You are on page 1of 9

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Who Suffers Most From Being Involved in


Bullying—Bully, Victim, or Bully-Victim?
LAURALIISA MARK, PhDa AIRI VÄRNIK, MD, PhDb MERIKE SISASK, PhDc

ABSTRACT
BACKGROUND: Bullying has been associated with many mental health problems. The effect of bullying has been found to be
affected by the way students are involved in bullying. The purpose of the study was to explore the association between mental
well-being, hopelessness, and being involved in bullying (as a bully, victim, or bully-victim), and to detect more harmful bullying
types to students’ mental well-being.
METHODS: The study was conducted among Estonian students from grades 5 to 9 (N = 2048). The survey was nationwide,
cross-sectional, anonymous, and web-based by nature. Mental well-being was assessed by using WHO-5 well-being index
and hopelessness by 1-item Beck Hopelessness Scale.
RESULTS: The odds for being severely distressed (ie, students expressing both poor mental well-being and hopelessness) were
greater for both sexes bullying others and being victims of bullying. For bully-victims the odds were greater for girls only. The
victims of extensive cyber bullying expressed the lowest level of mental well-being for both sexes.
CONCLUSIONS: Boys might be the most vulnerable if being pure victims, while girls if being bully-victims. More attention
should be paid to cyber bullying, as victims of extensive cyber bullying had the lowest level of mental well-being.

Keywords: bullying; types of bullying; mental well-being; hopelessness; sex difference.


Citation: Mark L, Värnik A, Sisask M. Who suffers most from being involved in bullying—bully, victim, or bully-victim? J Sch
Health. 2019; 89: 136-144. DOI: 10.1111/josh.12720

Received on December 2, 2016


Accepted on March 25, 2018

A student can be involved in bullying as a victim,


as a bully or as a bully-victim. Previous studies
have tried to find out who suffers most from bullying.
It has also been found that internalized symp-
toms (such as depression, anxiety, fear, withdrawal
from social contacts) are more prevalent among
Several studies have shown greater risk for mental victims of bullying,9 while externalized symptoms
health problems among victims of bullying,1,2 whereby (such as anger, aggression, conduct problems, ten-
the worst effects of bullying involvement have been dency to engage in risky, impulsive, and crimi-
imputed to bully-victims.3-5 The effects of bullying on nal behaviors) are displayed more frequently by
pure bullies has been disputable as there is evidence bullies.10 For example, it has been found that
suggesting that bullies may be even healthier than bullies have heightened risk of developing antiso-
their peers,6 while some have found that bullies cial personality disorders in young adulthood, even
when controlling for preexisting psychiatric prob-
and victims are equally disturbed,7 and others have
lems, family hardships, and child maltreatment.11
communicated that the well-being of bullies can be
Thus, involvement in bullying reduces mental
severely compromised.5,8

a Adviser at Riigikogu, (lauraliisa.mark@gmail.com), Parliament of Estonia, Research Department of the Riigikogu, Lossi plats 1a Tallinn 15165 Estonia; PhD candidate at Tallinn
University, School of Governance, Law and Society, Narva mnt 25 Tallinn 10120 Estonia.
bManagement Board Member and Principal Investigator, (varnik.airi@gmail.com), Estonian-Swedish Mental Health and Suicidology Institute (ERSI), Õie 39 Tallinn 11615 Estonia;

Professor of Mental Health at Tallinn University, School of Governance, Law and Society, Narva mnt 25 Tallinn 10120 Estonia.
c Executive Director and Senior Researcher, (sisask.merike@gmail.com), Estonian-Swedish Mental Health and Suicidology Institute (ERSI), Õie 39 Tallinn 11615 Estonia; Professor of

Social Health Care at Tallinn University, School of Natural Sciences and Health, Narva mnt 25 Tallinn 10120 Estonia.
Address correspondence to: Lauraliisa Mark, Adviser at Riigikogu, (lauraliisa.mark@gmail.com), Parliament of Estonia, Research Department of the Riigikogu, Lossi plats 1a Tallinn
15165 Estonia; PhD Candidate at Tallinn University, School of Governance, Law and Society, Narva mnt 25, Tallinn 10120, Estonia.
This work was supported by the EU employment and social solidarity program PROGRESS (2007-2013) via the Law School of the Tallinn University of Technology under public
procurement number 156437 (Uuring kiusamine koolis; Bullying at school study). We thank participants and school personnel who were helpful in conducting the survey in the
classroom.

136 • Journal of School Health • February 2019, Vol. 89, No. 2 • © 2019, American School Health Association
well-being and may hinder from evolving to healthy Ethics and Consent Issues
adulthood. Participation in the study was voluntary. The con-
There are no simple and universal explanation of sent for participation was obtained from schools,
why do some adolescents bully others. For example, parents, and students. The primary consent for par-
bullying has been attributed to low self-control,12 ticipation was obtained from school managements.
poor impulsivity,13 psychological disturbances,7 psy- After obtaining the agreement from 58 schools, each
chiatric disorders,14,15 unfavorable family factors,16 school appointed the administrator for the survey.
and to attaining a social status goals.17 Thus, bul- Before the survey, an information letter with the pos-
lying others can be viewed as an attempt to man- sibility to refuse from filling in the questionnaire was
age one’s social problems. However, there is no sent to parents and students. Those who refused, did
adequate reason for being a target of bully.18 Usu- not participate in the survey (the respective data was
ally these children are simply different in some collected and stored by the administrator). In addi-
ways (shorter/taller, thinner/fatter, wears ‘‘funny’’ tion, the confirmation of agreement to participate was
clothes/glasses, talks or walks ‘‘funny’’), as if it would obtained directly before filling in the questionnaire (it
justify violent acts. was impossible to access the questionnaire without
According to theory of psychosocial development,19 the confirmation).
young person’s peer group is a major source of his/her
self-esteem. Thus, bullying may severely impair Instruments
young person’s self-esteem. Furthermore, when tra- In addition to sex of the students, the following
ditional bullying stops after crossing the doorsill of variables were selected for analysis.
one’s home, an era of technology has granted the Bullying. The meaning of bullying and bullying
possibility to bully even behind the closed home others was provided in the questionnaire along
doors. with the respective questions. It was emphasized
The present study investigates sex-specific bullying that bullying is an intended and repeated behavior,
experience among Estonian students from grades 5 to which is (probably) hurtful for the victim. It was also
9 in association with mental well-being and hopeless- emphasized that bullying is not friendly or playful
ness. The association between mental well-being and behavior, as far as the friendliness or playfulness of the
hopelessness and being involved in bullying is analyzed behavior was understood by the other party as well.
separately for bullies, victims, and bully-victims. The Students were asked if they have:
means of bullying reported by victims are under special 1) Been bullied during the last 12 months? (If yes,
attention with an intent to detect bullying types that the experienced means of bullying were ticked):
are more harmful to students’ mental well-being and I was:
hopelessness.
1. laughed at/ridiculed
2. mocked by homophobic words or movements
METHODS (somebody called you a ‘‘gay,’’ imitated same-sex
Procedure sexual intercourse etc)
This study analyzes the data from the Bullying 3. mocked for my look
at School (Kiusamine koolis) study, which was con- 4. mocked for not being enough masculine/feminine
ducted in Estonia in spring 2015. The survey was 5. mocked for my academic achievements/capacity
nationwide, cross-sectional, anonymous, and web- 6. mocked for socioeconomic reasons (rich, poor)
based (using Limesurvey online) by nature. Fill- 7. mocked for my family (ethnicity, looks, profession
ing in the online questionnaires took approximately etc)
10 minutes and were administered by school person- 8. mocked for some other reason
nel during school lessons. Each student had a unique 9. left out/ostracized from the peer group
ID code for Limesurvey, the names of the students 10. hit
filling in the questionnaire are not identifiable. 11. pushed
Sampling was conducted by focusing on attaining 12. threatened
a representative sample in terms of sex, different 13. forced to do things I did not want to do
classes (grades 5-9), regions of Estonia, school type Others:
(urban or rural), and teaching language (Estonian or
Russian). Relevant data for sampling procedure were 1. spread rumors about me
derived from the Estonian Statistics.20 The general list 2. took away my money or my personal belongings
of schools was created using the Estonian Education 3. sent me mean or hurtful messages/e-mails
Information System (EHIS),21 which was then a 4. posted mean or hurtful messages about me in social
subject to random sampling procedure, where a class network
was handled as a sampling unit. 5. made mean or hurtful phone calls to me

Journal of School Health • February 2019, Vol. 89, No. 2 • © 2019, American School Health Association • 137
2) Bullied (alone or with others) other student as independent variable with being uninvolved in
during the last 12 months? bullying as a reference category.
Based on ‘‘yes’’ and ‘‘no’’ answers to being bullied To examine the relationship between the means
or bullied others, a new variable ‘‘Involvement in of bullying and to regroup them based on shared
bullying’’ was created. Each student was categorized variance, an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was
into one of the following categories: (1) pure victim, conducted. The data was subjected to principal
(2) bully-victim, (3) pure bully, (4) uninvolved. components analysis using oblique Promax rota-
Mental well-being. Mental well-being was assessed tion, which does not assume lack of correlation
by WHO-5 well-being index, which is a short, between variables and which is often seen as pro-
simple, positively loaded, and well-known instrument ducing more accurate results for research involv-
for assessing mental well-being and symptoms relevant ing human behaviors, or when the data does not
to depression among population.22-25 The raw score meet priori assumption.28 Variables with communal-
ranges from 0 to 25, where ‘‘0’’ is representing the ities <0.3 (N = 4) and close to that value (N = 1),
worst possible and ‘‘25’’ the best possible subjective and factors with overlapping variance (N = 1) were
mental well-being.26 Score 13 is a critical cutoff point, discarded. Using an eigenvalue cut-off at 1.0 and find-
meaning that a score below that value indicates ings from the scree plot, 4 factors that explained
poor mental well-being. The WHO-5 well-being index 54.41% of the cumulative variance were retained.
consists of 5 statements (‘‘I have felt cheerful and in Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure (KMO = 0.73) indicated
good spirits,’’ ‘‘I have felt calm and relaxed,’’ ‘‘I have sufficient data for EFA and Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity
felt active and vigorous,’’ ‘‘I have felt fresh and rested,’’ (χ 2 = 587.70; p < .001) showed patterned relation-
‘‘My daily life has been filled with things that interest ships between the items. Each factor contained 3
me’’) and these statements are being assessed on a variables, which is the least recommended number
5-point scale from never to always. of variables in the factor.29 EFA was used to assem-
ble variables into descriptive categories only. There
Hopelessness. Hopelessness was measured by a one-
was no analyze conducted with computed factor
item Beck Hopelessness Scale (‘‘My future seems dark
scores.
to me’’). Previous studies have found that both, the
1-item and the full scale yield to reliable result.25,27
Mental well-being and hopelessness index. Based RESULTS
on ‘‘yes’’ and ‘‘no’’ answers to hopelessness toward Self-Reported Mental Well-Being and Hopelessness
future and calculated WHO-5 score (below 13 versus Toward the Future
equal to or above 13), the mental well-being From 2048 students studied, the overall mean score
and hopelessness index was created with categories: of WHO-5 was 16.7 (±4.75) with boys (17.1 ± 4.46)
(1) good (WHO-5 score equal to or above 13 and no having higher mean score than girls (16.4 ± 4.97)
hopelessness); (2) distressed (WHO-5 score below 13 (t = 3.54; p < .001). The proportion of students
or hopelessness); (3) severely distressed (WHO-5 score with poor mental well-being (WHO-5 score below
below 13 and hopelessness). 13) was 18.1% (N = 367). Poor mental well-being
was more common among girls (21.5%; N = 224)
compared to boys (14.4%; N = 143) (χ 2 = 16.98;
Data Analysis p < .001).
The data analysis was performed with SPSS 17. In The proportion of students reporting hopelessness
addition to descriptive statistics, χ 2 tests were used was 9.8% (N = 191) and there were no statistically
to compare and investigate the sex-specific differences significant differences between boys (10.1%; N = 93)
in mental well-being and hopelessness, experience and girls (9.6%; N = 98) (p > .05).
of bullying, and the association between experience of Based on ‘‘yes’’ and ‘‘no’’ answers to hopeless-
bullying and mental well-being. T-test and analysis of ness scale and calculated WHO-5 score (below 13
variance (ANOVA) was used to verify the differences versus equal to or above 13), 4.5% (N = 87) of
in the mean WHO-5 scores. students reported poor mental well-being with con-
Multinomial logistic regression analysis was current hopelessness (severely distressed); 18.7%
performed to investigate the association between (N = 360) reported poor mental well-being or
involvement in bullying and mental well-being and hopelessness (distressed), and 76.8% (N = 1480)
hopelessness index. The odds ratios (ORs) were expressed good mental well-being without hopeless-
calculated with a confidence interval (CI) at 95%. ness (Table 1). Being severely distressed was twice
The mental well-being and hopelessness index was as common among girls (5.9%) compared to boys
treated as a dependent variable using the category of (3.0%), while there was no significant difference
‘‘good mental well-being’’ (including hopefulness) as between boys (19.1%) and girls (18.2%) being
a reference category. The role in bullying was treated distressed.

138 • Journal of School Health • February 2019, Vol. 89, No. 2 • © 2019, American School Health Association
Table 1. Mental Well-Being and Hopelessness, and Involvement in Bullying by Sex

Boy Girl Total


N % N % N %
Mental well-being and hopelessness index
Severely distressed (poor well-being and hopelessness) 27 3.0 60 5.9 87 4.5
Distressed (poor well-being or hopelessness) 166 18.2 194 19.1 360 18.7
Good mental well-being without hopelessness 718 78.8 762 75.0 1480 76.8
Total 911 100.0 1016 100.0 1927 100.0
Missing 90 31 121
Involvement in bullying
Pure bully 75 7.7 30 2.9 105 5.2
Pure victim 119 12.1 160 15.3 279 13.8
Bully-victim 53 5.4 34 3.3 87 4.3
Uninvolved 733 74.8 818 78.5 1551 76.7
Total 980 100.0 1042 100.0 2022 100.0
Missing 21 5 26

Table 2. Mental Well-Being and Hopelessness Index in Association With Involvement in Bullying by Sex: Multinomial Logistic
Regression Analysis

Boy Girl
OR 95% CI OR 95% CI
Severely distressed (poor well-being and hopelessness) Pure bully 3.57 1.09-11.74 3.35 1.09-10.26
Pure victim 6.14 2.35-16.07 3.44 1.85-6.41
Bully-victim 3.63 0.76-17.33 4.85 1.71-13.76
Uninvolved
Distressed (poor well-being or hopelessness) Pure bully 0.69 0.34-1.44 0.84 0.29-2.49
Pure victim 1.59 0.96-2.63 2.40 1.61-3.57
Bully-victim 2.19 1.13-4.27 1.71 0.70-4.14
Uninvolved
The bold values indicate that independent variables (involvement in bullying) predict the outcome of dependent variable (poor well-being and/or hopelessness) at a statistically
significant level (*p < .05, **p < .01). The OR represents a relative measure of effect of being involved in bullying one one’s mental well-being and hopelessness (compared to
those who have not been involved in bulling).

Self-Reported Involvement in Bullying and Its Association (9.4%) and being hit (7.1%) were the most frequently
With Mental Well-Being and Hopelessness experienced means of bullying among boys. Being
Based on ‘‘yes’’ and ‘‘no’’ answers to being a laughed at (11.2%), being a subject for rumors (9.2%),
victim of bullying and bullying others, 5.2% (N = 105) and being mocked for the look (8.8%) were the most
of students reported being a pure bully, 13.8% frequently reported by girls.
(N = 279) reported being a pure victim, and 4.3% Most of the experienced means of bullying were
(N = 87) reported being a bully-victim (Table 1). Being associated with poorer mental well-being and/or hope-
a pure bully (7.7%) or bully-victim (5.4%) was more lessness for boys and girls (exact results in Table 3).
common among boys compared to girls (2.9% and Girls compared to boys had more statistically signifi-
3.3%, respectively). Girls (15.3%) compared to boys cant associations between a certain mean of bullying
(12.1%) reported more frequently being a pure victim. and poor mental well-being and/or hopelessness.
Multinomial logistic regression analysis indicated EFA modeled 4 factors (Table 4)—cyber bullying,
that the odds for being severely distressed was statisti- physical bullying, verbal bullying, and relational
cally significantly greater for boys and girls purely bul- bullying—which were the basis for creating respective
lying others (for boys OR = 3.57; for girls OR = 3.35) variables with 4 categories (experienced 2-3 different
and purely being victims of bullying (for boys means of bullying, experienced one mean of bullying,
OR = 6.14; for girls OR = 3.44). Only for girls the odds did not experience that bullying type; did not
for being severely distressed were greater (OR = 4.85) experience bullying at all). Then the mean WHO-5
if being bully-victims (Table 2). Odds for being dis- score was calculated for each category to investigate
tressed were greater for boys (OR = 2.19) being bully- the cumulative effect on mental well-being within a
victims and for girls (OR = 2.40) being pure victims. specific bullying type (Table 5). Boys being victims of
Victimized boys and girls reported somewhat cyber bullying in 2-3 different ways and verbal bullying
different experience in terms of the means of bullying had the lowest mean WHO-5 score. Being a victim
(Table 3). Being laughed at (12.2%), being pushed of extensive (2-3 different ways) cyber bullying and

Journal of School Health • February 2019, Vol. 89, No. 2 • © 2019, American School Health Association • 139
Table 3. Frequency of Experienced Means of Bullying by Sex

Male Female
Means of Bullying N % N %
I was laughed at/ridiculed 122 12.2 117* 11.2
I was pushed 94 9.4 42 4.0
I was hit 71 7.1 29* 2.8
I was left out/ostracized from the peer group 68 6.8 66* 6.3
I was mocked for my look 65 6.5 92 8.8
I was mocked for some other reason 59** 5.9 70 6.7
Others spread rumors about me 58 5.8 96** 9.2
I was mocked by homophobic words or movements 47** 4.7 36** 3.4
I was threatened 41 4.1 38 3.6
I was mocked for my academic achievements/capacity 40** 4.0 40 3.8
I was mocked for not being enough masculine/feminine 32* 3.2 18 1.7
I was mocked for socioeconomic reasons (rich, poor) 25 2.5 13* 1.2
I was forced to do things that I did not want to do 23 2.3 15* 1.4
Others took away my money or my personal belongings 18 1.8 18 1.7
Others sent me mean or hurtful messages/e-mails 17* 1.7 28 2.7
I was mocked for my family (ethnicity, looks, profession etc.) 16 1.6 15** 1.4
Others made mean or hurtful phone calls to me 9 0.9 12*,** 1.1
Others posted mean or hurtful messages about me in social network 8 0.8 14* 1.3
∗ The bold values indicate to means of bullying where there are statistically significant differences (p < .05) between those who have experienced the specified means of
bullying and those who have not experienced the specified means of bullying; the differences are assessed by WHO-5 well-being index (poor vs good).
∗∗ The bold values indicate to means of bullying where there are statistically significant differences (p < .05) between those who have experienced the specified means of
bullying and those who have not experienced the specified means of bullying; the differences are assessed by hopelessness (yes vs no).

Table 4. Factor Analysis of Means of Bullying (Significant Factor Criterion of 0.3)

Pattern Matrix Structure Matrix


(N = 385) 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4
I Cyber bullying (Cronbach α = 0.63)
Others sent me mean or hurtful messages/e-mails 0.778 0.765
Others posted mean or hurtful messages in social network 0.698 0.700
Others made mean or hurtful phone calls 0.683 0.725 0.334
II Physical bullying (and threats) (Cronbach α =0.66)
I was pushed 0.832 0.821
I was hit 0.827 0.839
I was threatened 0.361 0.585 0.404 0.610
III Verbal bullying (Cronbach α =0.43)
I was mocked for my academic achievements/capacity 0.712 0.693
I was mocked for not being enough masculine/feminine 0.664 0.676
I was mocked for my origin family (ethnicity, looks, profession, etc) 0.621 −0.316 0.396 0.656
IV Relational bullying (Cronbach α =0.39)
I was mocked for my look 0.655 0.662
I was laughed at/ridiculed 0.640 0.652
Others spread rumors about me 0.548 0.325 0.572
Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis. Rotation method: Promax with Kaiser Normalization. Rotation converged in 5 iterations. Coefficients with absolute value
below 0.30 are suppressed. The bold values represent variables assembled into factors with communalities > 0.5.

physical bullying yielded to the lowest mean WHO-5 these mental well-being parameters are associated
score among girls. with bullying experiences. The association between
mental well-being parameters and involvement in bul-
DISCUSSION lying was assessed from different perspectives (role
in bullying, sex, bullying type). In addition, the
The present study examined mental well-being,
hopelessness, and bullying experiences among Esto- authors attempted to answer a question, which bul-
nian students from grades 5 to 9. Mental well- lying types are more harmful on individual’s mental
being was assessed by the WHO-5 well-being index well-being.
and hopelessness by the 1-item Beck Hopelessness The study supports previous outcome that females
Scale, which has proven to yield a reliable result in pre- in general report several disadvantages of men-
vious studies.25,27 The study aimed to assess, whether tal health more frequently than males.30,31 The

140 • Journal of School Health • February 2019, Vol. 89, No. 2 • © 2019, American School Health Association
Table 5. Being a Victim of Bullying and Mean WHO-5 Score by Type of Bullying

Boy Girl
Mean WHO-5 score N % Mean WHO-5 score N %
Cyber bullying
2-3 different ways 14.43 (±5.71) 7 0.7 13.08 (±7.73) 12 1.2
1 way 15.78 (±5.01) 18 1.8 14.33 (±4.93) 24 2.3
No cyber bullying 15.84 (±4.85) 160 16.2 14.62 (±5.25) 160 15.3
No bullying experience 17.42 (±4.30) 805 81.3 16.80 (±4.78) 847 81.2
F= 7.09; p< .001 F= 12.22; p< .001
Physical bullying
2-3 different ways 16.00 (±4.94) 68 6.9 13.30 (±5.69) 27 2.6
1 way 15.07 (±4.61) 41 4.1 14.98 (±5.68) 44 4.2
No physical bullying 15.97 (±5.00) 76 7.7 14.58 (±5.19) 125 12.0
No bullying experience 17.42 (±4.30) 805 81.3 16.80 (±4.78) 847 81.2
F= 7.32; p< .001 F= 12.55; p< .001
Verbal bullying
2-3 different ways 14.80 (±5.65) 35 3.6 13.94 (±4.79) 47 4.5
1 way 14.17 (±6.66) 18 1.8 9.20 (±10.18) 5 0.5
No verbal bullying 16.27 (±4.31) 132 13.3 14.85 (±5.27) 144 13.8
No bullying experience 17.42 (±4.30) 805 81.3 16.80 (±4.78) 847 81.2
F= 8.81; p< .001 F= 14.34; p< .001
Relational bullying
2-3 different ways 15.03 (±5.25) 80 8.1 13.60 (±5.74) 105 10.1
1 way 15.90 (±4.72) 59 6.0 15.79 (±5.03) 58 5.6
No relational bullying 16.96 (±4.22) 46 4.6 15.03 (±4.18) 33 3.1
No bullying experience 17.42 (±4.30) 805 81.3 16.80 (±4.78) 847 81.2
F= 8.78; p< .001 F= 14.60; p< .001

proportion of boys and girls with poor mental well- result was statistically insignificant. Being distressed
being (WHO-5 score below 13) was 14% and 22%, (but not severely) had less clear associations with
respectively. However, there were no statistically bullying.
significant sex differences for hopelessness (10% for The devastating impact of bullying can be at least
both sexes). partly be explained by the need to feel accepted by
Hopelessness and poor mental well-being have been others, which is especially true during adolescence,35
consistently associated with depressive disorders.24,32 when individuals rely in developing self-esteem more
It has also been found that hopelessness maybe on social comparison and feedback from peers.19
even stronger predictor of adolescent suicidal ideation Furthermore, boys might be especially vulnerable to
than depression.33,34 Thus, hopelessness and poor being pure victims, as Western role for men expresses
subjective mental well-being are health risk factors, characteristics such as strength and endurance.36 Thus,
which may refer to a suicide potential, and therefore being a pure victim may impair boys’ perception of
should be taken seriously. themselves as a male. Likewise, as the traditional
In the present study, it was assumed that co- role for female embodies tolerance, mildness, and
occurrence of hopelessness and poor mental well- solicitude,36 being a victim and bully at the same
being indicates severely impaired mental state, and time may add extra stress for girls, as in addition to
was therefore treated as the status of being severely impaired feeling of acceptance, girls may experience
distressed (3% of boys and 6% of girls). The presence role confusion due to their anti-feminine partake in
of either hopelessness or poor mental well-being was relations with peers.
treated as the status of being distressed (18% of boys Most of the experienced means of bullying associ-
and 19% of girls). ated with poor mental well-being and/or hopelessness
The study results showed that being involved in for both sexes. Girls compared to boys had more sta-
bullying was primarily associated with being severely tistically significant associations between means of
distressed. Being a pure bully increased the odds for bullying and mental well-being/hopelessness, which
being severely distressed more than 3-fold for both might be explained by their greater interpersonal
sexes (in comparison with uninvolved group). Being a sensitivity and concern about peer evaluation.37,38
pure victim increased the respective odds more than 6 Furthermore, all types of bullying (cyber bullying,
times for boys and more than 3 times for girls. Being physical bullying, verbal bullying, and relational bully-
a bully-victim increased the odds for being severely ing) associated significantly with mean WHO-5 score:
distressed almost 5 times for girls, while for boys the the more extensive the experienced bullying type,

Journal of School Health • February 2019, Vol. 89, No. 2 • © 2019, American School Health Association • 141
the lower the mental well-being score. Being a vic- struggle with poor mental health as well. Moreover,
tim of extensive (2-3 different ways) cyber bullying as found in present study, the vulnerability to bullying
and physical bullying yielded to the lowest mean depends on the role being involved in bullying
WHO-5 score among girls; being a victim of cyber and varies between the sexes—boys had the highest
bullying or verbal bullying among boys. Cyber bul- odds for being severely distressed if being pure victims,
lying turned out to be the most devastating form of while girls had the highest odds if being bully-
bullying. The reason presumably lies in the character victims. This should be considered when monitoring
of cyber bullying—it can get you any time and in any the school climate, addressing within-school relations,
place; the perpetrators are often meaner than they and providing support services for the distressed
would be in face-to-face communication; the higher students. Thus, boys being a victim of bullying and girls
level of perceived anonymity encourages more people being bully-victims need special attention, although,
to post hurtful messages; hurtful messages often reach they might be one of the most difficult target groups
many people, but at the same time it is unknown who to approach (due to withdrawing into oneself, distrust,
exactly has seen the writing, photo, or video, which fear of reactions from others, etc). When possible,
in turn can make the victim more embarrassed and these students should be interviewed by psychologist
anxious. or other specialist with appropriate training.
We recognize that schools are not solely respon-
sible for solving bullying. However, schools have
Limitations
considerable potential to reduce persecution in
The study has several limitations. First, the estimates
school environment—especially when implement-
were obtained by self-reports, which might cause
ing a whole-school approach, introducing partnership
some under- or overestimations as well as recall
with homes,39 shaping attitudes against violence, and
and interpretation problems. Second, the causal
contributing to raising students’ self-esteem and self-
relationship is unclear, as the study was cross-sectional
efficacy.
by nature. Third, only being victimized was analyzed
It is crucial that bullying behaviors are not
more in details, as bullying others was beyond
overlooked or ignored, but unequivocally condemned.
the volume of this paper. Further research is needed
Peers especially are able to discourage bullying
to explore the means used to bully others in association
behaviors (when condemning it), while at the same
with mental well-being.
time, to encourage it (when, eg, laughing with the
perpetrator). It is worth bearing in mind that at the
Conclusions time school personnel becomes aware of the bullying,
Although the authors recognize that mental well- it might have been lasted for a relatively long time
being of schoolchildren is mediated by numerous already. The more systematic the bullying has been,
factors, not solely by bullying experience, it is the more detrimental are its’ effect.40 Therefore, it
obvious that bullying is associated with poorer is important to develop a nonviolent attitude—so
mental well-being and including hopelessness (with that the bystanders would not encourage bullying
being severely distressed). However, the damage unsuspectingly by tolerating bully’s attempts to raise
associated with bullying is affected by the role being his or her social status. It is worth to implement
involved in bullying. Elevated vulnerability of victims some existing anti-bullying program that have showed
among boys and bully-victims among girls should its efficacy in the area concerned already—the
be considered when providing assistance and care concrete programs with promising results may vary
to students involved in bullying. As assumed, the more by country/region, on account of which, the authors
extensive was the bullying, the lower was the reported do not point out any specific program.
mean WHO-5 score. Furthermore, being a victim According to findings of present study, girls had
of extensive cyber bullying associated with the lowest more statistically significant associations between
mean WHO-5 score for both, boys and girls. It is crucial mental health variables (hopelessness and poor mental
that schools would invest in consistent anti-bullying well-being) and experience of bullying. However, the
programs with proven effectiveness. Likewise, it is number of statistically significant associations between
important to talk about the dangers of social media, means of bullying and hopelessness was similar among
online safety and about the opportunities to help boys and girls. Thus, although girls seemed to be more
oneself or others in case of becoming a victim of cyber vulnerable to bullying, the harm of bullying should
bullying. not be underestimated for boys. It is possible, that
(depending on an experienced means and frequency
of bullying) boys and girls suffering from bullying
IMPLICATIONS FOR SCHOOL HEALTH
simply experience somewhat different mental health
We feel it is important to stress that it is not just complaints. Furthermore, not all means of bullying
the victims who suffer, just the opposite, the bullies have the same effect—some are more harmful than

142 • Journal of School Health • February 2019, Vol. 89, No. 2 • © 2019, American School Health Association
others, especially if these impair students´ perception 14. Holmberg K, Hjern A. Bullying and attention-deficit-
of themselves as a male/female or knocks down their hyperactivity disorder in 10-year-olds in a Swedish community.
Dev Med Child Neurol. 2008;50(2):134-138.
self-worth. Schools should provide necessary support
15. Kumpulainen K, Räsänen E, Puura K. Psychiatric disorders and
services to ensure counseling (or referral) to those in the use of mental health services among adolescents involved
need in order to avoid further damage. in bullying. Aggress Behav. 2001;27(2):102-110.
Bullying does not only relate to decreased school 16. Bowes LM, Arseneault L, Maughan B, Taylor A, Caspi A,
engagement, academic achievement, or low mood. Moffit TE. School, neighborhood, and family factors are
Bullying may have tremendous impact on young associated with adolescents’ bullying involvement: a nationally
representative longitudinal study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
people lives—starting with serious mental health
Psychiatry. 2009;48(5):545-553.
disorders, self-esteem problems, and finishing with 17. Sijetsema JJ, Veenstra R, Lindenberg S, Salmivalli C. Empirical
self-harming, school shooting and suicide. It is vital test of bullies’ status goals: assessing direct goals, aggression,
to invest in proven methodologies aimed to reduce and prestige. Aggress Behav. 2009;35(1):57-67.
bullying. 18. University of New Hampshire. Why Do some Children Bully
Others? Bullies and their Victims - Family Development Fact
Sheet. University of New Hampshire cooperative extension.
Human Subjects Approval Statement Available at: http://blogs.cornell.edu/ccesuffolkfhw/files/2013/
Approval to conduct the survey was obtained from 07/NH-Why-Do-Some-Children-Bully-26s07gn.pdf. Accessed
national research ethic committee (Decision No 969, May 4, 2016.
12.03.2015). 19. McLeod S. Erik Erikson. Simply Psychology; 2017. Avail-
able at: http://www.simplypsychology.org/Erik-Erikson.html.
Accessed March 9, 2015.
REFERENCES 20. Statistics Estonia. Population Indicators and Composition
of Population. Available at: http://pub.stat.ee/px-web.2001/
1. Brunstein Klomek A, Sourander A, Gould M. The association
I_Databas/Population/databasetree.asp. Accessed October 6,
of suicide and bullying in childhood to young adulthood: a
2014.
review of cross-sectional and longitudinal research findings.
21. Eesti Hariduse Infosüsteem (EHIS; Info System of Estonian
Can J Psychiatr. 2010;55(5):282-288.
Education). A State Register of Estonian Schools and School
2. Reijntjes A, Kamphuis JH, Prinzie P, Telch MJ. Peer victimiza-
Network. Available at: http://www.ehis.ee/. Accessed October
tion and internalizing problems in children: a meta-analysis of
longitudinal studies. Child Abuse Negl. 2010;34(4):244-252. 6, 2014.
3. Hunter SC, Durkin K, Boyle JME, Booth JN, Rasmussen S. 22. Henkel V, Mergl R, Kohnen R, Maier W, Möller H-J,
Adolescent bullying and sleep difficulties. Eur J Psychol. Hegerl U. Identifying depression in primary care: a comparison
2014;10(4):740-755. of different methods in a prospective cohort study. BMJ.
4. Wolke D, Copeland WE, Angold A, Costello EJ. Impact of 2003;326(7382):200-201.
bullying in childhood on adult health, wealth, crime, and social 23. Henkel V, Mergl R, Coyne JC, Kohnen R, Möller H-J, Hegerl U.
outcomes. Psychol Sci. 2013;24(10):1958-1970. Screening for depression in primary care: will one or two items
5. Eisenberg ME, Gower AL, McMorris BJ, Bucchianeri MM. suffice? Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2004;254(4):215-223.
Vulnerable bullies: perpetration of peer harassment among 24. Topp CW, Østergaard SD, Søndergaard S, Bech P. The WHO-
youths across sexual orientation, weight, and disability status. 5 Well-Being Index: a systematic review of the literature.
Am J Public Health. 2015;105(9):1784-1791. Psychother Psychosom. 2015;84(3):167-176.
6. Wolke D, Woods S, Bloomfield L, Karstadt L. Bullying 25. Sisask M, Värnik A, Kõlves K, Konstabel K, Wasserman D. Sub-
involvement in primary school and common health problems. jective psychological well-being (WHO-5) in assessment of the
Arch Dis Child. 2001;85(3):197-201. https://doi.org/10.1136/adc severity of suicide attempt. Nord J Psychiatry. 2008;62(6):431-
.85.3.197. 435.
7. Kumpulainen K, Räsänen E, Henttonen I, et al. Bullying and 26. WHO-Five Well-Being Index. Psykiatri. Available at: https://
psychiatric symptoms among elementary school-age children. www.psykiatri-regionh.dk/who-5/Documents/WHO5_English
Child Abuse Negl. 1998;22(7):705-717. .pdf. Accessed June 3, 2014.
8. Mark L, Samm A, Tooding L-M, et al. Suicidal ideation, risk 27. Aish AM, Wasserman D, Renberg ES. Does Beck’s hopeless-
factors, and communication with parents: an HBSC study on ness scale really measure several components. Psychol Med.
school children in Estonia, Lithuania, and Luxembourg. Crisis.
2001;31(2):367-372.
2013;34(1):3-12.
28. Costello AB, Osborne JW. Best practices in exploratory factor
9. Olweus D, Limber SP. Bullying in school: evaluation and
analysis: four recommendations for getting the most from your
dissemination of the Olweus Bullying Prevention Program. Am
analysis. Pract Assess Res Eval. 2005;10(7):1-9.
J Orthopsychiatry. 2010;80(1):124-134.
29. Yong AG, Pearce S. A beginner’s guide to factor analysis:
10. Kumpulainen K, Räsänen E. Children involved in bullying at
elementary school age: their psychiatric symptoms and deviance focusing on exploratory factor analysis. Tutor Quant Methods
in adolescence—an epidemiological sample. Child Abuse Negl. Psychol. 2013;9(2):79-94.
2000;24(12):1567-1577. 30. Nolen-Hoeksema S. Sex Differences in Depression. Stanford, CA:
11. Copeland WE, Wolke D, Angold A, Costello EJ. Adult psychiatric Stanford University Press; 1990.
outcomes of bullying and being bullied by peers in childhood 31. Eaton WW, Dryman A, Weissman MM. Panic and phobia. In:
and adolescence. JAMA Psychiat. 2013;70(4):419-426. Robins LN, Regier DA, eds. Psychiatric Disorders in America: The
12. Unnever JD, Cornell DG. Bullying, self-control, and ADHD. Epidemiologic Catchment Area. Study. New York, NY: The Free
J Interpers Violence. 2003;18(2):129-147. Press; 1991:155-179.
13. Olweus D. Bullying at school: basic facts and effects of a 32. Young M, Fogg LF, Scheftner W, Fawcett J, Akiskal H, Maser J.
school based intervention program. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. Stable trait components of hopelessness: baseline and sensitivity
1994;35(7):1171-1190. to depression. J Abnorm Psychol. 1996;105(2):155-165.

Journal of School Health • February 2019, Vol. 89, No. 2 • © 2019, American School Health Association • 143
33. Beck AT, Steer RA, Kovacs M, Garrison B. Hopelessness and 37. Kuperminc GP, Blatt SJ, Leadbeater BJ. Relatedness, self-
eventual suicide: a ten-year prospective study of patients hos- definition, and early adolescent adjustment. Cogn Ther Res.
pitalized for suicidal ideation. Am J Psychiatry. 1985;142(5):559- 1997;21(3):301-320.
563. 38. Rudolph KD, Conley CS. The socioemotional costs and benefits
34. Swedo SE, Rettew DC, Kuppenheimer M, Lum D, of social evaluative concerns: do girls care too much? J Pers.
Dolan S, Goldberger E. Can adolescent attempters be dis- 2005;73(1):115-137.
tinguished from at-risk adolescents? Pediatrics. 1991;88(3): 39. Mann MJ, Kristjansson AL, Sigfusdottir ID, Smith ML. The
620-629. role of community, family, peer, and school factors in group
35. Coterell J. Social Networks and Social Influences in Adolescence. bullying: implications for school-based intervention. J Sch
London, UK: Routledge; 1996. Health. 2015;85(7):477-486.
36. Möller-Leimkühler AM. The gender gap in suicide and 40. Dombeck M. The long term effects of bullying. MentalHelp.net;
premature death or: why are men so vulnerable? Eur Arch 2016. Available at: https://www.mentalhelp.net/articles/the-
Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2003;253(1):1-8. long-term-effects-of-bullying/. Accessed November 22, 2016.

144 • Journal of School Health • February 2019, Vol. 89, No. 2 • © 2019, American School Health Association

You might also like