical, this remains to be seen. There are two separate on mere exposure (e.g., Kunst-Wilson & Zajonc, 1980) and sub-
assumptions behind this idea: (a) Consciousness is good at weigh- liminal evaluative conditioning (Dijksterhuis, 2004; Krosnick,
ing attributes, and (b) the unconscious is not very good at it. There Betz, Jussim, & Lynn, 1992). The unconscious can master wildly
is reason to disagree with both. difficult tasks that consciousness cannot master at all, even if
First, is consciousness good at assigning appropriate weights to pressed (e.g., Hassin & Bargh, 2003; Lewicki, Hill, & Czyzewska,
attributes? When the decision problem is simple, consciousness is 1992). The unconscious can decide to behave more intelligently
indeed likely to be good. As argued in the introduction, if one is without any conscious mediation (Bargh, Gollwitzer, Lee-Chai,
faced with an apartment with many wildly positive attributes and Barndollar, & Trotschel, 2001; Dijksterhuis & van Knippenberg,
a single critical negative one (it is much too expensive), conscious- 1998). Finally, recent research has shown that the unconscious can
ness will likely be good at quickly deciding against it. However, force a person to behave intelligently when consciousness is trying
when the situation is much more complex, the low capacity of to obstruct this (Wegner, Fuller, & Sparrow, 2003).
consciousness should obstruct this weighing process. What if one In Experiment 3, peoples decisions were investigated from both
finds three attributes very important, four attributes moderately a normative perspective (as in Experiments 1 and 2) and a sub-
important, four attributes rather unimportant, and two attributes jective perspective. The information about the alternatives was
not important at all? again presented in random order, as in Experiment 1. However, to
A telling example comes from work by Wilson et al. (1993). avoid making the task too difficult, only one filler alternative was
Participants were presented with five posters and asked to choose presented, rather than two as in Experiment 1.
one to take home. Later, they were called and asked how satisfied
they were with their choicea wonderful measure of whether they Method
made the right decision from a subjective point of view. Before
Participants and design. One hundred forty-five undergraduate stu-
choosing, some participants thought about the posters for a little
dents (107 women and 38 men) from the University of Amsterdam par-
while, whereas others were asked to carefully analyze the pros and ticipated in the experiment. They were randomly assigned to one of three
cons of each poster. It turned out that people who carefully conditions: an immediate decision condition, a conscious thought condi-
analyzed were less satisfied than people who merely thought about tion, and an unconscious thought condition. They either received course
them. That is, people who pressed consciousness to carefully credits or money (10; approximately US$12) for their participation.
weigh the various attributes made relatively poor decisions. Wil- Procedure and materials. The experiment was the last experiment in a
son and colleagues (see, e.g., Wilson, Dunn, Kraft, & Lisle, 1989) longer session with multiple, unrelated experiments. Participants worked in
explained these and other findings by claiming that too much separate cubicles. The experiment was announced as an experiment on
conscious reasoning increases the weight that people attach to decision making. Participants were asked to imagine they would have to
reasons that are very accessible and easy to verbalize. These find a roommate to share an apartment with (it is very common for
Amsterdam undergraduates to have one or more roommates). They were
reasons are not always the ones that should receive more weight.
told they would be presented with information about three hypothetical
In essence, conscious reasoning leads to a weighing process, but roommates that were all described by various positive and negative at-
the weights are wrong. tributes. They were asked to form an impression of the three roommates
Levine, Halberstadt, and Goldstone (1996) obtained evidence and to choose between the roommates at a later stage.
further supporting this claim. In their work, participants were Each roommate was described with 12 attributes, for a total of 36 pieces
presented with 60 faces that differed on a number of dimensions. of information. These 36 attributes were presented in random order. Each

141
592 DIJKSTERHUIS

attribute was presented for 2 s in the center of the screen, followed, after p # .01, and than participants in the conscious thought condition,
a 0.5-s pause, by the next attribute. Roommate A was the most attractive F(1, 84) ! 4.60, p # .04. The comparison between the uncon-
person, with 8 positive and 4 negative attributes, whereas Roommate C was scious thought condition and the conscious thought condition also
the least attractive, with 4 positive and 8 negative attributes. Roommate B revealed a significant two-way interaction, F(1, 84) ! 4.03, p #
was of medium attractiveness, with 6 positive and 6 negative attributes. All
.05. Male participants performed poorly in the conscious thought
roommates were described on the same 12 dimensions (e.g., how friendly,
condition (M ! 1.56) and very well in the unconscious thought
how neat). Roommate A and C were exact opposites. On the 8 dimensions
where Roommate A was described as positive (e.g., has fun friends, is condition (M ! 3.44), whereas this difference was almost absent
neat), Roommate C was described as negative (e.g., has annoying for female participants (Ms ! 3.00 and 3.06, respectively). The
friends, is very messy), and vice versa. Roommate B partly overlapped conscious thought condition and the immediate decision condition
with both of them. The choice for the attributes used was based on pilot did not differ from each other, F(1, 94) ! 0.55, but the comparison
testing aimed at assessing which attributes people find important. Only did reveal a main effect of sex of participant, F(1, 94) ! 6.42, p #
attributes that students indicated were at least moderately important were .02. Female participants (M ! 2.69) outperformed male partici-
usedat least 4.6 on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 (extremely unimpor- pants (M ! 1.65).
tant) to 7 (extremely important) but they were phrased in ways that made In all experiments so far, preferences were evaluated from a
them not too extreme.
normative perspective. In this experiment, ratings people gave
After participants read all information, they were randomly allocated to
about the importance of the 12 dimensions used to describe the
one of three conditions. In the immediate decision condition, they were
immediately asked to give their attitude toward each of the three room- three roommates were used to look at subjective preferences. For
mates. Participants were asked to indicate their answers on 9-point scales each participant, the scores on the 8 dimensions on which the
ranging from 1 (extremely negative) to 9 (extremely positive). All partici- attractive roommate was described as positive and the unattractive
pants answered the questions in the same order, starting with Roommate A roommate was described as negative were added. The scores on
and ending with Roommate C. the 4 dimensions on which the attractive roommate was described
The conditions were the same as in Experiments 1 and 2, except that the as negative and the unattractive roommate was described as pos-
time people were given to think (in the conscious thought condition) or the itive were then subtracted. The higher the resulting score, the more
time people were distracted (in the unconscious thought condition) was favorable a participant should be about the attractive roommate
now 4 min. In the unconscious thought condition, participants were dis-
and the more unfavorable a participant should be about the unat-
tracted with an anagram task. They were presented with one anagram at a
tractive roommate. The scores were correlated with the difference
time (e.g., ecipbs); the next anagram appeared after participants had solved
the anagram (biceps), or after 45 s had passed. The task was interrupted score of the attitudes toward the attractive roommate and the
after 4 min, and participants were then asked to complete the attitude unattractive roommate. The higher the correlation, the better the
questions. participants preference is from a subjective perspective. That is,
After a filler task that lasted about 4 min, all participants were asked to the higher the correlation, the more a participant chose the one he
indicate how important they found various attributes of people when trying or she should have chosen according to his or her own weighing of
to find a roommate. They were asked to indicate the importance of each of the attributes. The correlation in the immediate decision condition
the 12 dimensions used in the experiment (e.g., How important is it for was significant, r(51) ! .39, p # .005. The correlation in the
you that a roommate is neat?). Participants indicated their answers on conscious condition was nonsignificant, r(47) ! .21, p # .17,
7-point scales ranging from 1 (very unimportant) to 7 (very important). The
whereas the correlation in the unconscious thought condition was
12 questions were presented in random order.
the largest of the three, r(41) ! .48, p # .002. On the basis of these
data, there is no reason to assume that conscious thought helps to
Results make better decisions from a subjective point of view, nor is there
any reason to assume that unconscious thought hampers this pro-
It was first confirmed that the desirable roommate was judged to cess. The data point in the opposite direction, although it should be
be more positive than the undesirable roommate. Indeed, the noted that the correlations do not differ from each other signifi-
overall attitude toward the desirable roommate was higher (M ! cantly. The comparison between the unconscious and conscious
6.85) than that toward the undesirable (M ! 4.20), with the attitude thought conditions failed to reach significance ( p ! .16).2
toward the filler roommate falling in between (M ! 5.45).
Difference scores were computed by subtracting the attitude
toward the least attractive roommate from the attitude toward the What Is Unconscious Thought? Experiments 4 and 5
most attractive one. As expected, participants in the unconscious
The first three experiments show that participants in the uncon-
thought condition scored highest (M ! 3.15, SD ! 1.92). Partic-
scious thought condition generally outperform participants in the
ipants in the immediate decision condition scored lowest (M !
remaining two conditions. However, it is not yet clear what exactly
2.08, SD ! 1.80), with the participants in the conscious thought
happens during the unconscious thought period. Experiments 4 and
condition falling in between (M ! 2.72, SD ! 1.60). These scores
5 were designed to shed more light on this process.
were reliably higher than zero, as confirmed by t tests, all ts(50, 47,
A number of different processes can take place during distrac-
45) " 7, ps # .01, indicating that in all conditions participants
tion. One should distinguish between processes whereby the role
preferred the attractive roommate.
of the unconscious is passive and a process whereby the uncon-
A 3 (experimental condition) $ 2 (sex of participant) ANOVA
revealed a main effect of condition, F(2, 133) ! 4.69, p # .02, and
a nonsignificant two-way interaction, F(2, 133) ! 2.34, p # .13. 2
Another reason for being careful is that the ratings of importance of
Participants in the unconscious thought condition did better than dimensions were administered after rather than before people gave their
participants in the immediate decision condition, F(1, 88) ! 8.07, attitudes toward the different roommates.

142
MERITS OF UNCONSCIOUS THOUGHT 593

scious engages in active thought. Most findings in the domain of ious aspects was assessed. This was done to test the hypothesis of
incubation are explained by processes whereby the role of the greater polarization. Is it true that unconscious thought leads to
unconscious is passive. better judgment because representations become more polarized?
Schooler and Melcher (1995) reviewed findings showing that This should mean that positive aspects of the attractive alternative
distraction can lead to the change of a mental set. Here, the role and negative aspects of the unattractive alternative should come to
of the unconscious is proposed to be passive. People often ap- dominate the representation. Both accuracy and speed of recogni-
proach a problem with wrong cues, wrong heuristics, and/or wrong tion were assessed to answer these questions. Accuracy is indica-
information. Following a period of distraction, wrong approaches tive of availability of information. Polarization may lead to rela-
become less accessible or are forgotten altogether. The effects of tively accurate recognition of positive aspects of the attractive
distraction on a change of mental set can be either fairly strong alternative and negative aspects of the unattractive alternative.
(such as when one tries to solve a chess problem and initially gets Speed is indicative of the accessibility of information. Polarization
truly fixed in thinking along a wrong path) or relatively subtle may lead to higher accessibility of positive aspects of the attractive
(such as when distraction merely attenuates the biasing influence alternative and negative aspects of the unattractive alternative
of primacy or recency effects). One could lump these processes relative to other information.
together under the umbrella of the fresh look explanation: Putting Experiment 5 was designed to test the clustering hypothesis. Is
a problem aside for a while allows for a fresh, unbiased new start. it true that unconscious thought leads to better organization in
These effects notwithstanding, here it is proposed that the un- memory whereby information is more meaningfully clustered?
conscious also actively thinks. In the first three experiments, The paradigm differs from the one used in Experiment 3. Partic-
participants in the unconscious thought conditions were better able ipants were given behavioral information about a person. In this
to distinguish an attractive alternative from an unattractive alter- information, three trait dimensions were hidden. Some informa-
native. A reasonable assumption is that the superior judgments of tion suggested that the stimulus person was intelligent, some that
the unconscious thinkers were based on their representations of the the person was extroverted, and some that the person was politi-
various alternatives in memory. These participants were distracted cally left-wing. If the unconscious is a better integrator of infor-
for a while, and when they were asked to judge the various mation, unconscious thought should lead participants to organize
alternatives, they must have relied on the representations they their memory more along these three trait dimensions than partic-
retrieved from memory. Because their judgments were better than ipants in the remaining two conditions.
the judgments made by participants in the other conditions, their
representations were also somehow better (or at least different). Experiment 4
Their representations must have changed during distraction.
The hypothesis that representations change over time is akin to Method
the idea that people engage in thought. The term to think is derived
Participants and design. One hundred fourteen undergraduate students
from the Latin verb cogitare, which literally means to shake
(88 women and 26 men) from the University of Amsterdam participated in
together (Koestler, 1964). This meaning reflects the process of the experiment. They were randomly assigned to one of three conditions:
unconscious thought proposed here quite well. If people are pre- an immediate decision condition, a conscious thought condition, and an
sented with a lot of information in a relatively short period of time, unconscious thought condition. They either received course credits or
the resulting representation in memory is likely to be disorganized. money (10; approximately US$12) for their participation.
Individual pieces of information still have to be associated and Procedure and materials. Experiment 4 was exactly the same as Ex-
integrated. Unconscious thought, it is proposed, does exactly this. periment 3, with one exception. Rather than measuring attitudes, we
That is, unconscious thought is expected to turn an initial, disor- assessed recognition. After participants read the information about the
ganized set of information into a clearer and more integrated roommates (in the immediate condition), or after either thinking for 4 min
(in the conscious thought condition) or after being distracted for 4 min (in
representation of information in memory. It is quite possible that
the unconscious thought condition), they were presented with a recognition
the representations can change in various ways. Given the results
task. One by one, in random order, all 36 aspects of the roommates were
of the experiments above, it is likely that unconscious thought presented to them again. This time the aspects were presented without
leads to representations that become more polarizedthat is, the roommate labels. Participants task was to quickly decide whether an
representations of moderately positive alternatives become more aspect belonged to Roommate A, B, or C by pressing a corresponding
dominated by positive aspects, whereas the representations of button.
negative alternatives become more negative over time. A second
possibility is that unconscious thought, by a process of continued Results
associative activity, results in a more organized representation. It
could lead to greater clustering, where pieces of information that No effects or even trends of sex of participants were obtained
load on the same dimension or pertain to the same aspect become (all Fs ! .43), so this factor was not further investigated.
clustered. Recognition accuracy. First, the proportion of correct recog-
Experiments 4 and 5 were designed to test the hypothesis that nition of positive and negative aspects for the three roommates
people engage in active unconscious thought and to shed light on were calculated separately. These proportions were subjected to a
the nature of this process. Experiment 4 was designed to test the 3 (condition: immediate vs. conscious thought vs. unconscious
polarization hypothesis, and Experiment 5 was designed to test the thought) between-subjects " 3 (roommate: attractive vs. unattrac-
clustering hypothesis. In Experiment 4, the same paradigm was tive vs. neutral filler) " 2 (valence of aspect: positive vs. negative)
used as in Experiment 3, but rather than measuring peoples within-subjects ANOVA. A main effect of condition was found,
attitudes toward the different alternatives, recognition of the var- F(2, 111) # 5.29, p ! .01, indicating that participants in the

143
594 DIJKSTERHUIS

immediate condition had higher proportions of correct recognition. on the accuracy data. They were first analyzed with a 3 (condition:
This is not surprising, because these participants did not pause immediate vs. conscious thought vs. unconscious thought) be-
between information acquisition and recognition. More interesting, tween-subjects ! 3 (roommate: attractive vs. unattractive vs. neu-
this main effect was qualified by a two-way interaction of Condi- tral filler) ! 2 (valence of aspect: positive vs. negative) within-
tion ! Roommate, F(2, 111) " 2.91, p # .03. Superior recognition subjects ANOVA. The only reliable effect was the two-way
in the immediate condition accurately recognized information of interaction of Condition ! Roommate, F(2, 111) " 3.10, p # .02.
all roommates, whereas participants in the other conditions showed Participants in the immediate condition recognized information
impaired recognition of information of the neutral filler roommate. about all three roommates with the same speed, whereas partici-
It seems that both conscious and unconscious thinkers focused on pants in the other two conditions showed slower recognition of
the attractive and unattractive roommate and forgot the irrele- information about the neutral roommate.
vant filler. The same analysis was done on the speed of recognition for the
To investigate the possibility of a more polarized memory attractive and unattractive roommates only. It revealed a two-way
representation, the same analyses were done on the proportions of interaction of Roommate ! Valence of Aspect, F(1, 111) " 13.31,
recognition for the attractive and unattractive roommate only. A 3 p # .001, which indeed indicated that people polarized. Recogni-
(condition: immediate vs. conscious thought vs. unconscious tion of positive aspects of the attractive roommate and negative
thought) between-subjects ! 2 (roommate: attractive vs. unattrac- aspects of the unattractive roommate was clearly faster than rec-
tive) ! 2 (valence of aspect: positive vs. negative) within-subjects ognition of negative aspects of the attractive roommate and posi-
ANOVA revealed a two-way interaction of Roommate ! Valence tive aspects of the unattractive roommate. However, here the
of Aspect, F(1, 111) " 63.37, p # .0001, indicating that people predicted three-way interaction was significant, F(2, 111) " 4.19,
polarized. Recognition of positive aspects of the attractive room- p # .02. As can be seen in Figure 2, evidence for polarization was
mate and negative aspects of the unattractive roommate was su- only obtained for unconscious thinkers.
perior to memory for negative aspects of the attractive roommate The evidence for polarization of representations in memory is
and positive aspects of the unattractive roommate. It was predicted fairly strong. Although the evidence based on recognition accuracy
that the unconscious thinkers especially would polarize, but the is suggestive, the evidence based on speed is unequivocal. There is
three-way interaction was not significant, F(2, 111) " 2.23, p # .12. one caveat. Throughout the presentation of this experiment, the
As can be seen in Figure 1, people generally polarized, and term recognition was used even though it was essentially an
participants in the unconscious thought condition tended to do this allocation task. Participants did not have to recognize the aspects,
to an even greater degree. Pairwise contrasts were calculated to but they were asked to determine (or recognize) the source (Room-
compare recognition accuracy for positive and negative aspects for mate A, B, or C). Therefore, allocation biases played a role as well.
each roommate in each condition. Unconscious thinkers had better Presented with a positive aspect, people may have allocated it to
recognition for positive aspects of the attractive roommate than for the roommate they thought to be the most positive (Friendly?
negative aspects of the attractive roommate ( p # .001), whereas This must have been Roommate A, because I like him most).
the reverse was true for the unattractive roommate ( p # .003). In However, an allocation bias is based on expectations (I like A, so
both other conditions, one of the two contrasts failed to reach he must have this positive attribute), and these expectations are
significance ( ps $ .05). based on underlying representations. So although pure recognition
Recognition speed. In the analyses on speed of recognition, and allocation are different processes (one is mediated by expec-
only data for aspects that were recognized accurately were used. tations, whereas the other is not necessarily), effects in both reflect
The analyses performed on the speed data were the same as those effects of the same underlying representation. If one wants to

Figure 1. Experiment 4: Proportions of accurate recognition of positive (Pos) and negative (Neg) aspects of the
attractive (Attr) and unattractive (Unattr) roommate per condition.

144
MERITS OF UNCONSCIOUS THOUGHT 595

Figure 2. Experiment 4: Speed of recognition (in seconds) of positive (Pos) and negative (Neg) aspects of the
attractive (Attr) and unattractive (Unattr) roommate per condition .

explain the differences between conditions with differences in vs. unconscious thought) design. They either received course credits or
allocation bias, one still has to first assume differences in under- money (7; approximately US$9) for their participation.
lying representation. Procedure and materials. The experiment was the last experiment in a
longer session with multiple, unrelated experiments. Participants worked in
separate cubicles. The experiment was announced as an experiment on
Experiment 5 person perception. Participants were told they would be presented with
Experiment 5 was designed to investigate whether unconscious information about a person named Jeroen. They were told that they
would have to read various behavioral descriptions about Jeroen. Half the
thought leads to more integrated representations of information in
participants were asked to form an impression of him, whereas the other
memory. To test this, a paradigm was used that was used before by half were asked to memorize the information.
Hamilton, Katz, and Leirer (1980; see also Chartrand & Bargh, Subsequently, 18 short sentences were presented one by one on the
1996). In their work, participants were presented with information screen in random order. A sentence stayed on the screen for 5 s, with the
about a stimulus person and instructions to either form an impres- next sentence appearing after a pause of 0.5 s. All sentences were pretested
sion or to memorize the information. At a later stage, participants to load on one of three trait categories. Six of the sentences indicated
recall was assessed. It is interesting to note that participants with intelligence, 6 others indicated extroversion, and the remaining 6 were
an impression instruction recalled more information than people indicative of Jeroen being politically left-wing.
who were specifically asked to memorize the information. In After presentation of the sentences, participants in the immediate recall
addition, people with an impression instruction also showed a condition were given the recall task, during which they had 4 min to list as
many of the behavioral descriptions as possible. Participants in the con-
more integrated organization of information in memory, as as-
scious thought condition were asked to think about Jeroen for 4 min prior
sessed by the order of recall.3 to the recall task, whereas participants in the unconscious thought condi-
The present experiment examined whether unconscious thought tions were distracted for 4 min. The distractor task was the same as in
contributes to this improved organization of information in mem- Experiments 3 and 4.
ory. As in Hamilton et al. (1980), participants were presented with
behavioral information about a stimulus person. Each behavioral
description was indicative of one of three trait concepts. After- Results
ward, participants were either asked to recall the information
The number of descriptions recalled correctly was assessed
immediately, after some conscious thought, or after unconscious
using a gist criterion (Hamilton et al., 1980). The recall scores
thought. Our hypothesis was that unconscious thinkers would have
the highest clustering scores, indicative of more integrative mem-
ory organization. The two different instructions (impression vs. 3
The reason for using this paradigm rather than the paradigms used in
memory) were included. These are not directly related to the Experiments 1 4 is that the dimension that people can use to cluster should
current hypothesis; they were kept as a tribute to the original work. not be too obvious. Underlying trait constructs are excellent, but offering
people information about three individuals named A, B, and C presumably
Method leads to very strongand intentional clustering on the basis of these
three individuals (lets begin with listing what I remember of person A).
Participants and design. Sixty-nine undergraduate students (58 Adding a second, underlying dimension is a possibility, but this would
women and 11 men) from the University of Amsterdam participated in the require much more stimulus material. More stimulus material, however,
experiment. They were randomly assigned to the cells of a 2 (instruction: leads to poorer relative recall, which in turn makes clustering scores less
impression vs. memory) ! 3 (condition: immediate vs. conscious thought reliable.

145
596 DIJKSTERHUIS

were subjected to a 2 (instruction: impression vs. memory) ! 3 pared with conditions where people did not consciously think. The
(condition: immediate vs. conscious thought vs. unconscious relative inferiority of conscious thought was expected to be the
thought) between-participants ANOVA. No significant effects consequence of the low processing capacity of consciousness.
emerged. The absence of an effect of instruction may have been Some supporting evidence for this idea comes from Experiment 2.
caused by the fact that compared with the experiments by Hamil- Here, conscious thinkers reported that their decisions were often
ton et al. (1980) and Chartrand and Bargh (1996), our task was based on a few specific relevant attributes, whereas unconscious
easy. People were given few behavioral descriptions, and overall thinkers reported forming a more global judgment based on much
recall was high (over 50%). However, the immediate decision more information.
condition comes closest to the procedure in the original Hamilton Experiments 4 and 5 were designed to shed more light on the
et al. (1980) studies. Under these conditions, participants in the processes underlying unconscious thought. It was shown that un-
impression set condition indeed recalled more information than conscious thought led people to develop more polarized, clearer,
participants who received a memory set. This difference was not and more integrated representations in memory, supporting the
significant, F(1, 20) " 2.95, p # .11. proposed process underlying superior unconscious decision mak-
To assess organization in memory, conditional recall probabil- ing. It is interesting to note with respect to Experiment 5 that
ities were computed by looking at the order of recall. The number conscious thought almost completely prevented meaningful clus-
of same-trait sequences (e.g., an intelligent behavior recalled after tering. Clustering is an associative process, and it is likely that this
another intelligent behavior) was divided by the total number of process was disrupted because, as Wilson and others have shown
behaviors recalled minus one. The resulting probabilities are listed (e.g., Wilson et al., 1989; Wilson & Schooler, 1991; see also
in Table 1. A 2 (instruction: impression vs. memory) ! 3 (condi- Experiment 2 of the current article), conscious thought biases
tion: immediate vs. conscious thought vs. unconscious thought) people toward paying a lot of attention to very few attributes at the
between-participants ANOVA revealed both predicted main ef- expense of others. This may lead to meaningful associations be-
fects. First, in keeping with Hamilton et al. (1980) and Chartrand tween the few attributes that received attention, but it may hinder
and Bargh (1996), there was a main effect of instruction showing associative processes incorporating the other attributes.
that participants with an impression instruction (M " .44, SD " One avenue for further research concerns the goal directedness
.13) had higher clustering scores than participants with a memory of unconscious thought. In all experiments, participants were told
instruction (M " .35, SD " .14), F(1, 63) " 6.56, p # .02. In that they had to decide between various alternatives before they
addition, the predicted main effect of condition was obtained, F(2, engaged in unconscious thought. Would the unconscious have
63) " 4.32, p # .02. Participants in the unconscious thought engaged in relevant thinking had it not been instructed to do so?
condition had higher clustering scores (M " .46, SD " .11) than This is likely not the case. The literature on creativity and incu-
participants in the conscious thought condition (M " .37, SD " bation (e.g., Bowers et al., 1990; Smith & Blankenship, 1989) has
18), F(1, 43) " 5.50, p # .03, and than participants in the suggested that unconscious thought can be goal directed. The
immediate recall condition (M " .35, SD " .13), F(1, 42) " 9.58, quote about Newton in the introduction also points in this direc-
p # .005. These latter two conditions did not differ. It is interesting tion. He knew that he had to bear in mind what he needed to know.
that recall in random order (without meaningful clustering) would One could say that he gave his unconscious a clear goal. In
lead to a clustering score of about .30. Only participants in the addition, recent evidence has shown that goal activation and goal
unconscious thought condition had scores considerably higher than pursuit can be achieved unconsciously (Bargh et al., 2001; Char-
.30. In sum, only unconscious thought led to more integrated trand & Bargh, 1996). This also implies that unconscious thought
representations in memory. processes can be goal directed. However, this has not yet been
shown.
General Discussion
Unconscious thought improved the quality of decisions. When A Note on Incubation
people were faced with complex decisions, a few minutes of
distraction during which people could engage in unconscious As said above, empirical evidence for incubation is limited.
thought but not in conscious thoughtled to superior decisions People often have failed to find it, and the little evidence available
compared with circumstances under which people could not en- is hard to replicate (Olton, 1979). Furthermore, most effects that
gage in unconscious thought or to circumstances under which were obtained could be explained by processes whereby the role of
people engaged in conscious thought. Moreover, a few minutes of the unconscious was passive. It is possible that the nature of the
conscious thought generally did not lead to better decisions com- problems people had to solve was responsible for this state of
affairs. Researchers used insight problemsthe kind of problems
where the solution to a problem is very specific and often coun-
Table 1 terintuitive, resulting in a eureka experience. This choice is
Experiment 5: Clustering Scores as a Function of Condition understandable, because incubation is studied in the context of
creativity, and creativity is often (but not always; see Weisberg,
Condition
1995) the consequence of such insights. However, the unconscious
Instruction Immediate Conscious Unconscious may not have been given a fair chance. Solving insight problems
is often like trying to find a needle in a haystack, and the uncon-
Memory .29 .35 .41 scious may need more time than the amount people are given in a
Impression .42 .38 .52
lab experiment. The current research points to the possibility that

146
MERITS OF UNCONSCIOUS THOUGHT 597

incubation can be obtained even within minutes if more mundane, well laid out with few attributes that do allow for meaningful
analytical problems are used rather than insight problems. comparisons. If not, unconscious thought should be used to work
on more global or holistic impressions first.
Should We Stop Thinking? Some Potential Moderators One should not infer from the present work that consciousness
is rather stupid and the unconscious is smart. Rather than making
One may infer from the present work that it constitutes good such categorizations, it seems fruitful to more closely examine the
news for people who dislike the hard labor of conscious thinking. strong and weak points of both systems (see Claxton, 1997; Kihl-
The unconscious can be left to deal with making decisions while strom, Barnhardt, & Tataryn, 1992). General claims such as the
consciousness can be directed elsewhere, such as at fun things. unconscious is fast and consciousness is slow or the unconscious
This conclusion is clearly too bold. First of all, the findings is stupid and consciousness is smart do not make much sense. The
reported here should be placed in the context of the paradigm used. unconscious automatically evaluates stimuli within milliseconds
Both consciousness and the unconscious had to work within the (Bargh, Chaiken, Govender, & Pratto, 1992; Fazio, Sanbonmatsu,
confines of this paradigm. Perhaps the circumstances under which Powell, & Kardes, 1986), but Newton, who contemplated for
consciousness had to work diverged too strongly from how con- years, may not have agreed that the unconscious is fast. Likewise,
sciousness often deals with decision problems in real life. For one in this article, much evidence has been described implying that the
thing, consciousness (but also the unconscious) was only given a unconscious can be very smart, but when asked, What is the
few minutes to think. Maybe it performs better with more time. In square root of 625? your unconscious is not going to solve it. You
addition, conscious thought took place after participants had read could be given a distractor task lasting for months, but this ques-
the information. Maybe conscious thought would have led to better tion can only be answered (assuming no calculator is used) after
results if the information had been visually available during conscious work. Likewise, consciousness is neither always smart
thought. Hence, it is important to pit conscious and unconscious (as the current work shows) nor always very slow (it finds the
thought against each other in different paradigms before bold square root of 625 in seconds). The bottom line is that both
conclusions can be drawn. For now, it is more interesting to systems can be fast, slow, smart, or stupid. It all depends on what
speculate about when conscious versus unconscious thought may they are asked to do.
be more fruitful. Nevertheless, the current work demonstrates one thing the un-
As to when one mode of thought may be better than the other, conscious is good at: making complex decisions. When faced with
some educated hypotheses can be formulated. First, there are complex decisions such as where to work or where to live, do not
various reasons to propose the somewhat counterintuitive idea that think too much consciously. Instead, after a little initial conscious
the more complex a problem is, the less likely it becomes that information acquisition, avoid thinking about it consciously. Take
conscious thought can contribute much. If a problem is complex, your time and let the unconscious deal with it.
it by definition means that a lot of information has to be taken into
account. Conscious thought is not very good at this. One could say References
that conscious thought is very focused but not very encompassing
Bargh, J. A. (1997). The automaticity of everyday life. In R. S. Wyer Jr.
or inclusive. There are various (related) reasons for this. Con-
(Ed.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 10, pp 1 61). Mahwah, NJ:
sciousness has low capacity, and conscious reasoning biases the Erlbaum.
weights people attach to different pieces of information (e.g., Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Govender, R., & Pratto, F. (1992). The generality
Wilson et al., 1989). In addition, verbalizing information (an act of of the automatic evaluation effect. Journal of Personality and Social
consciousness) makes these biases even worse, as the work on Psychology, 62, 893912.
verbal overshadowing (Schooler et al., 1993) has demonstrated. If Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of
we are willing to assume that mundane decisions are often rela- being. American Psychologist, 54, 462 476.
tively simple, whereas vital matters are usually complex (which is Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., Lee-Chai, A., Barndollar, K., & Trotschel,
not always the case), we may conclude that Freud was right: Use R. (2001). The automated will: Nonconscious activation and pursuit of
consciousness for relatively simple and mundane decisions, but behavioral goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81,
1014 1027.
refrain from using it too much for more complex matters.
Betsch, T., Plessner, H., Schwieren, C., & Gutig, R. (2001). I like it but I
The observation that on some occasions weighing the impor- dont know why: A value-account approach to implicit attitude forma-
tance of attributes is easier than in other cases may also lead to a tion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 242253.
hypothesis. Sometimes weighing is easy or even unnecessary. Bowers, K. S., Regehr, G., Balthazard, C., & Parker, K. (1990). Intuition
When someone from the support staff comes to fix a software in the context of discovery. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 72110.
problem, all we care about is whether she is able to do the job Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1996). Automatic activation of impres-
properly in a reasonable amount of time. On the other hand, sion formation and memorization goals: Nonconscious goal priming
sometimes weighing attributes is mind-boggling, such as when reproduces effects of explicit task instructions. Journal of Personality
choosing a doctoral program. When weighing is easy, one can and Social Psychology, 71, 464 478.
make decisions by first comparing how well various alternatives Claxton, G. (1997). Hare brain, tortoise mind: How intelligence increases
when you think less. New York: HarperCollins.
score on the one or two attributes that matter. When weighing is
Dijksterhuis, A. (2004). I like myself but I dont know why: Enhancing
hard, one could start out by forming global impressions of the implicit self-esteem by subliminal evaluative conditioning. Journal of
different alternatives and then start to compare. It is likely that Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 345355.
conscious thought is more proficient in the former case, whereas Dijksterhuis, A., & Aarts, H. (2003). On wildebeests and humans: The
unconscious thought is better able to do the latter. If this is true, preferential detection of negative stimuli. Psychological Science, 14,
conscious thought may be fruitful when the decision problem is 14 18.

147
598 DIJKSTERHUIS

Dijksterhuis, A., & van Knippenberg, A. (1998). The relation between depends on the difficulty of the judgment. Journal of Personality and
perception and behavior, or how to win a game of Trivial Pursuit. Social Psychology, 68, 581594.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 865 877. Pierce, C. S., & Jastrow, J. (1884). On small differences in sensation.
Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). Memoirs of the National Academy of Science, 3, 75 83.
On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Schooler, J. W., & Melcher, J. (1995). The ineffability of insight. In S. M.
Social Psychology, 50, 229 238. Smith, T. B. Ward, & R. A. Finke (Eds.), The creative cognition
Gardner, H. (1993). Creating minds. New York: Basic Books. approach (pp. 97134). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Ghiselin, B. (1952). The creative process. New York: New American Schooler, J. W., Ohlsson, S., & Brooks, K. (1993). Thoughts beyond
Library. words: When language overshadows insight. Journal of Experimental
Gilbert, D. T. (1989). Thinking lightly about others: Automatic compo- Psychology: General, 122, 166 183.
nents of the social inference process. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh Smith, S. M. (1995). Fixation, incubation, and insight in memory and
(Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 189 211). New York: Guilford Press. creative thinking. In S. M. Smith, T. B. Ward, & R. A. Finke (Eds.), The
Hamilton, D. L., Katz, L. B., & Leirer, V. O. (1980). Organizational creative cognition approach (pp. 135156). Cambridge, MA: MIT
processes in impression formation. In R. Hastie, T. M. Ostrom, E. B. Press.
Ebbesen, R. S. Wyer Jr., D. L. Hamilton, & D. E. Carlston (Eds.), Smith, S. M., & Blankenship, S. E. (1989). Incubation effects. Bulletin of
Person memory: The cognitive basis of social perception (pp. 121153). the Psychonomic Society, 27, 311314.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Wegner, D. M., Fuller, V. A., & Sparrow, B. (2003). Clever hands:
Hassin, R., & Bargh, J. A. (2003). Implicit insight: The case for the on-line Uncontrolled intelligence in facilitated communication. Journal of Per-
extraction of rules. Manuscript under review. sonality and Social Psychology, 85, 519.
Jonides, J., Schumacher, E. H., Smith, E. E., Lauber, E. J., Awh, E., Wegner, D. M., & Smart, L. (1997). Deep cognitive activation: A new
Minoshima, S., & Koeppe, R. A. (1997). Verbal working memory load approach to the unconscious. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy-
affects regional brain activation as measured by PET. Journal of Cog- chology, 65, 984 995.
nitive Neuroscience, 9, 462 475. Weisberg, R. W. (1995). Case studies of creative thinking: Reproduction
Kihlstrom, J. F., Barnhardt, T. M., & Tataryn, D. (1992). The psycholog- versus restructuring in the real world. In S. M. Smith, T. B. Ward, &
ical unconscious: Found, lost, and regained. American Psychologist, 47, R. A. Finke (Eds.), The creative cognition approach (pp. 5372). Cam-
788 791. bridge, MA: MIT Press.
Koestler, A. (1964). The act of creation. London: Penguin. Whitehead, A. N. (1911). An introduction to mathematics. New York:
Krosnick, J. A., Betz, A. L., Jussim, L. J., & Lynn, A. R. (1992). Sublim- Henry Holt.
inal conditioning of attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulle- Wilson, T. D. (2002). Strangers to ourselves: Discovering the adaptive
tin, 18, 152162. unconscious. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Kunst-Wilson, W., & Zajonc, R. (1980, February 1). Affective discrimi- Wilson, T. D., Dunn, D. S., Kraft, D., & Lisle, D. J. (1989). Introspection,
nation of stimuli that cannot be recognized. Science, 207, 557558. attitude change, and attitude behavior consistency: The disruptive ef-
Levine, G. M., Halberstadt, J. B., & Goldstone, R. L. (1996). Reasoning fects of explaining why we feel the way we do. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),
and the weighing of attributes in attitude judgments. Journal of Person- Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 19, pp. 123205).
ality and Social Psychology, 70, 230 240. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Lewicki, P., Hill, T., & Czyzewska, M. (1992). Nonconscious acquisition Wilson, T. D., Lisle, D., Schooler, J. W., Hodges, S. D., Klaaren, K. J., &
of information. American Psychologist, 47, 796 801. LaFleur, S. J. (1993). Introspecting about reasons can reduce post-choice
Marcel, A. J. (1983). Conscious and unconscious perception: An approach satisfaction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 331339.
to the relations between phenomenal experience and perceptual pro- Wilson, T. D., & Schooler, J. W. (1991). Thinking too much: Introspection
cesses. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 238 300. can reduce the quality of preferences and decisions. Journal of Person-
Miller, G. A. (1956). Information theory. Scientific American, 195, 42 46. ality and Social Psychology, 60, 181192.
Nrretranders, T. (1998). The user illusion: Cutting consciousness down to Yaniv, I., & Meyer, D. E. (1987). Activation and metacognition of inac-
size. New York: Viking. cessible stored information: Potential bases for incubation effects in
Olton, R. (1979). Experimental studies of incubation: Searching for the problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Mem-
elusive. Journal of Creative Behavior, 13, 9 22. ory and Cognition, 13, 187205.
Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., & Johnson, E. J. (1988). Adaptive strategy
selection in decision making. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Received September 2, 2003
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 534 552. Revision received May 27, 2004
Pelham, B. W., & Neter, E. (1995). The effect of motivation on judgment Accepted June 2, 2004 !

Wanted: Old APA Journals!


APA is continuing its efforts to digitize older journal issues for the PsycARTICLES database.
Thanks to many generous donors, we have made great strides, but we still need many issues,
particularly those published in the 1950s and earlier.

If you have a collection of older journals and are interested in making a donation, please e-mail
journals@apa.org or visit http://www.apa.org/journals/donations.html for an up-to-date list of the
issues we are seeking.

148
Intelligence in Public Literature

T h in k in g, Fast an d S low
Da n iel Ka h n em a n , (New Yor k : Fa r r a r, St r a u s a n d Gir ou x, 2011), 418 pp.

Reviewed by Frank J. Babetski

Few books a r e m u st r ea ds for in t elligen ce Th e t it le of t h e book r efer s t o wh a t Ka h n e-


officer s. Fewer st ill a r e m u st r ea ds t h a t m en - m a n , a da pt in g a device t h a t ot h er r esea r ch er s
t ion In t elligen ce Com m u n it y fu n ct ion s or t h e or igin a lly pr oposed, ca lls t h e t wo syst em s of
CIA on ly on ce, a n d t h en on ly in pa ssin g. Da n - t h e h u m a n m in d. Syst em 1, or fa st t h in kin g,
iel Ka h n em a n h a s wr it t en on e of t h ese r a r e oper a t es a u t om a t ica lly a n d qu ickly wit h lit t le
books. Th in k in g, Fast an d S low r epr esen t s a n or n o effor t a n d n o sen se of volu n t a r y con t r ol.
elega n t su m m a t ion of a lifet im e of r esea r ch in Most Syst em 1 sk illssu ch a s det ect in g t h e
r ela t ive dist a n ces of object s, or ien t in g t o a su d-
wh ich Ka h n em a n , P r in cet on Un iver sit y P r ofes-
den sou n d, or det ect in g h ost ilit y in a voicea r e
sor E m er it u s of P sych ology a n d P u blic Affa ir s,
in n a t e a n d a r e fou n d in ot h er a n im a ls. Som e
a n d h is la t e colla bor a t or, Am os Tver sky,
fa st a n d a u t om a t ic Syst em 1 skills ca n be
ch a n ged t h e wa y psych ologist s t h in k a bou t a cqu ir ed t h r ou gh pr olon ged pr a ct ice, su ch a s
t h in kin g. Ka h n em a n , wh o won t h e 2002 Nobel r ea din g a n d u n der st a n din g n u a n ces of socia l
P r ize in E con om ics for h is wor k wit h Tver sky sit u a t ion s. E xper t s in a field ca n even u se Sys-
on pr ospect t h eor y, a lso h igh ligh t s t h e best t em 1 t o qu ickly, effor t lessly, a n d a ccu r a t ely
wor k of ot h er r esea r ch er s t h r ou gh ou t t h e book. r et r ieve st or ed exper ien ce t o m a ke com plex
T h in k in g, Fast an d S low in t r odu ces n o r evolu - ju dgm en t s. A ch ess m a st er qu ickly fin din g
t ion a r y n ew m a t er ia l, bu t it is a m a st er piece st r on g m oves a n d a qu a r t er ba ck ch a n gin g a
beca u se of t h e wa y Ka h n em a n wea ves exist in g play sen t t o h im fr om t h e sidelin e wh en h e r ec-
r esea r ch t oget h er. ogn izes a defen sive wea kn ess a r e exa m ples of
a cqu ir ed Syst em 1 t h in kin g.
E xper t in t elligen ce officer s a t CIA, a n
a gen cy wit h t h e h u m a n in t elligen ce m ission Syst em 2, or slow t h in kin g, a lloca t es a t t en -
a t it s cor e, h a ve com e t h r ou gh exper ien ce a n d t ion t o t h e m en t a l a ct ivit ies t h a t dem a n d
pr a ct ice t o u n der st a n d a n d exploit t h e h u m a n effor t , su ch a s com plex com pu t a t ion s a n d con -
cogn it ive pr ocesses of wh ich Ka h n em a n wr it es. sciou s, r ea son ed ch oices a bou t wh a t t o t h in k
a n d wh a t t o do. Syst em 2 r equ ir es m ost of u s t o
Th ese exper t officer s will h a ve m a n y m om en t s
pa y a t t en t ion t o do t h in gs su ch a s dr ive on a n
of r ecogn it ion in r ea din g t h is book, wh ich gives
u n fa m ilia r r oa d du r in g a sn owst or m , ca lcu la t e
a n em pir ica l u n der pin n in g for m u ch of t h eir
t h e pr odu ct of 17x24, sch edu le t r a n spor t a t ion
h a r d-won wisdom . for a t een a ge da u gh t er s a ct ivit ies, or u n der-
st a n d a com plex logica l a r gu m en t .
Ka h n em a n a lso m a y ch a llen ge som e st r on gly
h eld beliefs. T h in k in g, Fast an d S low gives Ka h n em a n focu ses m u ch of t h e book on t h e
exper t s a n d n ewer officer s, r ega r dless of t h e in t er a ct ion s of Syst em 1 a n d Syst em 2 a n d t h e
in t elligen ce a gen cy in wh ich t h ey ser ve, a n pr oblem s in h er en t in t h ose in t er a ct ion s. Bot h
en or m ou sly u sefu l cogn it ive fr a m ewor k u pon syst em s a r e on wh en we a r e a wa ke. Syst em 1
wh ich t o h a n g t h eir exper ien ces. r u n s a u t om a t ica lly a n d effor t lessly bu t

A ll statem en ts of fact, opin ion , or an alysis expressed in th is article are th ose of th e au th or. N oth in g in th is article sh ou ld be
con stru ed as assertin g or im plyin g U S govern m en t en d orsem en t of its factu al statem en ts an d in terpretation s.

Studies in Intelligence Vol. 56, No. 2 (Extracts, June 2012) 1


149
Thinking, Fast and Slow

Syst em 2 idles, beca u se u sin g it r equ ir es effor t A t a r get s bia ses pu t t h e pla u sible in pla u -
a n d is t ir in g. Syst em 1 gen er a t es im pr ession s sible den ia bilit y du r in g cover t a ct ion s. E ffec-
a n d feelin gs, wh ich becom e t h e sou r ce of Sys- t ive decept ion s a lso fu n da m en t a lly r ely on a
t em 2s explicit beliefs a n d deliber a t e ch oices. t a r get s u n ch a llen ged bia ses a n d so m a ke it
Syst em 1, wh en it en cou n t er s som et h in g it ca n - ea sy for t h e t a r get t o believe wh a t t h ey a lr ea dy
n ot qu ickly u n der st a n d a n d did n ot expect (in a r e pr edisposed t o believe. E ffect ive fa br ica -
ot h er wor ds, a su r pr ise), en list s Syst em 2 t o t or s, especia lly t h ose wit h t a n t a lizin g a ccess,
m a ke sen se of t h e a n om a ly. Th e a ler t ed Sys- r ely on ou r bia sed desir e t o believe t h em . On e
t em 2 t a kes ch a r ge, over r idin g Syst em 1s a u t o- or t wo pla u sible r epor t s fr om su ch a per son
m a t ic r ea ct ion s. Syst em 2 a lwa ys h a s t h e la st m ay be en ou gh t o en ga ge t h e exa gger a t ed em o-
wor d wh en it ch ooses t o a sser t it . t ion a l coh er en ce or h a lo effect . Rou gh ly pu t ,
on ce la zy Syst em 2 is sa t isfied, it t en ds t o defer
Th e syst em s oper a t e t o m in im ize effor t a n d t o Syst em 1, wh ich in t u r n pr oject s posit ive
m a xim ize per for m a n ce a n d a r e t h e r esu lt of qu a lit ies in on e a r ea in t o a gen er a lized posi-
h u n dr eds of t h ou sa n ds of yea r s of h u m a n evo- t ive a ssessm en t .
lu t ion in ou r en vir on m en t . Th ey wor k
ext r em ely well, u su a lly. Syst em 1 per for m s Ter r or ist s r ely on t h ese bia ses, bu t t h ey a r e
well a t m a kin g a ccu r a t e m odels a n d pr edic- a lso vu ln er a ble t o t h em . Ter r or ism wor ks
t ion s in fa m ilia r en vir on m en t s. Syst em 1 h a s beca u se it pr ovides ext r em ely vivid im a ges of
t wo sign ifica n t wea kn esses: it is pr on e t o m a ke dea t h a n d dest r u ct ion , wh ich con st a n t m edia
syst em ic er r or s in specified sit u a t ion st h ese a t t en t ion m a gn ifies. Th ese im a ges a r e im m edi-
a r e bia sesa n d it ca n n ot be t u r n ed off. Sys- a t ely a va ila ble t o a t a r get s Syst em 1.
t em 2 ca n , wit h effor t , over r u le t h ese bia ses if Syst em 2, even wh en a r m ed wit h r elia ble st a -
it r ecogn izes t h em . Un for t u n a t ely, Syst em 2 is t ist ics on t h e r a r it y of a n y t ype of t er r or ist
dem on st r a bly ver y poor a t r ecogn izin g on es even t , ca n n ot over com e Syst em 1s a ssocia t ive
own bia sed t h in kin g. Tr yin g t o en ga ge Syst em r ea ct ion t o specific even t s. If you a r e a CIA offi-
2 a t a ll t im es t o pr even t Syst em 1 er r or s is cer wh o wa s wor kin g in La n gley on 25 J a n u a r y
im pr a ct ica l a n d exh a u st in g. 1993, t h en ch a n ces a r e t h a t you ca n n ot m a ke
t h e left t u r n in t o t h e com pou n d fr om Dolley
In t er m s of Ka h n em a n s con st r u ct , a sign ifi- Ma dison Bou leva r d wit h ou t t h in kin g of Aim a l
ca n t pa r t of t h e m ission s of in t elligen ce a gen - Ka si, t h e Pa kist a n i wh o killed t wo CIA officer s
cies boils down t o seizin g oppor t u n it ies a n d wou n ded t h r ee ot h er s a t t h a t in t er sect ion
pr esen t ed by t h e fla wed in t er a ct ion s of t h e Sys- t h a t day.
t em 1 a n d Syst em 2 t h in kin g of for eign a ct or s
wh ile a t t h e sa m e t im e r ecogn izin g a n d m it i- Th e 9/11 h ija cker s on t h e fir st t h r ee pla n es
ga t in g t h e fla ws of t h eir own Syst em 1 a n d cou ld cou n t on pa ssen ger s t o st a y sea t ed, r ely-
Syst em 2 in t er a ct ion s. H ost ile ser vices a n d in g on t h eir a bilit y t o qu ickly r em em ber
or ga n iza t ion s t r y t o do t h e sa m e t h in g in a ccou n t s of pr eviou s h ija ckin gs in wh ich t h e
r et u r n . Oper a t ion s officer s r ely on t h e bia ses of h ija cker s wer e m ot iva t ed t o su r vivet h is is
for eign cou n t er in t elligen ce officer s, essen t ia lly wh a t Ka h n em a n ca lls t h e a va ila bilit y bia s.
a dvisin g a sset s t o a void excit in g a n y Syst em 2 H owever, beca u se of t h eir su ccess a t t h e Wor ld
t h in kin g in people posit ion ed t o do t h em h a r m . Tr a de Cen t er a n d t h e Pen t a gon , t h e t er r or ist s
Aldr ich Am ess Soviet h a n dler s pr efer r ed t h a t u n wit t in gly a n d im m edia t ely r en der ed h ija ck-
we n ot focu s Syst em 2 t h ou gh t on h ow h e in g a less effect ive t a ct ic. Th e pa ssen ger s on
bou gh t a J a gu a r on a GS-14 pa ych eckSys- F ligh t 93, qu ickly a r m ed wit h kn owledge of t h e
t em 1 fou n d a t a le a bou t h is wifes in h er it a n ce ot h er t h r ee fligh t s, wer e a ble t o en ga ge Sys-
cogn it ively ea sy t o a ccept . a t em 2 t o over com e Syst em 1s exist in g a va il-

a If you t h in k t h a t you cer t a in ly wou ld h a ve k n own Am es wa s a Soviet spy h a d you kn own of h is J a gu a r, t h en you a r e pr oba bly

gu ilt y of h in dsigh t bia s, or t h e t en den cy t o u n der est im a t e t h e ext en t t o wh ich you wer e su r pr ised by pa st even t s. On t h e ot h er h a n d,
you a r e n ot gu ilt y of h in dsigh t bia s if you t h in k t h is (befor e h a vin g r ea d a bou t Am es) a n d h a ve ever r epor t ed a collea gu e t o cou n -
t er in t elligen ce for own in g a J a gu a r.

2 Studies in Intelligence Vol. 56, No. 2 (Extracts, June 2012)


150
Thinking, Fast and Slow

a bilit y bia s a n d m a ke t h e decision t o ph ysica lly look-u p t a bles, pr ecise ca lcu la t ion s, a n d explicit
over power t h e t er r or ist s. a n a lyses of ou t com es obser ved on sim ila r occa -
sion s. Th is is t h e a ppr oa ch a n a n a lyst u ses t o
Ka h n em a n s in sigh t s per t a in t o t h e en t ir e pr edict t h e a m ou n t of explosive for ce n eeded t o
spect r u m of in t elligen ce oper a t ion s. We a ccept pen et r a t e a cer t a in t h ickn ess of con cr et e, or
in for m a t ion secu r it y pr a ct ices t h a t dem on st r a - ca lcu la t e h ow m u ch fu el a cer t a in t ype of a ir-
bly im pede pr odu ct ivit y in or der t o r edu ce t h e pla n e n eeds t o com plet e a cer t a in t ype of m is-
da n ger of wor se losses posed by cyber a t t a ck or sion .
pen et r a t ion . At t h e sa m e t im e, we wou ld
a lm ost cer t a in ly con sider t h e sa m e a m ou n t of Ot h er for eca st s a n d pr edict ion s in volve in t u -
lost pr odu ct ivit y a m a jor defea t if a h a cker h a d it ion a n d Syst em 1 t h in kin g. Ka h n em a n fu r-
in flict ed it on u s. Th is is wh a t Ka h n em a n ca lls t h er br ea ks down t h is va r iet y of pr edict ion in t o
t h e loss a ver sion bia s. Syst em 2 does n ot a sser t t wo su bva r iet ies. Th e fir st dr a ws on t h e skills
con t r ol over Syst em 1s cogn it ive ea se a t im a g- a n d exper t ise a cqu ir ed by r epea t ed exper ien ce,
in in g a disa st er beca u se in cr ea sed pr odu ct ivit y in wh ich a solu t ion t o t h e cu r r en t pr oblem
is m u ch m or e difficu lt for Syst em 2 t o im a gin e. com es qu ickly t o m in d beca u se Syst em 1 a ccu -
r a t ely r ecogn izes fa m ilia r cu es. Th e secon d
An y in t elligen ce officer m a kin g bu dget deci- su bva r iet y of in t u it ive pr edict ion , wh ich is
sion s sh ou ld r ea d Ka h n em a n s t h ou gh t s on t h e oft en in dist in gu ish a ble fr om t h e fir st , is ba sed
bia ses u n der lyin g t h e su n k-cost fa lla cy, or t h e on bia sed ju dgm en t s. Th is t ype of in t u it ive pr e-
decision t o in vest a ddit ion a l r esou r ces in los- dict ion , t ypica lly for wa r ded wit h con sider a ble
in g en dea vor s wh en bet t er in vest m en t s a r e con fiden ce, ver y oft en lea ds t o t r ou ble. Th e
a va ila ble. People fin d it difficu lt t o en ga ge Sys- expa n ded u se in in t elligen ce a n a lysis of st r u c-
t em 2 t o cu t t h eir losses in su ch sit u a t ion s, t u r ed a n a lyt ic t ech n iqu es a n d a ppr oa ch es
especia lly wh en Syst em 1 ca n ea sily con vin ce a dopt ed in t h e wa ke of t h e 9/11 a t t a cks a n d t h e
t h em of t h e loss of pr est ige t h a t wou ld su r ely Na t ion a l In t elligen ce E st im a t e on Ir a qi wea p-
follow. H ow oft en does t h e sa m e officer wh o on s of m a ss dest r u ct ion r epr esen t s in pa r t a n
st a r t ed a n expen sive m a jor pr oject a lso decide effor t t o elim in a t e t h is la t t er t ype of pr edict ion .
t o kill it ? You likely did n ot h a ve t o en ga ge Sys-
t em 2 t o a n swer t h e qu est ion . Th e t r ick is in u sin g st r u ct u r ed t ech n iqu es
a n d a ppr oa ch esor a pplied Syst em 2 t h in k-
Likewise, n on e of u s a r e im m u n e t o wh a t in gin a wa y t h a t elim in a t es bia sed in t u it ive
Ka h n em a n ca lls t h e pla n n in g fa lla cy, wh ich for eca st s a n d pr edict ion s wit h ou t a lso discou r-
descr ibes pla n s a n d for eca st s t h a t a r e u n r ea lis- a gin g, dela yin g, or even elim in a t in g t h e in t u i-
t ica lly close t o best -ca se scen a r ios a n d cou ld be t ive in sigh t s t h a t t r u e exper t ise pr ovides. Th is
im pr oved by con su lt in g st a t ist ics in sim ila r dilem m a pr oba bly expla in s in pa r t wh y som e
ca ses. Th is r eview, for exa m ple, t ook t wice a s exper t s in t h e CIAs Sen ior An a lyt ic Ser vice
lon g t o wr it e a s I t h ou gh t it wou ld, ju st like r em a in a m biva len t a bou t st r u ct u r ed a n a lyt ic
a lm ost ever y ot h er pa per I h a ve ever wr it t en . t ech n iqu es a n d a ppr oa ch es.

In t elligen ce a n a lyst s sh ou ld pay pa r t icu - Ka h n em a n , despit e h is st a t ed pr efer en ce for


la r ly close a t t en t ion t o Ka h n em a n s ch a pt er s st a t ist ics a n d a lgor it h m s, ca n n ot dism iss ou t of
on t h e n est ed pr oblem s of pr edict ion , in t u it ion , h a n d t h e va lu e of in t u it ive pr edict ion bor n e of
a n d exper t ise. a For eca st in g a n d pr edict ion a r e t r u e exper t ise. H is E xper t In t u it ion : Wh en
cor e m ission elem en t s for a n a lyst s. Ka h n em a n Ca n We Tr u st It ? ch a pt er cen t er s on wh a t h e
br ea ks t h em down in t o t wo m a in va r iet ies. Th e ca lls h is a dver sa r ia l colla bor a t ion wit h Ga r y
fir st , su ch a s t h ose en gin eer s m a ke, r ely on Klein , a lea din g pr opon en t of N a t u r a list ic Deci-

a Ma n y in t elligen ce a n a lyst s a r e fa m ilia r wit h som e of t h ese t h eor ies fr om Rich a r ds J. H eu er, J r.s Psych ology of In telligen ce A n alysis

(Wa sh in gt on , DC: Cen t er for t h e St u dy of In t elligen ce, 1999), wh ich is ba sed in pa r t on ea r lier ver sion s of Ka h n em a n s a n d Tversk ys
wor k. Th is pu blica t ion is a va ila ble on lin e a t h t t ps://www.cia .gov/libr a r y/cen t er-for-t h e-st u dy-of-in t elligen ce/csi-pu blica t ion s/book s-
a n d-m on ogr a ph s/psych ology-of-in t elligen ce-a n a lysis/in dex.h t m l.

Studies in Intelligence Vol. 56, No. 2 (Extracts, June 2012) 3


151
Thinking, Fast and Slow

sion Ma kin g, wh o r eject s Ka h n em a n s em ph a - ir r espect ive of t h e con fiden ce wit h wh ich t h ey


sis on bia ses a n d focu ses in st ea d on t h e va lu e a r e st a t ed. Mor eover, t h ey wou ld pr oba bly con -
of exper t in t u it ion a n d on h ow in t u it ive skills sider t h e feedba ck a va ila ble t o a n a lyst sfr om
develop. It is n ot difficu lt t o im a gin e t h a t t h eir policym a ker s a n d even t sin a dequ a t e for effi-
colla bor a t ion wa s m or e difficu lt t h a n Ka h n e- cien t lea r n in g a n d exper t ise developm en t . Ka h -
m a n gen er ou sly por t r a ys it t o h a ve been , wh ich n em a n wa s n ot r efer r in g specifica lly t o
m a kes t h e a r ea s on wh ich t h ey wer e a ble t o in t elligen ce a n a lyst s wh en h e wr ot e, it is
a gr ee even m or e n ot ewor t h y. wr on g t o bla m e a n yon e for fa ilin g t o for eca st
a ccu r a t ely in a n u n pr edict a ble wor ld, bu t h e
Th ey a gr eed t h a t t h e con fiden ce t h a t exper t s h a s given in t er views in wh ich h e discu sses
expr ess in t h eir in t u it ive ju dgm en t s is n ot a in t elligen ce a n a lyst s in t h is con t ext . At t h e
r elia ble gu ide t o t h eir va lidit y. Th ey fu r t h er sa m e t im e, h e a lso wr ot e, h owever, it seem s
a gr eed t h a t t wo ba sic con dit ion s m u st be pr es- fa ir t o bla m e pr ofession a ls for believin g t h ey
en t befor e in t u it ive ju dgm en t s r eflect t r u e ca n su cceed in a n im possible t a sk. In sh or t ,
exper t ise: a n en vir on m en t t h a t is su fficien t ly Ka h n em a n con cedes t h a t in t u it ion h a s t o be
r egu la r t o be pr edict a ble a n d a n oppor t u n it y t o va lu ed, bu t it ca n n ot n ecessa r ily be t r u st ed.
lea r n t h ese r egu la r it ies t h r ou gh pr olon ged
pr a ct ice. An exper t fir efigh t er s sen sin g t h e T h in k in g, Fast an d S low pr ovides in t elli-
n eed t o or der h is m en t o eva cu a t e a bu r n in g gen ce officer s wit h a n a ccessible voca bu la r y t o
bu ildin g ju st befor e it colla pses or a r a ce discu ss t h e pr ocesses of h u m a n cogn it ion t h e
dr iver s kn owin g t o slow down well befor e t h e in t er a ct ion s bet ween Syst em 1 a n d Syst em 2
m a ssive a cciden t com es in t o view a r e du e t o t h in kin gwh ich a r e a t t h e cen t er of t h eir
h igh ly va lid clu es t h a t ea ch exper t s Syst em 1 wor k. It does n ot , h owever, pr ovide solu t ion s or
h a s lea r n ed t o u se, even if Syst em 2 h a s n ot r elia ble a ppr oa ch es t o bia s m it iga t ion . Accor d-
lea r n ed t o n a m e t h em . in g t o Ka h n em a n , t h e best we ca n h ope t o do is
lea r n t o r ecogn ize sit u a t ion s in wh ich m is-
Lea r n in g, in t u r n , r elies on r eceivin g t im ely t a kes a r e likely, a n d t r y h a r der t o avoid spe-
a n d u n a m bigu ou s feedba ck . Ma n y if n ot m ost cific er r or s wh en t h e st a kes a r e h igh .
of t h e issu es wit h wh ich in t elligen ce a n a lyst s Ka h n em a n a lso spen ds ver y lit t le t im e discu ss-
a r e seized a r e wh a t Ka h n em a n a n d Klein in g h ow bia ses wor k in colla bor a t ive en vir on -
wou ld pr oba bly ca ll low-va lidit y en vir on - m en t s, despit e h is own ver y in sigh t fu l a ccou n t s
m en t s, in wh ich t h e in t u it ive pr edict ion s of of h is colla bor a t ion wit h Tver sky. We ca n h ope
exper t s sh ou ld n ot be t r u st ed a t fa ce va lu e, h e will explor e t h a t in h is n ext wor k.

4 Studies in Intelligence Vol. 56, No. 2 (Extracts, June 2012)


152

You might also like