You are on page 1of 14

Journal of Applied Psychology Copyright 1992 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

1992, Vol. 77, No. 1,65-78 0021-9010/92/S3.00

Antecedents and Outcomes of Work-Family Conflict:


Testing a Model of the Work-Family Interface
Michael R. Frone and Marcia Russell
Research Institute on Alcoholism
Buffalo, New York
M. Lynne Cooper
Center for the Study of Behavioral and Social Aspects of Health
State University of New York at Buffalo

A comprehensive model of the work-family interface was developed and tested. The proposed
model extended prior research by explicitly distinguishing between work interfering with family
and family interfering with work. This distinction allowed testing of hypotheses concerning the
unique antecedents and outcomes of both forms of work-family conflict and a reciprocal relation-
ship between them. The influence of gender, race, and job type on the generalizability of the model
was also examined. Data were obtained through household interviews with a random sample of 631
individuals. The model was tested with structural equation modeling techniques. Results were
strongly supportive. In addition, although the model was invariant across gender and race, there
were differences across blue- and white-collar workers. Implications for future research on the
work-family interface are discussed.

Work and family represent two of the most central realms of begun to examine work-family conflict (WFC) as another
adult life. Therefore, each offers a unique vantage point from source of stress that may influence well-being (e.g., Greenhaus
which to investigate important aspects of human behavior. Al- & Parasuraman, 1986; Voydanoff, 1987). We believe that this
though the study of work and family has spawned rich concep- last line of research is especially promising in terms of integrat-
tual and empirical literatures, these two domains of life tradi- ing research on work and family stress. More specifically, be-
tionally have been studied independently. More recently, how- cause WFC reflects the goodness of fit between work and fam-
ever, the interface between work and family roles has captured ily life, it may function as a critical intervening pathway
the interest of a growing number of work and family re- through which conditions at work affect the quality of family
searchers. This new focus has been fueled by several demo- life and vice versa.
graphic trends that are reshaping American society. For exam- To examine fully the relationship of WFC to well-being and
ple, there has been a dramatic increase in the number of its potential to integrate the study of work and family stress,
married women who have young children and who are joining WFC must be examined within the context of a multivariate
the work force and an increase in the prevalence of employed model that meets at least four major criteria. First, key work-
adults who are part of dual-earner families, single-parent fami- and family-related antecedents of both WFC and well-being
lies, and families facing the demands of elder care (e.g., Burke & need to be assessed (e.g., Lambert, 1990). Second, both domain-
Greenglass, 1987; Hall & Richter, 1988; Matthews & Rodin, specific and general measures of well-being should be exam-
1989; Zedeck & Mosier, 1990). ined (Kline & Cowan, 1988; Lambert, 1990). Third, the bidirec-
Interest in the work-family interface has produced a strong tional nature of WFC (i.e., work interfering with family versus
emphasis on integrating work and family research. Within the family interfering with work) must be addressed (Greenhaus,
area of stress research, new questions are being asked regarding 1988; Greenhaus & Beutell, 1985; Gutek, Searle, & Klepa,
such issues as the relative impact of work and family stressors 1991). Finally, the sample should be large, heterogeneous, and
on overall well-being, the impact of job stressors on family life, representative of employed adults. To date, however, research
and the impact of family stressors on work life (cf. Burke & testing multivariate models of WFC that meet even the first
Greenglass, 1987; Voydanoff, 1987). In addition, research has two criteria is rare (Burke, 1988). Thus, the goal of this study is
to develop and test a comprehensive model of the work-family
interface that meets each of these criteria. Although such a
This research was supported by the National Institute on Alcohol
Abuse and Alcoholism, Grant No. AA05702, awarded to Marcia Rus-
model is by necessity complex, it is consistent with Lambert's
sell. (1990) recent admonition that "only by looking at the work/
We wish to thank Ann Gerber, Michael Windle, and the two anony- family nexus in its entirety can we uncover the intricacies of the
mous reviewers for their thoughtful comments on an earlier version of relationship between work and home" (p. 250).
this article.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Mi- A Model of the Work-Family Interface
chael R. Frone, Research Institute on Alcoholism, 1021 Main Street, The conceptual model that guided the present research is
Buffalo, New York 14203. illustrated in Figure 1. We would like to acknowledge that this
65
66 M. FRONE, M. RUSSELL, AND M. COOPER

study does not represent the first attempt to formulate and test We concur with Greenhaus and Beutell (1985) that failing to
a model of the work-family interface. Rather, our model should examine both types of WFC may limit our understanding of
be viewed as a significant extension of several previous models the work-family interface to the extent that each is associated
(e.g., Bacharach, Bamberger, & Conley, 1991; Bedeian, Burke, & with different antecedents and consequences (as discussed be-
Moffett, 1988; Burke, 1988; Greenhaus & Beutell, 1985; Green- low). However, another limitation of failing to examine both
haus & Parasuraman, 1986; Kopelman, Greenhaus, & Con- types of WFC simultaneously is that the potential reciprocal
nolly, 1983; Rice, Frone, & McFarlin, in press). In outlining our relationship between conflicting work and family roles has
model, we begin by discussing the bidirectional nature of never been examined. As shown in Figure 1, we hypothesize a
WFC. Next we summarize hypotheses concerning the predic- positive reciprocal relationship between the two types of WFC.
tors of WFC. We then summarize hypotheses regarding the The rationale for this relationship is based on the assumption
predictors of domain-specific distress (i.e., job distress and fam- that if one's work-related problems and responsibilities begin to
ily distress) and overall psychological distress (i.e., depression). interfere with the accomplishment of one's family-related obli-
Finally, we discuss the importance of examining the influence gations, these unfulfilled family obligations may begin to inter-
of key sociodemographic characteristics on the generalizability fere with one's day-to-day functioning at work. Conversely, if
of our model. one's family-related problems and responsibilities begin to in-
terfere with the accomplishment of one's work-related obliga-
Bidirectional Nature of Work-Family Conflict tions, these unfulfilled work obligations may begin to interfere
with one's day-to-day functioning at home. This hypothesis is
Definitions of WFC explicitly portray a bidirectional concep- consistent with Schaubroeck's (1990) recent suggestion that the
tualization (e.g., Greenhaus & Beutell, 1985). That is, they dis- conflict relationship between work and family may be exam-
tinguish between work interfering with family (W -+ F conflict) ined appropriately within the context of a reciprocal relation-
and family interfering with work (F -* W conflict). Despite this ship.
conceptual distinction, previous research has relied almost ex-
clusively on measures that assess only W - F conflict (Green-
haus & Beutell, 1985). Furthermore, there is evidence that Predictors ofWork-Family Conflict
mixed directional measures of WFC (e.g., "How much do your The direct predictors of WFC portrayed in our model (Fig-
job and family life interfere with one another?"; Pleck, Staines, ure 1) are job stressors, family stressors, job involvement, and
& Lang, 1980) primarily assess W -* F conflict (Frone, Russell, family involvement. We include these four predictors because
& Cooper, 1991; Gutek et al., 1991). For example, Frone et al. each has been identified as an important antecedent of WFC
(1991) found that the experience of W -* F conflict was re- (Greenhaus & Beutell, 1985). Moreover, each predictor was ex-
ported almost three times more frequently than the experience pected to be directly related to only one type of WFC.
of F - W conflict among men and women. With regard to job and family stressors, Greenhaus and Beu-

W ->F
Conflict

\ \

\\
+ f I

Conflict

Figure 1. Conceptual model of the work-family interface. (Pluses and minuses represent the direction of
hypothesized relationships. Broken lines signify two nested models, one without [Model A] and one with
[Model B ] paths from work-family conflict to depression. The letter d represents the disturbance term for
each endogenous variable. To simplify presentation of the model, the correlations among the exogenous
variables and the measurement model are not shown.)
WORK-FAMILY CONFLICT 67

tell (1985) hypothesized that exposure to stressors in a given be offered for the hypothesized positive relationship between
domain (e.g., work) may lead to irritability, fatigue, or preoccu- W - F conflict and family distress. Although previous re-
pation with those problems, thereby limiting one's ability to search has found that W - F conflict is associated with low
meet the demands of other domains of life (e.g., family). Ex- levels of family satisfaction (e.g., Bedeian et al., 1988; Beutell,
panding this general argument to include the two types of 1983; Beutell & O'Hare, 1987; Coverman, 1989; Holahan &
WFC, we hypothesize that job stressors are directly and posi- Gilbert, 1979; Parasuraman et al., 1989; Rice et al., in press), no
tively related to W -* F conflict, whereas family stressors are research has examined the relationship between F -* W con-
directly and positively related to F - W conflict. Previous re- flict and job-related affect.
search strongly supports a positive relationship between per- With regard to job and family stressors, we hypothesize that
ceived work stressors and W -* F conflict (e.g., Bacharach et al, they are directly and positively related to their respective
1991; Bedeian et al., 1988; Burke, 1988; Greenhaus & Beutell, within-domain measure of distress. Previous research lends
1985; Kopelman et al., 1983; Parasuraman, Greenhaus, strong support for an association between job and family stress-
Granrose, Rabinowitz, & Beutell, 1989; VoydanofF, 1988). No ors and decreased job and family satisfaction, respectively (e.g.,
research, however, has examined the relationship between fam- Bedeian et al., 1988; Kandel, Davies, &Raveis, 1985; Kopelman
ily stressors and F -* W conflict. et al., 1983; Parasuraman et al., 1989).
The second major antecedent of WFC is the psychological Finally, with regard to job and family involvement, we hy-
importance of work and family roles. Greenhaus and Beutell pothesize that they are directly and negatively related to their
(1985) argued that high levels of psychological involvement may respective within-domain measure of distress. This hypothesis
lead to increased WFC in two ways. First, high levels of psycho- is based on the conceptual and empirical work of several re-
logical involvement in one role may be associated with an in- searchers who have examined the relationship between job in-
crease in the amount of time and effort devoted to that role, volvement and job satisfaction (e.g., Rabinowitz & Hall, 1977;
thereby making it more difficult to comply with pressures asso- Sekaran, 1989; Weiner, Muczyk, & Gable, 1987; Weiner &
ciated with another role. Second, high levels of psychological Vardi, 1980). For example, Sekaran (1989) postulated that
involvement in a given role may cause one to be mentally preoc- highly job-involved workers invest greater amounts of time and
cupied with that role even when physically attempting to fulfill effort in their work. As a result, they are more likely than low
the demands of a second role. Expanding these arguments to job-involved workers to develop a greater sense of work-related
include the two types of WFC, we hypothesize that job involve- mastery, which in turn promotes higher levels of job satisfac-
ment is directly and positively related to W -* F conflict, tion. Sekaran's (1989) study supports a positive relationship be-
whereas family involvement is directly and positively related to tween job involvement and job satisfaction by way of increased
F -* W conflict. Previous research supports a positive relation- perceptions of work-related mastery. Although a similar pro-
ship between job involvement and W -* F conflict (e.g., Beutell, cess may lead to a relationship between family involvement and
1983; Beutell & O'Hare, 1987; Frone & Rice, 1987; Greenhaus & family distress, no research has examined the relationship be-
Kopelman, 1981; Greenhaus, Parasuraman, Granrose, Rabin- tween these two variables.
owitz, & Beutell, 1989; Wiley, 1987). Although the relationship
between family involvement and F - W conflict has not been
Predictors of Overall Psychological Distress
directly examined, there is indirect evidence to support such a
relationship. Gutek et al. (1991) found that the number of hours As shown in our model, we hypothesize that both job- and
devoted to family activities, which is presumably positively re- family-related psychological distress are directly and positively
lated to family involvement, was positively related to F -* W related to depression (used here as an indicator of overall psy-
conflict. chological distress). This hypothesis was derived from additive
models of overall quality of life (e.g., Andrews & Withey, 1976;
Predictors of Domain-Specific Psychological Distress Campbell, Converse, & Rodgers, 1976; Michalos, 1985; Rice,
McFarlin, Hunt & Near, 1985). Additive models propose that
As indicated in Figure 1, the direct predictors of both job the perceived quality of life associated with each constituent
distress and family distress are WFC, domain-specific stress- domain of life combine additively to determine overall quality
ors, and domain-specific psychological involvement. With re- of life (Rice et al., in press). This proposition has been sup-
gard to WFC, we hypothesize that W -* F conflict is directly ported using both measures of satisfaction and psychological
and positively related to family distress, whereas F -> W con- distress (e.g., Bedeian et al., 1988; Coverman, 1989; Kandel et
flict is directly and positively related to job distress. The ratio- al., 1985; Kopelman et al., 1983; Rice et al., in press).
nale for this hypothesis is that high levels of psychological dis- A strict interpretation of additive models implies that the
tress associated with a given role may be experienced if one is effect of any other variable on overall quality of life must be
frequently struggling to meet the demands of that role because indirect, that is, mediated by the quality of life in one or more
of interference from another role. For example, relative to indi- specific domains of life (Rice et al., in press). Consistent with
viduals whose family life does not interfere with their jobs, this proposition, our model indicates that the relationships of
individuals who experience high levels of F -* W conflict may stressors, psychological involvement, and WFC to depression
report elevated levels of job-related distress because they are are mediated through their direct or indirect relationships to
more likely to feel overwhelmed by the ensuing struggle to meet either job or family distress. With regard to work and family
their responsibilities at work and therefore experience a reduc- stressors, previous research lends strong support to this hy-
tion in the quality of their work life. A parallel explanation may pothesis (Bedeian et al., 1988; Coverman, 1989; Kandel et al.,
68 M. FRONE, M. RUSSELL, AND M. COOPER

1985; Kopelman et al., 1983). In contrast, results concerning the Procedures


relationship between W -* F conflict and overall life satisfac-
tion are inconsistent. Previous research has found indirect ef- Data for this study were collected by 20 professionally trained inter-
viewers during the spring and summer of 1989 as part of a larger study
fects only, via job or family satisfaction (e.g., Coverman, 1989;
of stress processes. Field work for this study (i.e., interviewer recruit-
Rice et al., in press); direct effects only (Kopelman et al., 1983); ment and training; supervision of interviewers; validation of inter-
and both indirect (via job or family satisfaction) and direct ef- viewers' work; and data preparation, entry, and cleaning) was con-
fects (Bedeian et al., 1988). Because these mixed results do not ducted by WESTAT, Inc., a nationally recognized survey research
lend strong support for a fully mediated relationship between firm. Interviews were conducted in respondents' homes with a highly
WFC and overall psychological distress, we will examine (via structured interview schedule that contained both interviewer- and
nested models) whether a model containing the two direct self-administered sections. The complete interview required approxi-
paths between WFC and depression fits the data better than a mately 90 min to administer. Respondents were compensated $25 for
model that does not contain these two paths (see broken lines in their time.
Figure 1).
Measures
Generalizability of the Model Each of the measures is described in detail below. All measures were
administered by the interviewer, except those assessing psychological
Previous conceptual discussions of work-family stress pro- involvement and depression, which were self-administered. Descrip-
cesses propose that gender may represent an important modera- tive statistics (i.e., means, standard deviations, and internal consis-
tor variable (e.g., Eckenrode & Gore, 1990; Kline & Cowan, tency reliability estimates) and zero-order correlations for the major
1988; Lambert, 1990). However, potential gender differences in study variables are presented in Table 1. Except where otherwise noted,
work-family stress processes are not always examined. Further- all variables were created by averaging their respective items and were
more, most research testing models of WFC has used relatively scored so that a high score represents higher levels of the construct.
circumscribed samples of White individuals in white-collar
jobs. Thus, we know little about the generalizability of previous Sociodemographic Characteristics
research to minority and blue-collar workers (Burke & Green-
glass, 1987). Given that the sociodemographic composition of Gender. This variable was coded as 0 for men and 1 for women.
the American work force is becoming more diverse (e.g., Jack- Race. Respondents classified themselves into one of the following
seven racial or ethnic groups: Aleut, Eskimo, or American Indian;
son & Schuler, 1990; OfTerman & Gowing, 1990), the lack of
Asian/Pacific Islander; Black Hispanic; Black, not Hispanic; White
knowledge concerning the generalizability of work-family Hispanic; White, not Hispanic; and other. Respondents classifying
stress processes represents an important limitation. To address themselves as White, not Hispanic were coded as White (0), whereas
this issue, we examine the fit of our model across gender, race, all others were coded non-White (1). Although it is not desirable to
and job type. group all racial minorities together, we combined them into a single
group because the non-White sample in this study was composed al-
most entirely of Blacks (96%).
Method Age. This variable was based on self-reported date of birth and was
coded in years.
Sample Education. This variable was based on self-reported years of formal
education.
Respondents in this study were drawn from the longitudinal follow- Job type. This variable was based on responses to the following
up of a random sample survey of 1,933 adults residing in Erie County, open-ended question: What is your occupation or job title and what
New York. Designated respondents were identified in a three-stage sort of work do you do on your job? Professional coders classified
probability sample, designed to yield approximately equal representa- responses into one of the 13 major occupational categories used by the
tion of two racial groups (Blacks and non-Blacks) and three education U.S. Census Bureau (U.S. Department of Commerce, 1982). A respon-
levels (less than high school graduate, high school graduate, and at dent was coded as white-collar (1) if his or her job fell into categories 1
least some college). The overall completion rate for Wave 1 was 78.3%. through 5, whereas a respondent was coded as blue-collar (0) if his or
Of the 1,933 Wave 1 respondents, 1,616 were reinterviewed at Wave 2, her job fell into categories 6 through 13. Although this categorization is
representing 83.6% of the total original sample. somewhat crude, our measure of job type was, as expected, positively
Of the 1,616 respondents interviewed at Wave 2,631 met the follow- correlated with years of formal education (r = .43, p < .001).
ing criteria for selection into this study: (a) employed at least 20 hr per Job tenure. This variable was based on self-reported tenure in the
week, (b) currently married or living as married, or had children living person's current job and was coded in years.
at home, or both, and (c) provided valid data on all measures described Marital status. Respondents were coded either as not married (i.e.,
below. We used data only from the second wave of data collection never married, widowed, separated, or divorced [0]) or married or
because several of the major variables were not assessed in the first living as married (1).
wave. Number of children. Number of children currently living at home
On average, respondents were 40.7 years old (573 = 10.4) and had was coded into five categories ranging from 0 to 4 or more.
completed 13.3 years of formal education (SD = 2.3). Fifty-six percent Age of youngest child. Following a coding scheme outlined by
were women and 42% were White. Seventy-three percent were married Bedeian et al. (1988), we coded age of youngest child living at home
or living as married, and 78% had at least one child living at home. into one of the following five categories: no children (1), youngest child
Respondents had worked in their current jobs for an average of 8.7 over 18 years of age (2), youngest child 13-18 years of age (3), youngest
years (SD = 8.5); approximately equal numbers held blue-collar (49%) child 6-12 years of age (4), and youngest child less than 6 years of
and white-collar (51%) jobs. age (5).
WORK-FAMILY CONFLICT 69

Table 1
Means, Standard Deviations, Reliability Estimates, and Intercorrelations for Major Study Variables

Variable 1 M SD

1. Job involvement (.88) 3.92 1.18


2. Family involvement .14* (.88) 5.14 0.79
3. Job stressor index .02 -.06 (.72) 2.05 0.36
4. Family stressor index -.02 -.22* .21* (.66) 1.83 0.49
5. W -* F Conflict .05 -.06 .31* .23* (.76) 2.21 1.07
6. F -* W Conflict .09* .01 .19* .29* .33* (.56) 1.39 0.61
7. Job distress -.21* -.13* .53* .25* .28* .24* (-84) 1.80 0.60
8. Family distress -.03 -.31* .21* .60* .21* .25* .43* (.88) 1.57 0.48
9. Depression -.05 -.09* .28* .38* .26* .42* .37* .48* (.87) 1.58 0.39
Note. Numbers on the diagonal are internal consistency reliability estimates (coefficient alpha).
* p ^ .05.

Psychological Involvement child(ren)'s misbehavior. The marital stressor items were developed by
Kessler (1985) and assessed the following two dimensions: lack of
Job involvement. Job involvement represents the degree to which spouse support and the degree of tension or conflict in the relation-
one's job is central to one's self-concept or sense of identity (Kanungo, ship. Each family stressor item used either a 4-point or a 5-point fre-
1982; Lodahl & Kejner, 1965). This construct was assessed with five quency-based response scale. These eight items were combined to
items adapted from a measure developed by Kanungo (1982). A study create an overall family stressor score (see the description of family
by Blau (1985) suggested that Kanungo's scale is unidimensional and a involvement for the method of combining these items).
purer measure of psychological identification than Lodahl and
Kejner's (1965) short-form measure. Each job involvement item used a
6-point agree/disagree response scale. Work-Family Conflict
Family involvement. Family involvement was assessed by modify- Four items were developed to assess WFC: two items each assessed
ing the job involvement items so that they referred to either the respon- the degree to which a respondent's job interferes with his or her home-
dent's spouse or children). This procedure has been used successfully life (W - F conflict) and the degree to which a respondent's homelife
in previous WFC research (e.g., Frone & Rice, 1987). Specifically, five interferes with his or her job (F - W conflict). The items assessing
items assessed spouse involvement and five items assessed parental W - F conflict were: "How often does your job or career interfere
involvement. Each family involvement item used a 6-point agree/disa- with your responsibilities at home, such as yard work, cooking, clean-
gree response scale. An overall family involvement score was calcu- ing, repairs, shopping, paying the bills, or child care?" and "How often
lated as the average of the five spouse involvement items among re- does your job or career keep you from spending the amount of time
spondents who were married only, or as the average of the five parental you would like to spend with your family?" The items assessing F - W
involvement items among respondents who were single parents, or as conflict were: "How often does your homelife interfere with your re-
an average of the 10 spouse and parental involvement items among
sponsibilities at work, such as getting to work on time, accomplishing
respondents who were both married and parents.
daily tasks, or working overtime?" and "How often does your homelife
keep you from spending the amount of time you would like to spend
Work-Family Stressors on job or career-related activities?" Each item used a 5-point frequency-
based response scale.
Job stressors. The job stressor scale was based on 20 items taken
from several previously published measures (Beehr, 1976; House, Psychological Distress
McMichael, Wells, Kaplan, & Landerman, 1979; Insel & Moos, 1974;
Pearlin & Schooler, 1978; Rizzo, House, & Lirtzman, 1970; Sims, Szila- Job distress. Job distress was assessed with six items developed by
gyi, & Keller, 1976). The 20 items assessed three dimensions of work Kandel et al. (1985). This scale assessed the strength of negative emo-
stressors: work pressure, lack of autonomy, and role ambiguity. Work tional reactions to daily work experiences. Using a 4-point scale, re-
pressure (8 items) assessed the frequency with which individuals per- spondents were asked to indicate the extent to which they feel each of
ceive high job-related demands resulting from heavy workloads and six emotional reactions (e.g., bothered or upset, frustrated) when they
responsibilities. Lack of autonomy (6 items) assessed the frequency think of their day-to-day experiences on the job.
with which individuals perceive constraints on their ability to function Family distress. Family distress was assessed with 12 items devel-
autonomously and influence important job parameters. Role ambigu- oped by Kandel et al. (1985). This scale assessed the strength of nega-
ity (6 items) assessed the frequency of being confused or unclear about tive emotional reactions to daily experiences as a spouse (six items) or
day-to-day tasks and expectations and job-related goals. Each job parent (six items). Using a 4-point scale, respondents were asked to
stressor item used a 4-point frequency-based response scale. Each of indicate the extent to which they feel each of six emotional reactions
these three job stressor dimensions has been linked to W - F conflict (e.g., bothered or upset, frustrated) when they think of their day-to-day
and a variety of stress-related outcomes (see Voydanoff, 1987, for a experiences as a marital partner or parent. These 12 items were com-
review). bined to create an overall family distress score (see the description of
Family stressors. The family stressor scale was composed of four family involvement for the method of combining these items).
parental stressor items and four marital stressor items. Of the parental Depression. Depression was assessed with the 20-item Center for
stressor items, two were developed by Kessler (1985) and two were Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff, 1977). A
developed specifically for this study. Parental stressor items assessed 4-point scale was used to determine how frequently each of 20 symp-
the following two dimensions: parental workload and the extent of toms were experienced during the past month. The items in this scale
70 M. FRONE, M. RUSSELL, AND M. COOPER

assess several dimensions of depressive symptomatology, such as de- treated as a single indicator of each construct (see discussion of mea-
pressed mood, feelings of guilt and worthlessness, feelings of helpless- surement error below).
ness and hopelessness, psychomotor retardation, loss of appetite, and To determine whether a SEM is identified, one must first distin-
sleep disturbance. guish between nonrecursive and recursive models. A SEM is nonrecur-
sive if any of the following conditions are met: (a) the disturbances for
any two endogenous variables are allowed to correlate, (b) a reciprocal
Dimensionality of the Measures
relationship exists between any two endogenous variables, or (c) a feed-
To assess the appropriateness of computing unidimensional scores back loop connects any two endogenous variables (e.g., Berry, 1984;
for each of the major constructs described above, we submitted each Bollen, 1989). Conversely, a model that meets none of these conditions
scale to a principal components analysis (results can be obtained from is said to be recursive (e.g., Berry, 1984; Bollen, 1989). As can be seen in
Michael R. Frone). Specifically, principal components analyses for the Figure 1, our model is nonrecursive because it meets conditions (a) and
job involvement, job stressor, WFC, job distress, and depression items (b). Specifically, we hypothesize that W - F conflict and F - W
were conducted using the full sample. In contrast, principal compo- conflict are reciprocally related. Furthermore, as noted by Schau-
nents analyses for the family involvement, family stressor, and family broeck (1990), the disturbances for variables involved in a reciprocal
distress items were limited to married parents because they were the relationship can be expected to covary because they may share com-
only respondents to have scores on each of the marital and parental mon causes that are not explicitly modeled. Failure to estimate the
items constituting these scales. With the exception of the WFC and job covariation among the disturbances may lead to biased parameter es-
stressor items, an examination of the number of eigenvalues greater timates for the reciprocal relationship (Schaubroeck, 1990). Finally,
than or equal to one and scree plots for each of the remaining principal because previous research has found a strong positive correlation be-
components analyses supported our decision to create unidimensional tween job and family satisfaction (e.g., Bedeian et al., 1988; Coverman,
scales. 1989; Kopelman et al., 1983; Rice et al., in press), we estimated a param-
With regard to the four WFC conflict items, the principal compo- eter corresponding to the noncausal relationship between job and fam-
nents analysis revealed two factors with eigenvalues greater than or ily distress by permitting their disturbances to covary.
equal to one, as expected. The two W -* F conflict items loaded highly Assuming one is estimating a SEM using either single indicators of
on the first factor (oblique rotated loadings = .87 and .92), whereas the each construct or latent variables in which the measurement model is
two F - W conflict items loaded highly on the second factor (oblique identified, all recursive SEMs are either just-identified or overidenti-
rotated loadings = .82 and .85). In addition, the cross-factor loadings fied, thereby yielding unique parameter estimates. In contrast, nonre-
were small (ranging from -.05 to .06). These results support the con- cursive SEMs may be underidentified (Berry, 1984; Bollen, 1989). Un-
ceptual distinction between W - F and F - W conflict. deridentified SEMs contain at least one parameter for which unique
The principal components analysis of the 20 job stressor items re- estimates cannot be obtained. Thus, testing such a model limits infer-
vealed four factors with eigenvalues greater than or equal to one. How- ences concerning hypothesized structural relationships among vari-
ever, the scree plot suggested retaining two factors. A two-factor solu- ables. As noted earlier, our model is nonrecursive because it contains a
tion revealed that the ambiguity and lack of autonomy items loaded reciprocal relationship and two correlated disturbances; therefore, it
highly on the first factor (oblique rotated loadings = .33 to .72), may be underidentified.
whereas the work pressure items loaded highly on the second factor The identification of reciprocal relationships requires that instru-
(oblique rotated loadings = .52 to .70). With two exceptions, the re- ment variables be incorporated into one's model. To function as an
maining 18 cross-factor loadings were all below .30. Because these instrument, an antecedent variable must be theoretically related to
results indicated that it may not be appropriate to treat the 20 job only one of the variables that make up a reciprocal relationship (e.g.,
stressor items as unidimensional, we conducted two preliminary tests Berry, 1984; Schaubroeck, 1990). A simple rule of thumb is that a recip-
of our model, once using two separate job stressor scales and once rocal relationship is overidentified if each reciprocally related variable
using an overall job stressor scale. Consistent with our model, the has at least two associated instrument variables (e.g., Schaubroeck,
analysis using two job stressor scales revealed that each was positively 1990). Thus, it is clear from Figure 1 that the reciprocal relationship
related to both W -* F conflict and job distress. Moreover, the parame- between W - F conflict and F - W conflict is overidentified.
ter estimates for the remaining paths were virtually identical to those The instrument variable rule, however, does not address the identi-
from the analysis using the overall job stressor scale. Therefore, to fication of the remaining parameters. Therefore, an overall test of
simplify presentation of our results and to maintain consistency with model identification is necessary. One approach to determining
previous WFC research (e.g., Bedeian et al., 1988; Burke, 1988; Parasur- whether a nonrecursive SEM is identified is the use of necessary and
aman et al., 1989), we present our findings using the overall job sufficient rules of thumb or tests that exist for specific types of models
stressor score. (Berry, 1984; Bollen, 1989). A necessary and sufficient test for the
identification of nonrecursive models is the rank test (see Berry, 1984,
and Bollen, 1989, for a detailed discussion of this and other available
Path Analyses tests for identification, including the solution of reduced-form equa-
We tested our model using Bentler's (1989) EQS structural equa- tions). However, as a general check on model identification, this test is
tions program. Input for the EQS program consisted of a 9 X 9 covari- limited because it assumes that all disturbances are allowed to covary.
ance matrix and estimated reliabilities for each variable. We discuss Thus, if a SEM imposes some constraints on the covariation among
below several issues concerning the estimation and evaluation of struc- disturbances (i.e., constraining some to equal zero) and fails the rank
tural equation models. test, it is still possible that the model is identified. In contrast, if such a
Identification. A structural equation model (SEM) must be identi- model passes the rank test, it is certainly identified, indicating that all
fied before it can be estimated. Identification addresses the issue of structural relationships and relevant correlated disturbances can be
whether enough information exists to yield unique parameter esti- freely estimated. Submitting our model to the rank test revealed that it
mates (e.g., see Berry, 1984, Bollen, 1989, andSchaubroeck, 1990, fora is overidentified; therefore, not only can all parameters be uniquely
detailed discussion of this complex issue). Although identification is estimated, but goodness-of-fit tests also can be performed.
relevant for both the measurement model and the SEM, we address the Measurement error. A major advantage of structural equation pro-
identification of the SEM only because each multi-item scale was grams is their ability to estimate the parameters in a path model while
WORK-FAMILY CONFLICT 71

correcting for the biasing effects of random measurement error. The els. The first between-group model did not contain any cross-group
usual approach is to estimate structural relationships among latent invariance constraints. In other words, all of the parameter estimates
variables that are free of random measurement error. In the present were freely estimated within gender groups. The second between-
analyses, however, the multi-item scales were treated as single indica- group model, however, constrained each of the 16 hypothesized rela-
tors of each construct. We decided to use a single-indicator approach tionships to be invariant across gender. If the chi-square for the con-
rather than a latent variable approach because the latter would have strained model is significantly larger than the chi-square for the un-
required that each respondent answer each of the marital and parental constrained model, the assumption of invariance is not tenable.
items that went into the construction of the overall family involve- Finally, if the overall chi-square difference test revealed a lack of invar-
ment, family stressor, and family distress measures. Such an approach iance, we examined the univariate modification indices to locate spe-
would have eliminated from the analyses 311 individuals (49.3% of the cific parameters that significantly differed across gender (Bentler,
sample) who were either married without children living at home (n = 1989; Bollen, 1989).
138) or single parents (n = 173). Given that there is no theoretical rea-
son for restricting our analyses to married parents, we felt that the
arbitrary elimination of single parents and married respondents with- Results
out children would have compromised the generalizability of our re-
sults. Preliminary Analyses
Given our decision to treat each multi-item scale as a single indicator
of its corresponding construct, we corrected for random measurement We examined the potential confounding influence of socio-
error by setting the random error variance associated with each con- demographic characteristics by estimating the two nested
struct equal to the product of its variance and the quantity one minus SEMs (see description below), both controlling and not control-
its estimated reliability (Bollen, 1989). This approach has been used in ling for the following variables: gender, race, age, education, job
several recent studies (e.g., Bacharach et al., 1991; Farkas & Tetrick, type, job tenure, marital status, number of children, and age of
1989; Schaubroeck, Cotton, & Jennings, 1989; Wayne & Ferris, 1990; youngest child. The analyses revealed that the sociodemogra-
Williams & Hazer, 1986). Moreover, the utility of this approach was phic covariates had virtually no impact on the magnitude or
supported in a recent study (Netemeyer, Johnston, & Burton, 1990) significance of the parameter estimates. Therefore, to facilitate
that compared the parameter estimates for a SEM using a latent vari- model estimation, especially the estimation of simultaneous
able analysis, a single-indicator analysis corrected for measurement
error (as discussed earlier), and a single-indicator analysis that did not between-group models, the covariates were dropped from the
correct for measurement error. Netemeyer et al.'s (1990) study revealed analyses. Although gender, race, and job type did not act as
that the latent variable analysis and the corrected single-indicator anal- confounding variables, they may still moderate the relation-
ysis yielded virtually identical parameter estimates in terms of direc- ships outlined in Figure 1. This issue is examined in more detail
tion, magnitude, and significance. However, results from both of these below.
procedures diverged substantially from the uncorrected single-indica-
tor analysis.
Model evaluation. To evaluate the overall fit of a model, EQS pro- Model Evaluation for the Full Sample
vides a chi-square goodness-of-fit statistic. The chi-square statistic is
based on a comparison of the predicted and observed covariance ma- Overall goodness-of-fit. Table 2 summarizes the goodness-
trices. A nonsignificant chi-square value indicates good fit. However, of-fit indices for three models (one baseline model and two
because trivial differences between the predicted and observed covari- substantive models). The baseline model we estimated was the
ance matrices may lead to a significant chi-square when large samples standard null model in which covariation among the variables
are used, we also used the following goodness-of-fit indices that are was constrained to equal zero. As can be seen in Table 2, the
less dependent on sample size: normed fit index (NFI), nonnormed fit large and highly significant chi-square value for the null model
index (NNFI), and comparative fit index (CFI; e.g., see Bentler, 1989, reveals a poor fit to the data, indicating that there is significant
and Bollen, 1989, for computational details). Briefly, the NFI, CFI, covariation among the model variables.
and NNFI compare the fit of a substantive model to the fit of some
On the basis of our earlier discussion, we estimated two
predetermined baseline model, usually a null model in which covaria-
tion among variables is constrained to equal zero. The value for each of nested substantive modelsone that did not contain the two
these indices varies between zero and one, with values greater than or direct paths between the WFC conflict measures and depres-
equal to .90 used to indicate a good fit. To test whether adding the two sion (Model A) and one that did (Model B). Table 2 shows that
paths connecting the WFC variables to depression improved the over- Model A had a substantially and significantly smaller chi-
all fit of the model, we used a chi-square difference test (Bentler, 1989) square than the null model. Thus, Model A fit the data better
and compared the NFI, CFI, and NNFI for the two nested models. than the null model. The values of the various goodness-of-fit
Finally, we examined the univariate modification indices (i.e., La indices, however, yielded inconsistent conclusions regarding
Grange Multiplier tests) to see if overall model fit could be improved the absolute fit of Model A. The relatively large and highly
by freeing any of the 14 substantive paths that were constrained to significant chi-square value, coupled with a NNFI below .90,
equal zero (e.g., Bentler, 1989; Bollen, 1989).
indicated that substantial covariation may be unexplained,
Multiple group comparisons. To examine whether the results ob-
whereas the NFI and CFI suggested that Model A fit the data
tained from the full sample were invariant across gender, race, and job
type, we conducted both within- and between-group analyses. For ex- reasonably well.
ample, to examine invariance across gender, we first computed the Turning to Model B, it can be seen that freeing the two paths
four fit indices described above separately for men and women to see if between WFC and depression led to a substantial and statisti-
there was a good fit in each group. Second, to examine whether the cally significant reduction in chi-square compared with Model
magnitude or direction of each hypothesized relationship was invari- A. The relatively small and nonsignificant chi-square asso-
ant across gender, we specified two simultaneous between-group mod- ciated with Model B indicated that it fit the data well. More-
72 M. FRONE, M. RUSSELL, AND M. COOPER

Table 2
Goodness-of-Fit Summary
Model df X2 P Mf Ax2 P NFI NNFI CFI
Null 36 1,255.04 <.001
A 14 80.40 <.001 22 1,174.64 <.001 .936 .860 .946
B 12 15.13 .234 2 65.27 <.001 .988 .992 .997
Note. NFI = normed fit index; NNFI = nonnormed fit index; and CFI = comparative fit index.

over, the NFI, NNFI, and CFI for Model B were substantially reciprocal relationship between W -* F and F -* W conflict.
larger than those for Model A, and their absolute values were all Moreover, our hypotheses regarding the unique direct predic-
well above the .90 cutoff typically used to indicate a good fit tors of the two types of WFC were largely supported. Job stress-
between model and data. Finally, an examination of the univar- ore were positively related to W -* F conflict, whereas family
iate modification indices revealed that the overall fit of Model stressors and family involvement were positively related to F -*
B could not be substantially improved by freeing any of the W conflict. However, contrary to expectation, job involvement
remaining 14 substantive paths that were constrained to equal was not significantly related to W - F conflict.
zero. Domain-specific psychological distress. Three sets of predic-
Parameter estimates. Parameter estimates for Model B are tions were made regarding the antecedents of domain-specific
shown in Figure 2. Solid lines represent significant paths and affect. First, the hypothesized positive relationship between
broken lines represent nonsignificant paths. To facilitate pre- F -* W conflict and job distress was significant. In contrast,
sentation of the parameter estimates, we discuss the prediction W -* F conflict and family distress were not significantly re-
of WFC, domain-specific psychological distress, and overall lated. Second, job and family stressors were directly and posi-
psychological distress separately. tively related to their respective within-domain measure of dis-
Work-family conflict. As hypothesized, there was a positive tress, as hypothesized. Finally, as expected, there was a direct

.72

Intel-correlations Among Exogenous Variables


Exogenous Variables 1 2 3 4
1. Job Stressors
2. Job Involvement .03
3. Family Involvement -.09 .16'
4. Family Stressors .33* -.02 -.30'

Figure 2. Summary of standardized path coefficients for Model B that are based on the full sample.
(Solid lines represent significant paths, p < .05, and broken lines represent nonsignificant paths. The letter
d represents the disturbance term for each endogenous variable. To simplify presentation of results, the
measurement model is not shown.)
WORK-FAMILY CONFLICT 73

negative relationship between both job and family involvement fit well for both Whites and non-Whites. Moreover, the nonsig-
and their respective within-domain measure of distress. nificant between-group chi-square difference test and the rela-
Overall psychological distress. As anticipated, there was a tive values of the NFI, NNFI, and CFI for the constrained and
significant positive relationship between both job and family unconstrained between-group models indicated that the indi-
distress and depression. Furthermore, as noted earlier, freeing vidual parameter estimates were invariant across both racial
the two paths linking WFC and depression led to a significant groups. Thus, the model appears to generalize across racial
improvement in the fit of our model. The parameter estimates, groups.
however, indicated that only F -* W conflict was directly and Job type. The within-group fit indices revealed that the
positively related to depression. model fit well for both white-collar and blue-collar workers.
However, the significant between-group chi-square difference
Multiple Group Comparisons test and the relatively smaller NFI, NNFI, and CFI values from
the constrained between-group model compared with the un-
To examine whether the findings based on the full sample constrained between group-model suggested that one or more
were invariant across gender, race, and job type, a series of individual parameter estimates varied across the two groups.
within- and between-group models was specified. The results Examination of the univariate modification indices for each of
of these analyses are provided in Table 3. the 16 between-group constraints revealed two differences.
Gender. An examination of the within-group fit indices (Ta- First, the relationship between job involvement and W -* F
ble 3, lines 1 and 2) revealed that the model fit well for both men conflict was significantly different across blue- and white-col-
and women. The chi-square values for the unconstrained and lar workers, x20, N = 631) = 4.96, p = .026. Specifically, job
constrained simultaneous between-group analyses are pre- involvement was positively related to W -* F conflict among
sented on lines 3 and 4, respectively. The nonsignificant be- white-collar workers (standardized coefficient = . 11, p = .056),
tween-group chi-square difference test (line 5) indicated that whereas these two variables were unrelated among blue-collar
there was no evidence of significant gender differences in the workers (standardized coefficient = -.02, p = .688). This find-
parameter estimates for the 16 hypothesized relationships. The ing lends qualified support for a relationship between job in-
lack of gender differences also was supported by the fact that volvement and W - F conflict. Second, the relationship be-
the values of the NFI, NNFI, and CFI from the unconstrained tween W - F conflict and family distress was significantly
and constrained between-group models were virtually identi- different across the two collar-color groups, x2(l, N = 631) =
cal. Had there been substantial gender differences, the values 6.59, p = .010. Consistent with the results from the full sample,
of the NFI, NNFI, and CFI from the constrained model would W -* F conflict was not significantly related to family distress
have been smaller than their respective values from the uncon- among white-collar workers (standardized coefficient = -.09,
strained model. Given these results, we conclude that the p = . 139). However, W -* F conflict was positively related to
model generalizes across men and women. family distress among blue-collar workers (standardized coeffi-
Race. The within-group fit indices suggested that the model cient =. 12, p = .061). Thus, this finding lends qualified support

Table 3
Goodness-of-Fit Information for Within- and Between-Group Comparisons
Group 4f NFI NNFI CFI

Gender
Male (within-group, n = 278) 8.82 12 .712 .985 1.000 1.000
Female (within-group, n = 353) 14.86 12 .249 .977 .986 .995
Unconstrained between-group model 23.68 24 .480 .981 1.000 1.000
Constrained between-group model 40.10 40 .466 .968 1.000 1.000
X2 difference (constrained-unconstrained) 16.42 16 .424

Race
White (n = 262) 16.15 12 .185 .968 .974 .991
Non-White (n = 369) 13.64 12 .325 .982 .993 .998
Unconstrained 29.79 24 .192 .977 .986 .995
Constrained 45.06 40 .269 .965 .992 .996
Difference 15.27 16 .505
Job type
Blue-collar (n = 312) 14.81 12 .252 .979 .988 .996
White-collar ( = 319) 12.88 12 .378 .979 .995 .998
Unconstrained 27.69 24 .273 .979 .991 .997
Constrained 63.83 40 .010 .952 .966 .981
Difference 36.14 16 .003
Note. NFI = normed fit index; NNFI = nonnormed fit index; and CFI = comparative fit index.
74 M. FRONE, M. RUSSELL, AND M. COOPER

for the path between W -* F conflict and family distress in our suraman et al., 1989) is not direct, but rather is mediated by the
model. frequency of F -* W conflict.
Fourth, the unique pattern of domain-specific antecedents
Discussion and affective outcomes associated with the two forms of WFC
suggests that this construct plays a key mediating role in link-
The primary goal of this study was to examine the anteced- ing work and family stress processes. By virtue of its central
ents and outcomes of WFC and to assess whether WFC plays an location in models of the work-family interface, WFC draws
important integrative function in work and family stress re- attention to the fact that work and family conditions (e.g., stress-
search. Given that WFC was the primary focus of this study, we ors and involvement) may spill over to influence the quality of
begin our discussion by examining findings directly related to life associated with the other life domain. For example, pre-
W - F and F -* W conflict. We then discuss, in turn, findings vious research suggests that family stressors (Parasuraman et
regarding within-domain relationships, the generalizability of al., 1989) and homemaking commitment (Klein, 1988) are as-
our model, study limitations, and directions for future research. sociated with decreased job satisfaction. Our results suggest
that any relationship between either family stressors or home-
Work-Family Conflict making commitment and job-related affect is not direct, but is
mediated by the frequency of F - W conflict. Furthermore,
The present results revealed several general findings con- given the consistent positive relationship between F -* W con-
cerning WFC. First, as hypothesized, there was a positive recip- flict and job distress, coupled with the fact that W -* F conflict
rocal relationship between W -* F conflict and F - W con- was positively associated with family distress only among blue-
flict. This finding is consistent with conceptual definitions of collar workers, family-related factors appear more likely to in-
WFC, which suggest that the conflict relationship between fluence the quality of work life than vice versa. This pattern of
work and family is bidirectional or reciprocal (e.g., Greenhaus results is intriguing in light of Crouter's (1984) observation that
& Beutell, 1985). Second, both types of WFC were associated past research has emphasized the influence of work on the
with unique antecedent conditions. As one would expect, job family rather than the reverse.
stressors and job involvement were positively related to the fre- Finally, the present study did not support a fully mediated
quency of W - F conflict. Conversely, family stressors and relationship, via work or family distress, between WFC and
family involvement were positively related to the frequency of depression as suggested by a strict interpretation of additive
F -* W conflict. Although the findings concerning the direct models of overall quality of life (e.g., Rice et al., 1985, in press).
predictors of W -* F conflict are consistent with previous re- Specifically, F -* W conflict was both indirectly (via job dis-
search, our investigation of the direct predictors of F - W tress) and directly related to depression. In contrast, the rela-
conflict and the reciprocal relationship between W -* F and tionship of job and family stressors to depression was fully
F -* W conflict represents an extension of previous research. mediated by job and family distress, respectively. These results
Consequently, this study clarifies the positive relationship be- suggest that there may be a fundamental difference between
tween family stressors and W - F conflict documented in within-domain stressors and WFC that allows the latter to di-
previous studies (e.g., Burke, 1988; Kopelman et al., 1983; Voy- rectly influence one's overall sense of well-being. For example,
danoff, 1988), suggesting that any positive relationship between viewed from self-identity theory (e.g., Schlenker, 1987), the two
these two variables is indirect via increased F -* W conflict. forms of WFC may represent not only a source of pressure that
Third, both types of WFC were positively related to a spe- reduces the quality of life in a given domain, but also a threat or
cific measure of within-domain distress. As hypothesized, F -* impediment to self-identification. At the core of self-identity
W conflict was positively related to work distress, whereas theory is the notion that "people strive to construct and main-
W - F conflict was positively related to family distress, at least tain desired identity images" (Schlenker, 1987, p. 274). Further-
among blue-collar workers. These results suggest that if the more, people are threatened when their self-images are dam-
demands and responsibilities associated with one role fre- aged by impediments to self-identifying activities. As defined
quently interfere with the enactment of a second role, the qual- by Schlenker (1987), an impediment represents a "blocking or
ity of life associated with the second role may suffer. In contrast interruption of activity that has potential implications for peo-
to the present study, the principal hypothesis guiding past re- ple's goals and identity" (p. 277). Thus, F -* W conflict might
search was that W -* F conflict would be negatively related to represent a threat to constructing or maintaining a desired job-
both job and family satisfaction. It is also interesting to note related self-image that has direct implications for an individ-
that a precise explanation for this general hypothesis has not ual's overall sense of well-being.
been offered other than that W -* F conflict is a type of The fact that only F -* W conflict was related to depression
stressor and stressors are related to affective outcomes. The lack suggests that the relationship between W - F conflict and
of specificity concerning the relationship between W - F con- overall psychological distress or life satisfaction reported in pre-
flict and within-domain affect may reflect the fact that pre- vious studies (e.g., Bedeian et al., 1988; Kopelman et al., 1983;
vious research has not simultaneously considered both types of Parasuraman et al., 1989) may have been spurious because of a
WFC. Thus, the present study offers a refinement to this gen- failure to control for the positive relationship between W -* F
eral hypothesis linking WFC and domain-specific affect. On and F -* W conflict. Similarly, the two studies (Coverman,
the basis of our findings, we would argue that the relationship 1989; Rice et al., in press) that reported only an indirect rela-
between W - F conflict and job-related affect found in pre- tionship between WFC and overall life satisfaction (via job or
vious studies (e.g., Beutell & O'Hare, 1987; Burke, 1988; Para- family satisfaction) may have suffered from a methodological
WORK-FAMILY CONFLICT 75

weakness. Both studies used data from the 1977 Quality of though both indirect relationships (via F -* W conflict and
Employment Survey (Quinn & Staines, 1979), which used a family distress) between family stressors and depression are
mixed directional measure of WFC (i.e., "How much do your positive, family involvement has both a positive (via F -* W
job and family life interfere with one another?"). If the esti- conflict) and a negative (via family distress) indirect relation-
mated relationship between a mixed directional measure of ship to depression.
WFC and overall life satisfaction is functionally equivalent to
averaging across the separate effects of W - F and F -* W Generalizability of the Model
conflict, it is perhaps not surprising that no direct effect was
observed. This would be especially true if mixed directional Analyses examining the generalizability of our model
measures primarily assess the experience of W -* F conflict yielded no evidence of significant gender or racial differences
(Frone et al., 1991; Gutek et al., 1991). in the overall fit of the model or in the magnitude or direction
It is not clear, however, why only F -* W conflict should be of the 16 hypothesized relationships. The lack of observed
directly related to overall assessments of well-being. Perhaps gender differences supports the results of two previous studies
F - W conflict is more likely to lead to diminished percep- that tested for gender differences in models of WFC (Bedeian
tions of overall self-efficacy or mastery, which then lead to et al, 1988; Rice et al, in press). In addition, the lack of racial
heightened depressive affect or decreased satisfaction with life differences is consistent with one study that tested for such
(e.g., Bandura, 1989; Marshall & Lang, 1990; Pearlin, Men- differences (Rice et al, in press). These results suggest that
aghan, Lieberman, & Mullen, 1981). Because individuals are models of the work-family interface may operate similarly
ultimately responsible for managing their family life in such a across gender and race.
way that it does not interfere with their jobs, the frequent experi- In contrast, although the model fit well among both blue-
ence of F - W conflict may indicate that any difficulty they and white-collar workers, the parameter estimates for two paths
experience in constructing or maintaining a desired job-related differed across these two groups. Specifically, the hypothesized
self-image is due to their inability to effectively manage their relationship between W -* F conflict and family distress was
family demands. In contrast, individuals may be less likely to only observed among blue-collar workers, whereas the hypoth-
accept direct responsibility for managing their work roles in esized relationship between job involvement and W -* F con-
such a way that they do not interfere with their family life. flict was only observed among white-collar workers. Although
Rather, individuals may be more likely to hold their organiza- we do not have a ready explanation for the first cross-group
tions responsible for the occurrence of W -* F conflict. Thus, difference, the second difference is interesting because pre-
the experience of W - F conflict may be less likely to under- vious research documenting a relationship between job involve-
mine one's overall sense of self-efficacy or mastery. ment and W -* F conflict has only used respondents holding
white-collar jobs (Beutell, 1983; Beutell & O'Hare, 1987; Frone
& Rice, 1987; Greenhaus & Kopelman, 1981; Greenhaus et al,
Within-Domain Relationships
1989; Wiley, 1987). The fact that job involvement and W -* F
The results of this study replicated and extended several find- conflict were positively related among white-collar workers and
ings reported in previous research. First, job and family stress- were unrelated among blue-collar workers may reflect differ-
ors were positively related to their respective within-domain ences in the structural characteristics of jobs occupied by these
measures of distress (e.g., Bedeian et al., 1988; Kandel et al, two groups. For example, the hypothesized relationship be-
1985; Kopelman et al, 1983; Parasuraman et al, 1989). Second, tween job involvement and W -* F conflict was based on the
job involvement and family involvement were negatively related premise that high levels of psychological involvement in one's
to job distress and family distress, respectively. Although the job would lead to a higher probability of spending discretion-
relationship between job involvement and job distress is consis- ary time at work, completing work at home, or being psychologi-
tent with previous research (e.g, Rabinowitz & Hall, 1977; Se- cally engaged with work-related matters at home (Greenhaus &
karan, 1989; Weiner et al, 1987; Weiner & Vardi, 1980), our Beutell, 1985). However, even though blue-collar workers may
finding of a negative relationship between family involvement attach high levels of psychological importance to their jobs, the
and family distress extends such research. Third, both job dis- nature of their jobs may be such that they are enacted within a
tress and family distress were related to depression (e.g, fixed period of time (e.g, 9 a.m. to 5 p.m.). In other words,
Bedeian et al, 1988; Coverman, 1989; Kandel et al, 1985; Ko- blue-collar workers may be less likely to spend discretionary
pelman et al, 1983; Rice et al, in press). Fourth, the relation- time at their place of employment and be less likely to bring
ships of the stressor and involvement measures were indirectly work home than white-collar workers.
related to depression via their corresponding within-domain
measures of distress. Although the indirect relationships be-
Limitations
tween the stressors and depression via within-domain distress
are consistent with previous research (Bedeian et al, 1988; Co- Two important methodological limitations should be ac-
verman, 1989; Kandel et al, 1985; Kopelman et al, 1983), the knowledged. First, although our model was conceptualized in
parallel finding involving the relationships between psychologi- terms of putative causes and effects, it is recognized that the use
cal involvement and depression extends work and family re- of cross-sectional, correlational data does not allow us to draw
search. An additional finding was that family stressors and causal inferences concerning the various hypothesized rela-
family involvement were indirectly related to depression via tionships. Second, because the data were based on self-reports,
F -* W conflict. It should be pointed out, however, that al- the magnitude of relationships may have been inflated because
76 M. FRONE, M. RUSSELL, AND M. COOPER

of method variance or general personality dispositions such as ily, such as sibling or offspring. Broadening family-related mea-
negative affectivity (Brief, Burke, George, Robinson, & Web- sures would provide a more complete picture of the role of
ster, 1988). Although we cannot directly rule out such a possibil- family in models of the work-family interface.
ity, it is useful to point out that the influence of these two meth- The mediational processes assumed to underlie many of the
odological artifacts should be relatively constant across all rela- individual relationships also should be examined in future re-
tionships. Thus, if the relationships observed in this study were search. For example, the hypothesized positive relationship be-
largely a function of either method bias or personality disposi- tween family involvement and F -* W conflict was based on
tions, we would have expected to find significant structural the assumption that high levels of family involvement lead to an
relationships where none were predicted. Given the strong sup- increase in time and energy devoted to family responsibilities
port for the specific pattern of relationships outlined in our and in the likelihood of being psychologically preoccupied
model, it seems unlikely that our findings are solely attributable with family-related matters while at work. Presumably it is
to either method variance or stable personality dispositions. these differences in time and in physical and psychological
Moreover, in a systematic examination of this issue with regard energy devoted to family that ultimately link family involve-
to relationships between chronic job stressors and affective out- ment to the frequency of F -* W conflict. A detailed examina-
comes, Chen and Spector (1991) found that such relationships tion of the mediational processes thought to underlie the rela-
are largely unaffected by controls for negative affectivity (see tionships documented in this study would provide a more com-
Schaubroeck & Ganster, 1991, for a review). plete understanding of the work-family interface.
Another avenue for future research would be to examine
Future Research both W -* F and F -* W conflict with respect to a broader set
of outcome variables, such as job performance, tardiness or
Future research can extend the present study in several im- absenteeism, and turnover. For example, one might expect to
portant ways. Because many of the relationships outlined in our find a direct relationship between F -* W conflict and tardi-
model have not been examined previously, future research ness or absenteeism, whereas W -* F conflict may be directly
should attempt to replicate our findings. Ideally a replication of related to voluntary turnover. Finally, future research should
the present study would use longitudinal data. Assuming one's examine whether different types of family supportive programs
model is correctly specified, longitudinal data have two poten- or coping behaviors are needed to deal with exposure to the two
tial advantages over cross-sectional data. First, longitudinal different types of WFC. For example, limiting overtime hours
data allow stronger inferences concerning cause and effect than or allowing employees to work extra hours when it is most con-
do cross-sectional data. Second, as noted by Gollub and Rei- venient (e.g., later in the evening after children have gone to
chardt (1987), longitudinal data are more appropriate than bed) may be more likely to reduce W -* F conflict, whereas
cross-sectional data for estimating the magnitude of causal ef- reliable child care arrangements may be more likely to reduce
fects. However, these advantages are only realized if one knows F -* W conflict.
a priori the optimal time lag for a given relationship. In the
absence of appropriate time lags, longitudinal data will provide Conclusion
biased parameter estimates that may be worse than those ob-
tained from cross-sectional data (Gollub & Reichardt, 1987; The present study examined the intricate relationships be-
Kessler, 1987; Kessler & Greenberg, 1981; Leventhal & Tomar- tween work and family within the context of a complex multi-
ken, 1987). variate model of the work-family interface. Consequently, new
Testing the present model using longitudinal data would be a insights into WFC and its integrative role in models of the
complex task given that no theory exists to elucidate the opti- work-family interface were revealed. The results of this study
mal time lag associated with the various relationships and that clearly underscore the fundamental importance of distinguish-
the optimal time lag would undoubtedly differ across relation- ing between W -+ F and F -* W conflict and support the use of
ships. Given these difficulties, a daily diary study, perhaps last- multivariate models in future research on the work-family in-
ing 8 to 12 weeks, would seem to provide an appropriate data terface. If researchers continue to examine bivariate relation-
collection strategy (see Kessler, 1987, for a discussion of ana- ships or relatively incomplete models, they may risk the develop-
lytic issues involved in diary studies). Such a design would allow ment of erroneous and incomplete conclusions regarding the
work-family researchers to examine the various relationships interplay of these two important domains of life.
outlined in our model using time lags of different lengths. This
approach would allow stronger causal inferences, provide more References
accurate information concerning the magnitude of causal rela-
tionships, and contribute to the development of knowledge Andrews, F. M., & Withey, S. (1976). Social indicators of well-being in
concerning optimal time lags. America: Americans' perceptions of life quality. New \brk: Plenum
Future research also could broaden the domain of the family- Press.
Bacharach, S. B., Bamberger, P., & Conley, S. (1991). Work-home con-
related variables. For example, our family-related stressor, in- flict among nurses and engineers: Mediating the impact of role
volvement, and psychological distress measures contained stress on burnout and satisfaction at work. Journal of Organizational
items that assessed each of these constructs with respect to Behavior, 12, 39-53.
marital and parental roles. Although these two roles form the Bandura, A. (1989). Human agency in social cognitive therapy. Ameri-
core of the nuclear family, family-related measures could be can Psychologist, 44,1175-1184.
broadened to incorporate roles associated with extended fam- Bedeian, A. G., Burke, B. G., & Moffett, R. G. (1988). Outcomes of
WORK-FAMILY CONFLICT 77

work-family conflict among married male and female profes- Greenhaus, J. H., Parasuraman, S., Granrose, C. S., Rabinowitz, S., &
sionals. Journal of Management, 14, 475-491. Beutell, N. J. (1989). Sources of work-family conflict among two-
Beehr, T. A. (1976). Perceived situational moderators of the relation- career couples. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 34,133-153.
ship between subjective role ambiguity and role strain. Journal of Gutek, B. A., Searle, S., & Klepa, L. (1991). Rational versus gender role
Applied Psychology, 61, 35-40. explanations for work-family conflict. Journal of Applied Psychol-
Bentler, P. M. (1989). EQS structural equations program manual. Los ogy, 76, 560-568.
Angeles: BMDP Statistical Software. Hall, D. T., & Richter, J. (1988). Balancing work life and home life:
Berry, W D. (1984). Nonrecursive causal models. Newbury Park, CA: What can organizations do to help? Academy of Management Execu-
Sage. tive, 2,213-223.
Beutell, N. J. (1983, August). An investigation of conflict between work Holahan, C. K., & Gilbert, L. A. (1979). Conflict between major life
and nonwork roles: Work-role salience, family role expectations, and roles: Women and men in dual career couples. Human Relations, 32,
satisfaction. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Academy 451-467.
of Management, Dallas. House, J. S., McMichael, A. J., Wells, J. A., Kaplan, B. H., & Lander-
Beutell, N. I, & O'Hare, M. M. (1987). Work-nonwork conflict among man, L. R. (1979). Occupational stress and health among factory
MBAs: Sex differences in role stressors and life satisfaction. Work workers. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 20,139-160.
and Stress, 1, 35-41. Insel, P. M., & Moos, R. H. (1974). Work Environment Scale, Form R.
Blau, G. J. (1985). A multiple study investigation of the dimensionality Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologist Press.
of job involvement. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 27,19-36. Jackson, S. E., & Schuler, R. S. (1990). Human resource planning: Chal-
Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New lenges for industrial/organizational psychology. American Psycholo-
York: Wiley. gist, 45, 223-239.
Brief, A. P., Burke, M. J., George, J. M., Robinson, B. S., & Webster, J. Kandel, D. B., Davies, M., & Raveis, V H. (1985). The stressfulness of
(1988). Should negative aflectivity remain an unmeasured variable daily social roles for women: Marital, occupational and household
in the study of job stress? Journal of Applied Psychology, 73,193-198. roles. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 26, 64-78.
Burke, R. J. (1988). Some antecedents and consequences of work-fam- Kanungo, R. N. (1982). Measurement of job and work involvement.
ily conflict. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 3, 287-302. Journal of Applied Psychology, 67, 341-349.
Burke, R. J., & Greenglass, E. R. (1987). Work and family. In C. L. Kessler, R. C. (1985). [1985 Detroit area survey]. Unpublished ques-
Cooper & I. T. Robertson (Eds.), International review of industrial tionnaire. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan, Institute for Social
and organizational psychology 1987 (pp. 273-320). New York: Wiley. Research.
Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., & Rodgers, W (1976). The quality of
Kessler, R. C. (1987). The interplay of research design strategies and
American life: Perceptions, evaluations, and satisfactions. New York:
data analysis procedures in evaluating the effects of stress on health.
Russell Sage Foundation.
In S. V Kasl & C. L. Cooper (Eds.), Stress and health: Issues in re-
Chen, P. Y, & Spector, P. E. (1991). Negative affectivity as the underly-
search methodology (pp. 113-140). New \brk: Wiley.
ing cause of correlations between stressors and strain. Journal of
Kessler, R. C., & Greenberg, D. F. (1981). Liner panel analysis: Models
Applied Psychology, 76, 398-407.
of quantitative change. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Coverman, S. (1989). Role overload, role conflict, and stress: Address-
ing consequences of multiple role demands. Social Forces, 67, 965- Klein, H. (1988). Job satisfaction in professional dual-career couples:
982. Psychological and socioeconomic variables. Journal of Vocational
Crouter, A. C. (1984). Spillover from family to work: The neglected side Behavior, 32, 255-268.
of the work-family interface. Human Relations, 37, 425-442. Kline, M., & Cowan, P. A. (1988). Re-thinking the connections among
Eckenrode, J., & Gore, S. (1990). Stress and coping at the boundary of work and family well-being: A model for investigating employment
work and family. In J. Eckenrode & S. Gore (Eds.), Stress between and family work contexts. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality,
work and family (pp. 1-16). New York: Plenum Press. 3, 61-90.
Farkas, A. J., & Tetrick, L. E. (1989). A three-wave longitudinal analysis Kopelman, R. E., Greenhaus, J. H., & Connolly, T. F. (1983). A model of
of the causal ordering of satisfaction and commitment on turnover work, family, and interrole conflict: A construct validation study.
decisions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 855-868. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 32,198-215.
Frone, M. R., & Rice, R. W (1987). Work-family conflict: The effect of Lambert, S. J. (1990). Processes linking work and family: A critical
job and family involvement. Journal of Occupational Behaviour, 8, review and research agenda. Human Relations, 43, 239-257.
45-53. Leventhal, H., & Tomarken, A. (1987). Stress and illness: Perspectives
Frone, M. R., Russell, M., & Cooper, M. L. (1991). Prevalence of work- from health psychology. In S. V Kasl & C. L. Cooper (Eds.), Stress
family conflict: Are work and family boundaries asymmetrically per- and health: Issues in research methodology (pp. 27-55). New York:
meable? Manuscript submitted for publication. Wiley.
Gollub, H. F., & Reichardt, C. S. (1987). Taking account of time lags in Lodahl, T. M., & Kejner, M. (1965). Definition and measurement of
causal models. Child Development, 58, 80-92. job involvement. Journal of Applied Psychology, 49, 24-33.
Greenhaus, J. H. (1988). The intersection of work and family roles: Marshall, G. N., & Lang, E. L. (1990). Optimism, self-mastery, and
Individual, interpersonal, and organizational issues. Journal of So- symptoms of depression in women professionals. Journal ofPerson-
cial Behavior and Personality, 3, 23-44. ality and Social Psychology, 59,132-139.
Greenhaus, J. H., & Beutell, N. J. (1985). Sources of conflict between Matthews, K. A., & Rodin, J. (1989). Women's changing work roles:
work and family roles. Academy of Management Review, 10, 76-88. Impact on health, family, and public policy. American Psychologist,
Greenhaus, J. H., & Kopelman, R. E. (1981). Conflict between work 44,1389-1393.
and nonwork roles: Implications for the career planning process. Michalos, A. C. (1985). Multiple discrepancy theory (MDT). Social
Human Resource Planning, 4,1-10. Indicators Research, 16, 347-413.
Greenhaus, J. H., & Parasuraman, S. (1986). A work-nonwork interac- Netemeyer, R. G, Johnston, M. W, & Burton, S. (1990). Analysis of role
tive perspective of stress and its consequences. Journal of Organiza- conflict and role ambiguity in a structural equations framework.
tional Behavior Management, 8, 37-60. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75,148-157.
78 M. FRONE, M. RUSSELL, AND M. COOPER

Offerman, L. R., & Cowing, M. K. (1990). Organizations of the future: cial stress. In C. R. Snyder & C. E. Ford (Eds.), Coping with negative
Changes and challenges. American Psychologist, 45, 95-108. life events: Clinical and social psychological perspectives (pp. 273-
Parasuraman, S., Greenhaus, J. H., Granrose, C. S., Rabinowitz, S., & 321). New York: Plenum Press.
Beutell, N. J. (1989, August). Stressors, social support, and well-being Sekaran, U. (1989). Paths to the job satisfaction of bank employees.
among two-career couples. Paper presented at the annual meeting of Journal of Organizational Behavior, 10, 347-359.
the Academy of Management, Washington, DC. Sims, H. P., Szilagyi, A. D, & Keller, R. T. (1976). The measurement of
Pearlin, L. I., Menaghan, E. G., Lieberman, M. A., & Mullen, J. T. job characteristics. Academy of Management Journal, 19, 547-559.
(1981). The stress process. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 22, U.S. Department of Commerce, Bureau of the Census. (1982). 1980
337-356. Census of Population: Alphabetical index of industries and occupa-
Pearlin, L. I., & Schooler, C. (1978). The structure of coping. Journal of tions (Final ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Health and Social Behavior, 19, 2-21. Voydanoff, P. (1987). Work and family life. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Pleck, J. H., Staines, G. L., & Lang, L. (1980). Conflicts between work Voydanoff, P. (1988). Work role characteristics, family structure de-
and family life. Monthly Labor Review, 103, 29-32. mands, and work/family conflict. Journal of Marriage and the Fam-
Quinn, R., & Staines, G. (1979). Quality of Employment Survey. Ann ily, 50,749-761.
Arbor: University of Michigan, Institute for Social Research. Wayne, S. J., & Ferris, G. R. (1990). Influence tactics, affect, and ex-
Rabinowitz, S., & Hall, D. T. (1977). Organizational research on job change quality in supervisor-subordinate interactions: A laboratory
involvement. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 265-288. experiment and field study. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 487-
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale 499.
for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Mea- Weiner, Y, Muczyk, J. P., & Gable, M. (1987). Relationships between
surement, I, 385-501. work commitments and experience of personal well-being. Psycho-
Rice, R. W, Frone, M. R., & McFarlin, D. B. (in press). Work-nonwork logical Reports, 60, 459-466.
conflict and the perceived quality of life. Journal of Organizational Weiner, Y, & Vardi, Y. (1980). Relationships between job, organiza-
Behavior. tion, and career commitments and work outcomesAn integrative
Rice, R. W, McFarlin, D. B., Hunt, R. G, & Near, J. P. (1985). Organiza- approach. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 26,
tional work and the perceived quality of life. Academy of Manage- 81-96.
ment Review, 10, 296-310. Wiley, D. L. (1987). The relationship between work/nonwork role con-
Rizzo, J., House, R. 1, & Lirtzman, S. I. (1970). Role conflict and flict and job-related outcomes: Some unanticipated findings. Jour-
ambiguity in complex organizations. Administrative Science Quar- nal of Management, 13, 467-472.
terly, 75,150-163. Williams, L. J., & Hazer, J. T. (1986). Antecedents and consequences of
Schaubroeck, J. (1990). Investigating reciprocal causation in organiza- satisfaction and commitment in turnover models: A reanalysis using
tional research. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 11,17-28. latent variable structural equation methods. Journal of Applied Psy-
chology, 71, 219-231.
Schaubroeck, J., Cotton, J. L., & Jennings, K. R. (1989). Antecedents
Zedeck, S., & Mosier, K. L. (1990). Work in the family and employing
and consequences of role stress: A covariance structure analysis.
organization. American Psychologist, 45, 240-251.
Journal of Organizational Behavior, 10, 35-58.
Schaubroeck, J., & Ganster, D. C. (1991). The role of negative affectiv-
ity in work-related stress. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, Received March 25,1991
6,319-330. Revision received August 30,1991
Schlenker, B. R. (1987). Threats to identity: Self-identification and so- Accepted September 4,1991

You might also like