You are on page 1of 14

Journal of Travel Research http://jtr.sagepub.

com/

A General Theory of Tourism Consumption Systems: A Conceptual Framework and an Empirical


Exploration
Arch G. Woodside and Chris Dubelaar
Journal of Travel Research 2002 41: 120
DOI: 10.1177/004728702237412

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://jtr.sagepub.com/content/41/2/120

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

On behalf of:

Travel and Tourism Research Association

Additional services and information for Journal of Travel Research can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://jtr.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

Subscriptions: http://jtr.sagepub.com/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Citations: http://jtr.sagepub.com/content/41/2/120.refs.html

>> Version of Record - Nov 1, 2002

What is This?

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at Bond University Library on September 16, 2014


10.1177/004728702237412
NOVEMBER
JOURNAL OF2002
TRAVEL RESEARCH

A General Theory of Tourism Consumption


Systems: A Conceptual Framework
and an Empirical Exploration
ARCH G. WOODSIDE AND CHRIS DUBELAAR

A tourism consumption system (TCS) is defined as the set TOURISM CONSUMPTION SYSTEMS
of related travel thoughts, decisions, and behaviors by a dis-
cretionary traveler prior to, during, and following a trip. The Decisions and behaviors by travelers and tourists repre-
central proposition of a theory of TCS is that the thoughts, sent a rich mosaic of relationships among multiple sets of
decisions, and behaviors regarding one activity influence the variables. These variables include the following:
thoughts, decisions, and behaviors for a number of other ac-
tivities. Using exit interview travel data and quick clustering • background variables (demographic, psychographic,
analysis, this article empirically examines seven basic TCS and social);
propositions pertaining to decisions made once the destina- • destination marketing and related service marketing
tion has been selected. The findings support and extend the influences—for example, destination advertising Web
basic propositions specifically indicating clear patterns in sites and offers to provide inquirers with free visitor in-
the behaviors of visitors to Prince Edward Island, Canada. formation guides (VIGs) and the information and
The authors conclude by recommending that this approach is persuasiveness of these VIGs; related service market-
useful for tourism marketers and practitioners in general. ing influences include event and attraction marketing
Suggestions are provided for analyzing TCS to increase the and advertising by car rental firms, restaurants, and
effectiveness of tourism marketing strategies. accommodations;
• prior trip behavior, information search, and current trip
The purpose of this article is to suggest a framework for a planning;
theory of tourism consumption systems (TCSs) and an • choices and behaviors regarding destinations, trans-
exploratory empirical examination to illuminate this frame- portation modes, travel routes, accommodations, visit-
work. A tourism consumption system is defined as the set of ing attractions, restaurants and foods, durable pur-
related travel thoughts, decisions, and behaviors by a discre- chases, and local-area destinations;
tionary traveler prior to, during, and following a trip. The • micro and macro evaluations and satisfactions (i.e., re-
garding individual and global consumption events oc-
central proposition of a theory of TCSs is that the thoughts,
curring during the trip); and
decisions, and behaviors regarding one activity influence the • conations (e.g., willingness and intentions to repeat the
thoughts, decisions, and behaviors for a number of other tourism-related consumption events, such as visiting
activities, implying that behavioral patterns should be visible the same destination in the future).
in the consumption of tourism offerings.
A deep understanding of tourism behavior needs to For example, air versus car travel from home to a major des-
extend beyond theories and focus on information search and tination is likely to influence the use of destination-area
destination choice for which ample models already exist transportation mode, places visited, and event participation
(e.g., see Bello and Etzel 1985; Fodness and Murray 1999; in the local destination vicinity. Also, travelers’ use versus
Woodside and Lysonski 1989). The “defining moments” nonuse of VIGs (made available by many local and regional
(i.e., events vividly and automatically retrieved from mem-
Arch G. Woodside is a professor of marketing in the Carroll
ory) of a trip may not relate, per se, to unique features of a School of Management at Boston College in Boston. Chris
destination area. Thus, the theory of TCS focuses on achiev- Dubelaar is a senior lecturer in the Department of Marketing at
ing deep understanding of the multiple immediate and down- Monash University in Melbourne, Australia. The authors thank the
stream relationships among events prior to, during, and fol- four JTR reviewers and Charles R. Goeldner for the helpful com-
ments and suggested revisions on earlier drafts of this article. They
lowing a discretionary trip rather than predicting and gratefully acknowledge Roberta MacDonald, University of Prince
explaining destination choice. In particular, this article Edward Island, Charlottetown, and the Marketing Agency, Prince
focuses on the actions and decisions subsequent to the choice Edward Island, for the data files used to empirically explore the
propositions in this article.
and details the patterns of behavior, highlighting the regular-
Journal of Travel Research, Vol. 41, November 2002, 120-132
ity and striking differences present in groups that travel to the DOI: 10.1177/004728702237412
same destination. © 2002 Sage Publications

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at Bond University Library on September 16, 2014


JOURNAL OF TRAVEL RESEARCH 121

FIGURE 1
MODEL OF DISTANCE, USE OF ADVERTISING INFORMATION, AND FIRST/PRIOR VISIT
BEHAVIOR ON PRIME MOTIVE, TRAVELER DESTINATION BEHAVIORS, AND OUTCOMES

Phase 1

Phase 2
Distance to Phase 3
Destination c
for the Traveler l
4 = very far
3 = far e Evaluation of
2 = mid specific
1 = near destination Conations
d Traveler Behaviors
in Destination Areas places and
b events • Willingness
• Length of stay to recommend
Prime j k
First (=1) versus
motive
• Accommodations used m the destination
Prior (=0) Visit • Destination activities
for trip to
• Destination areas visited • Willingness
destination
• Destination expenditures to visit
a f
• Gift-buying behavior Overall destination
g evaluations again
Use of Advertising h of destination
Information
2 = heavy
1 = light i
0 = None

Note: Figure 1 includes 13 propositions of direct relationships. Each proposition is identified by a lowercase letter. For example,
Pa: the greater the distance to the destination for the traveler, the greater the use of advertising information; Pb: the greater the
distance to the destination for the traveler, the greater the likelihood of a first visit to a destination.

tourism boards around the world) is likely to have many in- 1. an outdoor recreation experience begins by anticipa-
fluences beyond the destination choice decision. For exam- tion, including planning;
ple, use of VIGs may influence choice of accommodations 2. travel to the actual site (“in almost every instance
and restaurants, attractions and events, and gift-buying be- some travel is required, even if it is only a short walk
havior (Woodside, Trappey, and MacDonald 1997). or bicycle ride to a local playground,” p. 34);
Any given discretionary trip is complex in the many 3. on-site experience and activities (even though this
related thoughts, decisions, behaviors, and evaluations that experience is what many seem to think is the total out-
occur prior to, during, and following the trip. The discretion- door recreation experience, it “may be less than half
ary traveler’s thoughts, actions, and attitudes are likely to be of the total,” p. 33);
influenced by seemingly minor events that often trigger sub- 4. travel back, which is unlikely to be a duplicate of the
stantial investments in time and money. Consequently, com- travel to the site (“Even when the route is the same,
plexity and nuance need to be captured in modeling and the recreationists are different. . . . If travel back is
empirically examining discretionary trips. from a vacation, memories of the vacation and antici-
While related, the concept of TCS differs from Solo- pation of the job are certainly different from the
mon’s (1988, 1999) view of consumption constellations. thoughts on the outbound trip,” p. 34);
“Consumption constellations [are] sets of products and activ- 5. recollection and sharing of recollections with friends,
ities used by consumers to define, communicate, and per- relatives, and associates.
form social roles” (Solomon 1999, p. 562). TCS refers to the
related thoughts, actions, and behaviors of discretionary trav- Clawson and Knetsch (1966) concluded their five-phase
elers prior to, during, and following their trips. Unlike con- theory of the total recreation experience by recommending
sumption constellation research, the primary focus of the that “all in all, each phase of the total experience seems to
study of TCSs is not on social roles reflected by the com- merit serious, if not equal attention in research, in planning,
bined use of several seemingly unrelated products-services and in operations” (p. 36). The following theoretical devel-
and brands. TCS focuses on direct and indirect relationships opment builds on the contributions of Clawson and Knetsch.
occurring between all variables relevant for a discretionary
trip. Earlier theoretical work by Clawson and Knetsch (1966) Issues and Propositions in
emphasizes such a systems view. Tourism Consumption Systems
Clawson and Knetsch (1966, pp. 33-36) advocated a
broad view in describing the “total recreation experience” as Figure 1 summarizes an example set of specific TCS
five related but distinctly different phases: issues. The following questions represent initial issues posed

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at Bond University Library on September 16, 2014


122 NOVEMBER 2002

by a theory of TCS. How is the distance traveled associated lationship is expected for “visiting friends and family mem-
with whether or not it is a first visit or how the visitor used the bers” as a primary trip motivation. While these propositions
information available? How are each of the above associated appear intuitive, their confirmation and relative strengths
with motivations for the travel and behavior in the area? How need to be examined empirically. Otherwise, valuable in-
are the above associated with evaluations and conations of formation into the nuances of such relationships and
the area? downstream impacts on evaluations and conations may go
Figure 1 is not intended to be a complete view of TCS. unnoticed.
The figure reflects both direct and indirect relationships sup- A strategic outcome from examining the impacts of first-
ported in travel, tourism, and consumer research literatures time and repeat destination visits is achieving deep
(for earlier versions and discussions of similar frameworks, “sensemaking” (Weick 1995) into the multiple modes of
see Clawson and Knetsch 1966; Woodside and MacDonald planning, behaviors, and evaluations by different visitor seg-
1994; Woodside 2000). For simplicity and ease of discus- ments. Consequently, such deep sensemaking may result in
sion, Figure 1 is divided into three phases: using multiple unique destination marketing strategies for
uniquely defined target markets. An example of such a strat-
• Phase 1: prior and during travel relationships affecting egy might be marketing Prince Edward Island (a small Cana-
traveler behaviors in destination areas dian Atlantic maritime province) as a future destination by
• Phase 2: during and post travel relationships providing free copies to Japanese schoolchildren of Akage
• Phase 3: post travel relationships affecting future No Anne (Red-Haired Anne, the Japanese translation of Anne
behaviors of Green Gables) and marketing “relaxation and escaping
from overwork” to repeat visitors from Canadian origins.
The one-directional arrows in Figure 1 summarize sev-
eral hypotheses regarding TCSs. To increase clarity by sim- Proposition 3: Use of information influences (h) prime
plifying this introduction to TCSs, we did not include feed- motives for the trip to the destination, (i) visit behav-
back loops in Figure 1. However, several feedback loops are iors in the destination area, and (j) evaluations of des-
likely to be found in real-life TCSs. For example, the length tination places and events.
of stay and accommodations used in the destination areas
may be revised based on the visitor’s evaluations of specific Information use influences previsit expectations of what
destination experiences. The prime motive for the trip re- the traveler might do, as well as not do, in the destination
ported by the traveler may be revised based on the experi- area. For example, VIGs sent by Nevada and Las Vegas ver-
ences evaluated during the trip. The view reflects the mental
sus Prince Edward Island will stress different motivations for
model (i.e., set of implicit or explicit assumptions) (see
Senge 1990) advocated by Weick (1995): “the creation of visiting and different behaviors available while in the respec-
meaning is an attentional process, but it is attention to that tive destination areas.
which has already occurred. . . . People discover their own in- Heavy versus light users of destination information (e.g.,
tentions” [and frequently revise their beliefs and attitudes VIGs) often stay longer, do more activities in the destination
based on their observations of outcomes to their own behav- area, spend more money, are more satisfied with their visits,
ior] (pp. 25-37). have a more positive overall attitude about the destination
area, are more willing to visit the destination in the future,
Proposition 1: Travel party characteristics influence (a) and are more willing to recommend a trip to the destination to
trip-related use of information, (b) prior visits to the friends (Woodside, Trappey, and MacDonald 1997). These
destination area, (c) destination evaluation, (d) prime findings occurred for both first-time and repeat visitors.
motives, and (e) behaviors in destination areas. Tourism strategy implication: getting VIGs into the hands of
repeat and first-time visitors may be have high positive impacts
As shown by the initial five arrows in Figure 1, travel for both the visitors and the destination tourism industry.
party characteristics are hypothesized to influence most plan-
ning and doing variables in TCSs. Several studies confirm Proposition 4: Prime motives for the trip to the destina-
the strong influences of travel party characteristics on infor- tion influences travelers’ destination behaviors (arrow k
mation search and use strategies implemented by discretion- in Figure 1).
ary travelers (e.g., see Fodness and Murray 1998, 1999;
Snepenger et al. 1990). In general, the planning and doing be- While a given destination rarely can be all things to all
haviors of travelers are heavily influenced by the composi- visitors (the mistake of a “We have it all” marketing strat-
tion of the traveling party (McIntosh and Goeldner 1990). egy), visitors often can be segmented meaningfully into a
few groups according to their principal reason for selecting
Proposition 2: Prior visits to the primary destination area the destination. Lengths of stay, expenditure amounts, and
influence (f) search/use of advertising information accommodations used are likely to vary by visitors seg-
and (g) the prime motive for a trip to the destination. mented according to their primary motive for visiting the
destination. For example, visitors attracted to the destination
“Destination-naive” (Snepenger et al. 1990) travelers to experience a culture different from their own might be ex-
search and use information more extensively than repeat vis- pected to tour local destination areas, stay overnight in bed-
itors to the destination. Extensive use of VIGs is more likely and-breakfast accommodations, eat more often at restaurants,
to occur among first-time visitors compared to repeat visi- and have greater destination-related expenditures compared
tors. “Experiencing a different culture” is more likely to be to visitors motivated primarily to visit friends and relatives.
seen as a primary motivation for the trip to the destination However, stating the seemingly obvious does not inform on
among first-time compared to repeat visitors; the opposite re- the strength of such relationships. Consequently, the several

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at Bond University Library on September 16, 2014


JOURNAL OF TRAVEL RESEARCH 123

relationships implied by arrow k in Figure 1 need to be exam- tions with respect to the destination (arrow m in Fig-
ined empirically. ure 1), and postexperience evaluations influence
conations (arrow n in Figure 1).
Proposition 5: Visitor behaviors in the destination area
influence their evaluations with respect to the destina- While the consumer behavior literature (see Peter and
tion (l in Figure 1). Olson 1999) provides strong rationales for the propositions
displayed by evaluation-conation arrows m and n in Figure 1,
Visitors may be segmented according to their destination- Weick (1995, pp. 192-93) states the case vividly in a travel
area behaviors. Visitors experiencing versus not experienc- context:
ing certain behaviors are likely to evaluate destination places
and events differently. For example, related to visitors expe- People discover what they think by looking at what
riencing nightclubs during their stays in Charlottetown, they say, how they feel, and where they walk. The talk
Prince Edward Island (PEI), versus visitors experiencing his- makes sense of walking, which means those best able
torical sites, the first visitor segment may have lower evalua- to walk the talk are the ones who actually talk the
tions overall of Charlottetown versus the second segment. walking they find themselves doing most often,
While some overlap of segment membership may occur, with most intensity, and with most satisfaction.
membership in the two destination-area behavior segments How can I know what I value until I see where I walk?
may be mostly unique. People make sense of their actions, their walking,
The issues here focus on the following questions. What their talking.
do visitors actually do in the destination area, and how do
their behaviors influence their evaluations of specific desti- Thus, while potential visitors’ evaluations are likely to
nation places and events, as well as their overall evaluations show expectations on how much visitors will like their expe-
of the destination? What specific destination behaviors influ- riences in a destination region (e.g., “It should be fun!” and
ence positive and negative evaluations about the destination “The region looks beautiful [in the photographs].”), such
area? evaluations are only preliminary. These preliminary evalua-
Studies of visitors to four Mediterranean countries (Tur- tions are subject to substantial revisions following actual vis-
key, Egypt, Greece, and Italy) (Baloglu 2000) support the its and participation in activities in the destination region.
hypothesis that participating versus not participating in spe- Related to this theoretical view, Howard and Sheth (1969)
cific destination behaviors influences visitors’ evaluations proposed the use of “attitude” and “attitude′” (attitude prime)
concerning the destinations. Consequently, Baloglu (2000) to distinguish preexperience brand attitude from
advocated adopting Woodside’s (1982) recommendation to postexperience brand attitude′. Also, proposition 6 is
create destination positioning strategies based on visitors’ grounded in Smith and Swinyard’s (1983) hypothesis and
experiences that they subsequently evaluate very positively. empirical results that only after purchasing and using a prod-
The point is worth noting that three levels of traveler uct or service are anything resembling beliefs and attitudes
evaluations occur related to a discretionary trip: formed.
Proposition 7: Multiple paths occur in traveler behaviors
• Prior to starting the current trip, some evaluations oc-
within destination areas.
cur about specific places, attractions, accommoda-
tions, local destination area touring, and events in a An additional proposition not shown in Figure 1 is worth
destination area (micro evaluations), as well as an discussing: travelers visiting a destination region may be
overall assessment of the destination region (macro segmented usefully by their behavior while in the destination
evaluation). region. One sequence of steps, a single behavioral path, is un-
• During the trip, both micro and macro evaluations are
likely to dominate most discretionary travelers’ behaviors in
subject to revisions based on the experiences and bene- a destination region. For some segment of visitors, gift buy-
fits realized (Woodside and Jacobs 1985). ing may dominate destination areas visited (see Kim and
• After the trip is completed, both micro and macro eval-
Littrell 1999) and vice versa. Traveling local heritage road-
uations are likely to undergo further revisions based on ways may affect accommodation choices.
reflections and discussions about what happened dur- What causes some travelers to tour outlying regions
ing the trip and the meanings of these happenings (see within the destination region while others travel only within
Arnould and Price 1993). a very limited geographic area in the destination region? The
segmentation issue: what is the personal (demographic-
To reduce complexity and achieve simplicity, Figure 1 em- psychographic) and trip-related profile of each visitor seg-
phasizes only the second and third levels of evaluations— ment? Such issues are of theoretical and practical impor-
micro and macro trip-related evaluations occurring while tance. Learning the nuances in triggering behavior paths that
still in progress, as well as after most of the trip experiences include visits to outlying tourism-dependent destination
are complete. Also, micro evaluations based on direct experi- regions may be helpful in planning strategies to influence
ences have the greatest impact on macro evaluations—such such behaviors. Many examples exist of outlying tourism-
as global attitude toward the destination—and conations (for dependent local destination areas that earn only a small frac-
reviews on this rationale, see Peter and Olson 1999, chap. 6; tion of the tourism revenues received by the main regions
Fazio and Zanna 1987; Smith and Swinyard 1982). visited by tourists—for example, the eastern versus the west-
ern side of the Big Island of Hawaii and the outer Prince and
Proposition 6: Visitor evaluations of their experiences in King Counties versus Queen County in PEI.
the destination area influence their overall evalua-

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at Bond University Library on September 16, 2014


124 NOVEMBER 2002

METHOD represent less than 0.2% of total leisure visitors. To ensure a


high cooperation rate (88% achieved), native Japanese inter-
viewers conducted interviews with Japanese respondents in
The Database Japanese.
A secondary database was used to examine hypotheses
related to the arrows in Figure 1. The extensive data file (i.e., Response rate. The overall cooperation/completion inter-
face-to-face interviews with 2,239 travel parties) from the view rate was 94%. Due mainly to some nonresponses
1992 PEI exit survey was used. The “Marketing Agency” (a to some of the questions, the useable number of responses
provincial government-sponsored organization) and Roberta to test the propositions was close to 88% of the completed
MacDonald, University of Prince Edward Island, provided interviews.
these data to us. The only use of the data made prior to the
study reported here was a government descriptive report pro- Assessment of data collection method. The exit-intercept
filing visitors’ demographics, attitudes, and behaviors (Mar- method used for data collection offers several advantages.
keting Agency 1993) and a study on the impact of PEI’s 1992 First, the 94% cooperation and 88% useable response rates
advertising campaign on attitudes, behaviors, and traveler are considerably higher than reported in “inquiry conver-
expenditures (see Woodside, Trappey, and MacDonald sion” studies (i.e., survey research studies with respondents
1997). being contacted by mail or telephone several months after
The data were collected using a 12-page questionnaire. they had requested advertised free information about a desti-
The data collected included motives of the current trip to nation). Reported response rates in inquiry conversion stud-
PEI; the number of previous trips and their purposes; the ies average less than 60% (see Woodside and Dubelaar
length of time since the last trip to PEI; visitors’ perceptions 1999).
of the PEI visitor information guide and whether or not they Second, exit-intercept interviews are likely to minimize
received the guide before or after entering PEI; their aware- memory problems in retrieving details of the trip in respond-
ness and extent of use of the VIG; their evaluations of the ing to questions asked in the survey instrument. Also, unlike
visual appeal, ease of use, and amount of information in the mail and telephone procedures, all respondents are answer-
VIG; and their mode of entry into PEI (e.g., ferry, air, cruise ing the questions at the same time during their trip; thus, con-
ship—at the time of the study, no “fixed link” [i.e., bridge] founding of responses caused by varying lengths of time
had been constructed that attached PEI to the Canadian main- since the trip and completing the questionnaire is eliminated.
land). Additional data collected included the following top- Also, we can be close to certain that the respondents in
ics: type of accommodations used on PEI, evaluations of the exit-intercept studies actually visited the destination on their
accommodations used, participation in each of 15 activities current trip—they are face-to-face with the interviewer. Thus,
while in PEI and evaluations of activities done, visits to each “telescoping” does not occur. Telescoping by the respondent
of 10 attractions in PEI and evaluations of these attractions, includes moving events in memory forward in time to report
the destination areas visited in PEI and overnight stays there, a visit to the destination that is the focus of the study. Tele-
the perceived quality of PEI’s road signage, evaluations of scoping may occur in some instances because the respondent
PEI on 10 image items, expenditures in Canadian dollars wants to be helpful and enjoys telling about a trip made some
while in PEI (including credit card purchases and spending years ago to the destination being asked about. The most
by children), the proportions of total PEI expenditures by important advantage of the exit-intercept method may be the
eight categories, the travel party size and description, and ability to compare responses to acquirers of advertising
demographic information (age, marital status, education, information (e.g., free VIGs) with nonacquirers of such
employment outside the home, life cycle stage, annual information. Such a comparison is a quasi-experiment design
household income, and origin by province, state, and coun- of advertising effectiveness research of tourism marketing
try). Additional details on the specific questions in the ques- programs (e.g., see Woodside, Trappey, and MacDonald
tionnaire are available elsewhere (see Woodside, Trappey, 1997).
and MacDonald 1997). The disadvantages include the higher research costs com-
pared with mail and telephone interviews and the inability to
Questionnaire administration procedure. The interviews randomly stop travelers to ask for their participation in the
were completed during the tourism seasonal period (May 22 study for destinations that many travelers can enter and exit
to October 5, 1992) when more than 95% of leisure travelers without stopping. As an island with limited entry-exit access
visit PEI. The questionnaire was administered at all points of at the time of the study, natural stops and queuing aided the
exit from PEI (ferries, airports, and cruise ships) in matching data collection procedure for the PEI study.
proportions to total trip visits for each travel mode.
A quota sampling procedure was used to ensure that the Data analysis. Because this study is intended to be ex-
proportions of Canadian, U.S., and European respondents ploratory in examining systems of relationships between lei-
matched the population of visitors from these three origins: sure travel behaviors, “quick clustering” (Kamen 1970) is the
65% of completed interviews were with Canadians, and 31% main data analysis method used for examining the proposi-
were respondents from the United States; previous to the tions. Quick clustering includes correlation analysis fol-
study, two-thirds of PEI leisure visitors were estimated to be lowed by creating maps showing the highly significant rela-
Canadians, and about 30% were estimated to be Americans. tionships between the variables under examination. For each
The only exception to the quota-sampling rule involved of the variables examined, ordinal and interval variables
Japanese visitors. Because profiles of Japanese visitors were were developed to permit the calculation of Pearson product-
an objective for the study, nearly 1% of the total respondents moment correlations. All the correlations included in the
were Japanese visitors. Japanese visitors are estimated to quick clustering maps in the Findings section are significant

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at Bond University Library on September 16, 2014


JOURNAL OF TRAVEL RESEARCH 125

statistically (p < .01). While some of the reported correla- The data collection procedure was retrospective and
tions appear small (e.g., r = .10, p < .01), “tipping point” mostly self-reports. The data were collected at one point
analysis (not shown in the findings, but see Gladwell 1996; toward the end of visitors’ stays in PEI. Even though the data
McClelland 1998) indicates that the correlation coefficient are mostly self-reports, we can be certain that the respon-
understates the significance of the relationship being de- dents had actually been to PEI during the time frame of the
scribed between the two variables. The rationale for tipping study since the data were collected face-to-face at PEI exit
analysis: behavioral scientists have observed that societal locations. Not all the details presented in Figure 1 are
variables make little impact on a dependent variable until described empirically in this report. For example, local-area
they reach a certain critical level (e.g., McClelland 1998; for travel routes and the use of transportation modes in PEI are
a related discussion, also see Bass, Tigert, and Lonsdale not described in this report. Data were not collected on these
two variables in the primary study. The reported empirical
1968). Consequently, a linear correlation coefficient under-
study is intended to explore associations, not to test causal
estimates the strength of such relationships. Thus, rather than
paths. Tests of alternative models that explain dependent
concluding that a small correlation that is highly significant relationships are possible using partial correlations and path
is indicative of an unimportant relationship, a relevant path analysis (see Pedhazur 1982). The results of the quick clus-
relating the two variables should be noted. tering method employed in this report suggest the value in
examining the relative impacts of direct and indirect paths of
Unique Aspects about the Research Setting influence on identifiable dependent variables.
The links among travel party characteristics and prime
motive for the trip to the destination area and the behaviors in
the destination area may be rather distant in time as well as FINDINGS
place. For example, the reading by young schoolgirls of the
Japanese translation of Anne of Green Gables (a novel of an Phase 1 Findings
orphaned girl who is adopted by a sister and brother and
grows up on Prince Edward Island, Canada, written by Lucy Figure 2 shows findings relevant for variables in the first
Maude Montgomery and translated by Muraoka 1952) cre- four boxes in the left-hand side of Figure 1. To increase clar-
ates a deep-seated attraction to visit PEI. Muraoka’s transla- ity and partially to reduce complexity, we show only distance
tions of the series of Anne books have been reprinted more as a chronic traveler characteristic in Figure 2. Because the
than 100 times in Japan (Cole 1993). This subtle link results PEI visitor information guide is not published in Japanese
in more than 1,000 Japanese visitors to PEI annually. These and its availability was not advertised in Japan in 1991-1992,
“pilgrimages” (Cole 1993) by young Japanese women and the responses by Japanese visitors are excluded from the
their travel companions occur usually 5 to 10 years after their analysis resulting in Figure 2.
reading the Anne book series. Several different ordinal scales indicating physical dis-
The PEI provincial government maintains houses fur- tance from PEI were used for computing an index of dis-
nished in the time period of the Anne series; guided tours are tance. We used kilometers from the center of each origin to
offered to visitors of the grounds and houses. The Anne the center of PEI and employed five distance zones:
musical is the primary performing arts attraction offered
each year in Charlottetown. While not all discretionary trips 1 = Canadian Atlantic maritime provinces
to PEI relate to the Anne book series, many Canadians, 2 = Quebec and Ontario provinces and U.S. New England
Americans, and Europeans are also motivated to visit the states
province because of their childhood reading of Anne of 3 = U.S. Mid-Atlantic states
Green Gables (Marketing Agency 1993). Consequently, 4 = Other U.S. states (mostly Midwest states)
travel party composition factors often influence behavior: 5 = European countries
search and use of information, perceptions and evaluations of
specific destination places and events, and the overall evalu- For purposes of data analysis, binary coding was used for
ations of destinations. The destination experiences of walk- first visit (FV = 1) versus prior visits (PV = 0) and for each
ing the paths, touring houses mentioned in the Anne books prime motive for the trip that included visiting PEI (e.g., fam-
series, and attending the Charlottetown (capital of PEI) sum- ily visit = 1 if prime visit; 0 for family visit if other prime mo-
mer festival of the musical Anne of Green Gables are the tive ticked). For use of the VIG, two variables were esti-
activities completed by Japanese visitors. Their evaluations mated: reporting receiving the VIG before the trip (coded 1 if
of their experiences are very positive. yes, 0 if no) and level of use of the VIG (0 = none, 1 = light,
2 = heavy).
Limitations Note in Figure 2 that as predicted in the first set of propo-
sitions (proposition 1), the travel party characteristic (i.e.,
The findings below describe empirical associations only. distance) strongly relates to first versus prior visits (r = .58,
Cause-and-effect relationships are not tested scientifically in p < .001). This finding reflects the useful conclusion for tour-
the study; formal treatment and test groups and treatments ism marketing strategy that two distinct visitor segments
were not included in the design of the study. The study is lim- occur for PEI—the first-time distant traveler and the local
ited by the use of secondary data that were not collected by repeat-visit traveler. Distance and receiving the VIG are
the primary researchers with the analysis presented in mind. related positively (r = .11, p < .01); distant travelers tend to
However, the primary researchers (PEI’s Marketing do their homework more so than nearby travelers.
Agency) did plan on collecting detailed data on all aspects of Figure 2 shows very strong relationships of distance with
leisure travel behavior involving visiting PEI. prime motives for visiting PEI. The distant travelers come to

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at Bond University Library on September 16, 2014


126 NOVEMBER 2002

FIGURE 2
DISTANCE, PRIOR VISITS, AND EVALUATION

Distance to Destination for the Traveler First versus Prior Visits

-29 29
58
Distance
-18 First Visit

11 -19
39 21 26
19
-10

42

-40 -21
-09 -15

Use of VIG Prime Motive for Trip


25 Family Vacation
13
12 Relax and Escape
Experience a Different Culture
Received VIG
18 16 17 Entertainment
-21
Visit Friends and Family
Level of VIG Use
-08 Sports and Social Activities
-20 Home Away from Home
Experience Nature
10

Note: Pearson product-moment correlations shown with decimal point omitted; for all r s, p < .001. Thick lines are used to empha-
size several strong associations. VIG = visitor information guides.

PEI for two prime motives: experience a different culture and to the other motives in Figure 2. Thus, tourism consumption
experience nature. Nearby originating travelers come to PEI systems analysis provides valuable insights into how
for two different prime motives: visit friends and family and motives connect visitors segmented by distance with destina-
for sports and social activities. Such findings indicate the tion behaviors (e.g., see Figure 3). Proposition 4 is sup-
value of using two distinct strategies to position the destina- ported: prime motives for the trip to the destination influence
tion in the minds of prospective visitors—depending on their travelers’ destination behaviors.
geographic distance. By analogy, “You have a friend in To increase clarity, Figure 3 includes only two of the pos-
Pennsylvania” may be a useful positioning message for only sible eight prime motives (see Figure 1 for the eight prime
one segment of the nearby visitors to this state; such a mes- motives). Figure 3 shows how these two prime motives are
sage is likely to be less effective for most visitors from dis- linked with travel behaviors in the destination region as well
tant origins. as the number of previous visits and other phase 1 variables.
Figure 2 includes a significant negative relationship Several findings in Figure 3 warrant discussion. First,
between use of the VIG and visiting friends and relatives (r = consider the negative relationship between number of previ-
–.20, p < .001). Given the substantial positive impacts of ous visits and use of advertising information (r = –.37, p <
using versus not using the VIG on increasing expenditures, .001). Conclusion: more frequent visitors tend to use adver-
the image of PEI, and satisfaction with the visit even among tising information infrequently compared to first-time and
nearby visitors, some marketing effort may be worthwhile in less frequent visitors. While frequency of visits is not associ-
attempting to eliminate this negative relationship. “Visit PEI ated with destination expenditures, use of advertising infor-
again for the very first time—New Experiences Guaranteed mation is strongly associated with destination expenditures
by Reading This Guide” may aid in increasing VIG reader- (r = .23, p < .001).
ship among nearby Atlantic maritime province visitors. For destinations having two primary markets by fre-
Proposition 3 receives strong support: use of VIG infor- quency of visits (i.e., first-time visitors represent 57% and
mation is associated with (h) prime motives for the trip to the repeat visitors 43% of PEI visitors), increasing the perceived
destination, (i) visit behaviors in the destination area, and (j) usefulness of information such as VIGs to repeat visitors is
evaluations of destination places and events. likely to increase the total expenditures in the destination
Additional findings are worth noting in Figure 2. As region. One exception to this proposition is gift-buying
prime motives, “family vacation” and “relax and escape” behavior. Use of advertising information is associated nega-
connect substantially less with key travel variables compared tively with gift-buying behavior (r = –.13).

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at Bond University Library on September 16, 2014


JOURNAL OF TRAVEL RESEARCH 127

FIGURE 3
EMPIRICAL RESULTS FOR PHASE 1

Distance to
Destination Prime 12
for the Traveler Motive
35
4 = very far for Trip to
3 = far Destination
2 = mid Learn about - 08
1 = near - 11 a different
35 culture Traveler Behaviors
in Destination Areas
- 53 - 05
• Length of stay
Number of • Accommodations used
- 35 14 • Destination activities
Previous Visits
• Destination areas visited
15
25 05 • Destination expenditures
14 • Gift-buying behavior 23
- 37 Prime 24
Motive - 16
for Trip to
Destination - 08
Use of Advertising
Relax and - 13
Information
09 escape from
2 = heavy
1 = light daily concerns
0 = None

The dominant relationship concerning gift-buying behav- and middle distance visitors. The i distance index includes
ior shown in Figure 3 is the positive connection with distance the following ordinal assignments:
to the destination (r = .24, p < .001). Similarly, a positive link
occurs between gift buying and learning about a different 1 = respondents living in the Canadian maritime prov-
culture as the prime motive for visiting PEI. inces (n = 595)
2 = other Canadian respondents and Americans living in
the New England states (n = 906)
Phase 2 Findings 3 = Americans living beyond the New England states and
Two issues of particular interest are related to distant and Europeans (n = 544)
4 = Japanese (n = 150)
mainly first-time visitors versus nearby and mainly repeat
visitors to PEI: Nearly all correlations are significant statistically. The
activities that visitors from distant homes are most prone to
• Do these two market segments engage in different be- do include the following:
haviors during their visits?
• Do these two market segments evaluate their experi- • Visit museums and historical or cultural sites
ences differently? • Attend lobster suppers
• Sightseeing
The answers from the data analysis present surprising
findings. The two customer segments do often participate in The activities that visitors from nearby origins are most
different activities. The more distant and mostly first-visit prone to do include the following:
customers consistently evaluate the activities less positively
compared to nearby customers. The lower evaluations of • Golfing
specific activities done in PEI imply negative distance and • Enjoying nightlife
conative (e.g., willingness to recommend and definitely visit • Water sports
again) linkages. Consequently, the findings provide strong
support for proposition 2. Note in Table 2 the substantial drops in being “very
Table 1 includes the correlation of participating in each pleased” with the activity engaged in for visitors from nearby
activity with a distance index. The revised distance index in- versus more distant origins. The majority of participants in
cludes Japanese respondents to compare these visitors by ac- most activities were very pleased only for the nearby cus-
tivities and postexperience evaluations with those of nearby tomer segment. Possibly, visitors from more distant origins

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at Bond University Library on September 16, 2014


128 NOVEMBER 2002

TABLE 1 TABLE 2
CORRELATIONS OF VISITORS’ DISTANCE INDEX SHARE OF RESPONDENTS “VERY PLEASED”
AND ACTIVITIES DURING VISIT IN WITH THE ACTIVITY FOR EACH OF THE VISITOR
PRINCE EDWARD ISLAND DISTANCE SEGMENTS (IN PERCENTAGES)

Activity Correlation p< Each Distance Segment


Going to the beach .08 .000 Activity 1 2 3 4
Water sports (swimming, windsurfing, Going to the beach 54 47 42 4
boating) –.14 .000 Water sports (swimming,
Golfing –.16 .000 windsurfing, boating) 55 45 14 0
Other active sports –.09 .000 Golfing 60 47 35 1
Spectator sports –.11 .000 Other active sports 54 35 28 0
Harbor cruise or land tour .11 .000 Spectator sports 74 31 29 0
Sightseeing within cities/towns Harbor cruise or land tour 45 40 48 0
(on your own) .22 .000 Sightseeing within
Sightseeing outside cities or towns cities/towns (on your own) 53 43 54 3
(on your own) .18 .000 Sightseeing outside cities or
Antique/handcraft shopping .05 .011 towns (on your own) 56 50 61 45
Shopping in general –.01 .541 Antique/handcraft shopping 45 34 34 3
Visiting museums and historical or Shopping in general 31 24 25 0
cultural sites .31 .000 Visiting museums and historical
Attending live theater .22 .000 or cultural sites 52 47 49 3
Attending lobster suppers .26 .000 Attending live theater 85 62 66 54
Eating local cuisine –.03 –.120 Attending lobster suppers 70 56 61 9
Enjoying nightlife –.16 .000 Eating local cuisine 49 36 38 0
Note: The distance index includes 1 = respondents living in Enjoying nightlife 49 41 46 0
the Canadian maritime provinces; 2 = other Canadians and
U.S. New England states respondents; 3 = other U.S. resi- Note: Sample sizes vary substantially by activities. For exam-
dents and Europeans; 4 = Japanese. ple, total responses evaluating the beaches were 1,322, but
only 131 watched spectator sports. Respondents are in-
cluded only if they participated in the activity and evaluated
the activity. See Tables 3 and 4 for an example.

Figure 5 summarizes key linkages among activities done


have more experiences in these activities at a greater number by visitors in PEI. For example, length of stay (i.e., number
of alternative destinations versus PEI, and comparisons to of nights in PEI) is associated positively with staying in bed
such alternatives result in PEI ratings below being very pleased. and breakfast accommodations but is unrelated to staying in
Tables 3 and 4 show examples of more detailed informa- hotels. Visiting Charlottetown is not included in Figure 5
tion than is summarized in Table 2. A striking finding in because nearly all travelers visit this city (the capital of the
Table 4 is that while 85% of the Japanese visitors reported
PEI) during their visit. Thus, visits to Charlottetown are not
attending PEI lobster suppers, only 9% were “very pleased.”
influenced and do not influence doing the activities included
Given that the overall majority of visitors do not report
in Figure 5.
being very pleased with the PEI beaches, going to PEI
As shown in Figure 5, New London and Kensington are
beaches is less of a key driver than other PEI activities (see
Table 3). However, those visitors who strongly agree that found to be activity hub cities. Note that this observation
“PEI’s beaches are superior” are significantly more likely to applies in particular to New London.
“definitely visit PEI again” (see additional findings below).
Thus, making sweeping conclusions regarding visitors’ eval- Phase 3 Findings
uations of their destination activities may be misleading; for
example, visitor experiences on PEI beaches are a positive What evaluations by visitors are the positive and negative
driver for nearby customers but not a key driver for visitors principal drivers to visiting PEI and being willing to recom-
from more distant origins. mend PEI to friends? Figure 6 summarizes the findings that
Through the 1990s, PEI’s general marketing strategy help answer this question.
included the message, “Warmest beaches north of the Caroli- Sightseeing in the cities and visiting PEI museums are
nas.” However, based on the findings in Tables 1 and 2, associated strongly with both conations in Figure 6. Antique
emphasizing beach activities to visitors from distant origins and craft shopping and the local PEI cuisine are the second-
may be counterproductive. ary positive drivers linked to the two primary activity drivers
Figure 4 reflects the negative relationships between dis- as well as directly with visiting PEI again. Evaluating the
tance to origins and visitors’ evaluations of PEI activities, as beaches is not associated directly with the two conations (see
well as the positive relationships with doing specific activi- Figure 6 for additional details).
ties on PEI with overall evaluations with respect to PEI. Note that as might be expected, global PEI evaluations
Note in Figure 4 that none of the variables shown was regarding PEI are associated most strongly with visiting PEI
found to be associated with perceiving PEI as an expensive again and with willingness to recommend PEI to friends. The
destination. Visits to PEI museums were related negatively combined factor that includes having no nightlife and being
to evaluating PEI as having no nightlife and being boring and boring and old-fashioned is the largest negative driver
old-fashioned. toward recommending PEI to friends (r = –.42, p < .001). The

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at Bond University Library on September 16, 2014


JOURNAL OF TRAVEL RESEARCH 129

TABLE 3
DETAILED EXAMPLE OF EVALUATIONS BY SEGMENTS: GOING TO THE BEACH

Postexperience Evaluation (%)


Very Very
Distance Segment Disappointed Disappointed Pleased Pleased Number
1 = Canadian maritime 0 2 44 54 283
2 = Other Canada and U.S. New England 0 1 52 47 627
3 = Other U.S. states and Europe 0 3 55 42 314
4 = Japan 1 9 86 4 98
Total 0 2 53 44 1,322
Note: Chi-square = 94.75, df = 9, p < .000; shares of each distance segment reporting going to the beach: segment 1 = 48% (i.e.,
283 of 595), segment 2 = 69%, segment 3 = 58%, and segment 4 = 65%.

TABLE 4
DETAILED EXAMPLE OF EVALUATIONS BY SEGMENTS: LOBSTER SUPPERS

Postexperience Evaluation (%)


Very Very
Distance Segment Disappointed Disappointed Pleased Pleased Number
1 = Canadian maritime 1 1 28 70 140
2 = Other Canada and U.S. New England 1 1 42 56 432
3 = Other U.S. states and Europe 1 2 37 61 243
4 = Japan 0 5 87 9 127
Note: Chi-square = 134.15, df = 9, p < .0000. Participation in lobster suppers: segment 1 = 23%, segment 2 = 48%, segment 3 =
45%, and segment 4 = 85%.
FIGURE 4
DISTANCE, PRIOR VISITS, AND EVALUATIONS

Distance to Destination for the Traveler 58


First versus Prior Visits

Distance
First Visit

-21

25

Overall Evaluations of PEI as a Destination


Evaluation of Specific Destinations, Places, and Events
Liked beaches 23
PEI is expensive
14 Inconvenient
-14 to reach
-19 Antique/craft shopping Hospitality
-14 staff well
trained
Shopping 19
-20

Local -21
-18 cuisine
Sightseeing 17 Beaches are
in city No nightlife, superior
23 Friendly &
-21 boring, old-
relaxing fashioned
Museums -27
-26

Note: PEI = Prince Edward Island.

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at Bond University Library on September 16, 2014


130 NOVEMBER 2002

FIGURE 5
KEY RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN ACTIVITIES

Length of Stay 25
B&B Summerside 44
-31
28 South Shore 25
Cabin 20 21
57
-36 New London
Hotel
28
Kensington
31
24
Went to Beach 22
26
23 Brack
20
28 23
25
Went Shopping
Gift Buying
23 22
26
22 22
20
Went Antique Shopping 20 54

26 Expenditures

20 Went to Museums
26
Sampled Local Cuisine
Went Sightseeing in City

Note: Pearson product-moment correlations shown with decimal point omitted; for all r s, p < .001. Thick lines used to emphasize
several strong associations.

overall image that PEI is friendly and relaxing is the largest way. Doing or not doing certain activities affects how the dis-
positive image driver for recommending PEI to friends (r = tance of visitors’ origins relates to their conations concerning
.45, p < .001). The findings support proposition 5: visitor the destination (cf. Etzel and Woodside 1982).
behaviors in the destination areas influence their evaluations
with respect to the destination. Proposition 6 is supported:
visitor evaluations of their experiences in the destination area CONCLUSIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS
influence their overall evaluations with respect to the desti- FOR TOURISM STRATEGIES
nation, and postexperience evaluations influence conations.
Figure 6 shows detailed results supporting proposition 6.
Notice the strong secondary influence shown in Figure 6 Viewing leisure travel behavior as a consumption system
of PEI’s hospitality staff being perceived as well trained. may help deepen understanding of the streams of thoughts
Visitor evaluations of destination hospitality quality are and actions by travelers prior to, during, and after travel. For
found to have strong direct influences on both conative fac- specific destinations and industries (e.g., hospitality, car
tors in Figure 6. rental, and retail stores), learning the details that culminate
Proposition 7 is supported: multiple paths do occur in in, as well as block, visits and purchases with respect to their
traveler behaviors within PEI. Multiple ways occur for services may be useful.
increasing and decreasing conations. The complexity in the Viewing tourism behavior as consumption systems
quick clustering maps (shown in Figures 2-6) threatens to enables us to focus on understanding multiple decisions and
overwhelm the viewer with equivocality rather than uncer- actions rather than only the destination choice decision (cf.
tainty. Some paths, starting with distance and ending with the Moutinho 1987; Woodside and Lysonski 1989). Such a sys-
two conations in Figure 6, show net gains (computed by mul- tems perspective shows the complexities that unfold within
tiplying the directionality of arrows for a given path), and leisure travel. While not all visitors to a destination from a
some paths show a net loss, that is, reductions in conations. given origin are the same, most likely a dominant customer
Thus, the following question has multiple, rather than behavior segment needs to be identified for planning market-
one, useful answers: does distance from origin increase or ing strategies to attract visitors from each major origin. Anal-
decrease visitors’ intentions to return and favorable word-of- ysis of visitors by their tourism consumption systems pro-
mouth communications? The most accurate answer is that vides the marketing strategist with deep profiles of different
the impact of distance to the destination on visitor conations customer segments demographics, trip planning activities,
depends on the activities and evaluations that occur along the trip behaviors, evaluations of trip services experienced,

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at Bond University Library on September 16, 2014


JOURNAL OF TRAVEL RESEARCH 131

FIGURE 6
RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN EVALUATION OF SPECIFIC DESTINATIONS, PLACES, AND EVENTS
WITH OVERALL DESTINATION EVALUATIONS AND CONATIONS

Evaluation of Specific Destinations, Places, and Events Overall Evaluations of PEI as a Destination

25
19
Liked beaches 39
18

Antique/craft shopping
47 23
17 PEI is expensive
63 54 Inconvenient
22
Shopping 19 to reach
Hospitality
29 28 staff well
20 17
Local trained
49 -21 -24 -27
cuisine
Sightseeing 18 -31
in city 22
23
57
31 Beaches are
-34
27 superior
43 Museums 23 No nightlife,
Friendly & boring, old-
18 27 .28 relaxing fashioned

-42 27
29 29
27 45
39
-39

Definitely Visit Again 55 Willing to Recommend Others


27

Conations

Note: Pearson product-moment correlations shown with decimal point omitted; for all r s, p < .001. Thick lines used to emphasize
several strong associations. PEI = Price Edward Island.

global evaluations of services and destinations, and tourism consumption systems. Interestingly enough, if we
conations—and how these variables are linked together. identify the tourism segment as the unit of analysis we wish
Consequently, the product and communication strategies to study in order to select the appropriate strategies implied
for destination marketers and marketers of tourism services above, Finn and Kayande (1997) suggested that we can sub-
can focus on (1) the trip activities actually done by each seg- stantially reduce the costs of data collection by being more
ment and (2) the experiences they evaluate very positively. efficient in how we design our studies. This implies that we
For example, many PEI visitors from the Canadian maritime need not survey 2,000 respondents face-to-face to collect the
provinces actually participate in water sports and evaluate data we need; instead, we can survey fewer people with a more
these experiences very positively. While communicating the focused instrument and achieve the same degree of accuracy.
activities and benefits of water sports to these PEI visitors is
likely to be an effective strategy, PEI attractions and activi-
ties other than water sports are more appealing to PEI’s other REFERENCES
customer segments. Promoting PEI water sports to Ameri-
cans from the midwestern and southern states may actually Arnould, E. J., and L. L. Price (1993). “ ‘River Magic’: Hedonic Consump-
tion and the Extended Service Encounter.” Journal of Consumer Re-
hurt rather than help build a favorable image toward the search, 20: 24-35.
destination. Baloglu, Seyhmus (2000). “A Path-Analytical Model of Visitation Intention
A regional destination, such as PEI, cannot afford to have Involving Information Sources, Socio-Psychological Motivations and
Destination Images.” In Consumer Psychology of Tourism, Hospitality
one positioning strategy for all relevant market segments. and Leisure, edited by A. G. Woodside, G. I. Crouch, J. A. Mazanec,
However, promoting a regional destination as “having it all” M. Oppermann, and M. Y. Sakai. Cambridge, UK: CABI Publishing.
Bass, Frank M., Douglas J. Tigert, and Robert T. Lonsdale (1968). “Market
needs to be avoided—this shopworn message does not match Segmentation: Group versus Individual Behavior.” Journal of Mar-
the benefits sought or experienced by visitors (see Woodside keting Research, 5 (August): 268-72.
and Jacobs 1985). Bello, Dan, and Michael Etzel (1985). “The Role of Novelty in the Pleasure
Travel Experience.” Journal of Travel Research, 24 (1): 20-26.
Creating a limited number of product-market strategies is Clawson, Marion, and Jack L. Knetsch (1966). Economics of Outdoor Rec-
recommended. Designing unique tourism products and expe- reation. Washington, DC: Resources for the Future Series, Johns
Hopkins University Press.
riences that can be delivered for each of three to six market Cole, Sally (1993). “Japanese Make Pilgrimage to PEI to Celebrate the
segments builds well onto what is known about alternative Amazing ‘Anne’ Link.” The Guardian, July 31: 2.

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at Bond University Library on September 16, 2014


132 NOVEMBER 2002

Etzel, Michael J., and Arch G. Woodside (1982). “Segmenting Vacation  (1983). “Attitude-Behavior Consistency: The Impact of Product
Markets: The Case of the Distant and Near-Home Travelers.” Journal Trial versus Advertising.” Journal of Marketing Research, 20: 257-67.
of Travel Research, 20 (Spring): 10-14. Snepenger, D. K., K. Meged, M. Snelling, and K. Worrall (1990). “Informa-
Fazio, Russell H., and Mark P. Zanna (1987). “Attitudinal Qualities Relating tion Search Strategies by Destination-Naïve Tourists.” Journal of
to the Strength of the Attitude-Behavior Relationship.” Journal of Ex- Travel Research, 29 (Summer): 13-16.
perimental Social Psychology, 14: 398-408. Solomon, Michael R. (1988). “Mapping Product Constellations: A Social
Finn, Adam, and Ujwal Kayande (1997). “Reliability Assessment and Opti- Categorization Approach to Symbolic Consumption.” Psychology &
mization of Marketing Measurement.” Journal of Marketing Re- Marketing, 5 (3): 233-58.
search, 34 (2): 262-75.  (1999). Consumer Behavior. 4th ed. Upper Saddle, NJ: Prentice
Fodness, Dale, and Brian Murray (1998). “A Typology of Tourist Informa- Hall.
tion Search Strategies.” Journal of Travel Research, 37 (November): Weick, Karl E. (1995). Sensemaking in Organizations. Thousand Oaks, CA:
108-19. Sage.
 (1999). “A Model of Tourist Information Search Behavior.” Journal Woodside, Arch G. (1982). “Positioning a Province Using Travel Re-
of Travel Research, 37 (February): 220-30. search.” Journal of Travel Research, 20: 2-6.
Gladwell, M. (1996). “The Tipping Point.” The New Yorker, 72: 32-39.  (2000). “Introduction: Theory and Research on the Consumer Psy-
Howard, John A., and Jagdish N. Sheth (1969). The Theory of Buyer Behav- chology of Tourism, Hospitality and Leisure.” In Consumer Psychol-
ior. New York: John Wiley. ogy of Tourism, Hospitality and Leisure, edited by A. G. Woodside,
Kamen, Joseph (1970). “Quick Clustering.” Journal of Marketing Research, G. I. Crouch, J. A. Mazanec, M. Oppermann, and M. Y. Sakai.
7 (2): 199-204. Wallingford, UK: CABI Publishing, pp. 1-17.
Kim, Soyoung, and Mary A. Littrell (1999). “Predicting Souvenir Purchase Woodside, Arch G., and Chris Dubelaar (1999). “Meta-Analysis of Adver-
Intentions.” Journal of Travel Research, 38 (4): 153-62. tising Conversion Research Findings: Testing the Self-Generated Va-
Marketing Agency (1993). Summary Profile of PEI Visitors. Charlottetown, lidity Hypothesis Regarding Sponsor Identity.” In Marketing in the
Prince Edward Island: Marketing Agency. Third Millennium: Proceedings—ANZMAC99 [CD]. Sydney: School
McClelland, David C. (1998). “Identifying Competencies with Behavioral- of Marketing, University of New South Wales.
Event Interviews.” Psychological Science, 9 (5): 331-39. Woodside, Arch G., and Laurence W. Jacobs (1985). “Step Two in Benefit
McIntosh, R. W., and Charles R. Goeldner (1990). Tourism: Principles, Segmentation: Learning the Benefits Realized by Major Travel Mar-
Practices, Philosophies. New York: John Wiley. kets.” Journal of Travel Research, 23 (Fall): 14-24.
Moutinho, Luiz (1987). “Consumer Behavior in Tourism.” European Jour- Woodside, Arch G., and Steven Lysonski (1989). “A General Model of
nal of Marketing, 21 (10): 2-44. Traveler Destination Choice.” Journal of Travel Research, 27 (4): 8-14.
Muraoka, Hanako (1952). Akage No Anne [Red-haired Anne]. Tokyo: Woodside, Arch G., and Roberta MacDonald (1994). “A General Systems
Mikasa-Shobou. Framework of Customer Choice Processes of Tourism Services.” In
Pedhazur, E. J. (1982). Multiple Regression in Behavioral Research. New Spoilt for Choice, edited by R. Gasser and K. Weiermair. Vienna: Kul-
York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. tur Verlag, pp. 30-59.
Peter, J. Paul, and Jerry C. Olson (1999). Consumer Behavior. New York: Woodside, Arch G., Randolph J. Trappey III, and Roberta MacDonald
McGraw-Hill. (1997). “Measuring Linkage-Advertising Effects on Customer Behav-
Senge, Peter (1990). The Fifth Discipline. New York: Doubleday. iour and Net Revenue: Using Quasi-Experiments of Advertising
Smith, Robert E., and William R. Swinyard (1982). “Information Response Treatments with Novice and Experienced Product-Service Users.”
Models: An Integrated Approach.” Journal of Marketing, 46: 81-93. Canadian Journal of Administrative Sciences, 14 (2): 214-28.

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at Bond University Library on September 16, 2014

You might also like