You are on page 1of 16

Arch Sex Behav (2008) 37:85–99

DOI 10.1007/s10508-007-9265-1

ORIGINAL PAPER: MINOT SPECIAL ISSUE

Sexual Orientation in Women with Classical or Non-classical


Congenital Adrenal Hyperplasia as a Function of Degree
of Prenatal Androgen Excess
Heino F. L. Meyer-Bahlburg Æ Curtis Dolezal Æ
Susan W. Baker Æ Maria I. New

Published online: 22 December 2007


Ó Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2007

Abstract 46,XX individuals with classical congenital adre- Bisexual/homosexual orientation was (modestly) correlated
nal hyperplasia (CAH) due to deficiency of the enzyme, with global measures of masculinization of non-sexual behav-
21-hydroxylase, show variable degrees of masculinization of ior and predicted independently by the degree of both prenatal
body and behavior due to excess adrenal androgen production. androgenization and masculinization of childhood behavior.
Increased bisexuality and homosexuality have also been We conclude that the findings support a sexual-differentiation
reported. This article provides a review of existing reports of the perspective involving prenatal androgens on the development
latter and presents a new study aimed at replicating the previous of sexual orientation.
findings with detailed assessments of sexual orientation on
relatively large samples, and at extending the investigation to Keywords Congenital adrenal hyperplasia 
the mildest form, non-classical (NC) CAH. Also, this is the first Sexual orientation  Homosexuality  Androgen effects 
study to relate sexual orientation to the specific molecular Disorders of sex development  Hermaphroditism
genotypes of CAH. In the present study, 40 salt-wasters (SW),
21 SV (simple-virilizing), 82 NC, and 24 non-CAH control
women (sisters and female cousins of CAH women) were Introduction
blindly administered the Sexual Behavior Assessment Sche-
dule (SEBAS-A, 1983 ed.; H. F. L. Meyer-Bahlburg & A. A. Sexual orientation is a trait with very large differences between
Ehrhardt, Privately printed). Most women were heterosexual, men and women. For instance, Hines (2004, p. 11) reported an
but the rates of bisexual and homosexual orientation were effect size d = 6.0, one of the largest for any gender-related
increased above controls not only in women with classical behavior or trait (although the distribution of sexual orientation
CAH, but also in NC women, and correlated with the degree of scores in terms of the widely used Kinsey scale raises some
prenatal androgenization. Classifying women by molecular doubts about the appropriateness of the usual statistical measure
genotypes did not further increase the correlation. Diverse of effect size, which assumes a Gaussian distribution). The
aspects of sexual orientation were highly intercorrelated, and demonstration of familiality and heritability of homosexuality
principal components analysis yielded one general factor. (e.g., Pillard & Bailey, 1998) has led to numerous attempts to
provide genetic explanations. Given the focus of evolution-
ary theory on reproduction and survival of the offspring, a
sexual orientation of women to men and of men to women is
eminently plausible. It is much more difficult to come up
H. F. L. Meyer-Bahlburg (&)  C. Dolezal with a compelling evolutionary raison d’être for homosex-
New York State Psychiatric Institute & Department of
Psychiatry, Columbia University, 1051 Riverside Drive,
uality and bisexuality. A number of non-endocrine explana-
NYSPI Unit 15, New York, NY 10032, USA tory hypotheses have been formulated. In the framework of
e-mail: meyerb@childpsych.columbia.edu evolutionary theory, overdominance (Miller, 2000), kin
altruism (Pillard & Bailey, 1998), and sexually antagonistic
S. W. Baker  M. I. New
Department of Pediatrics, Mount Sinai School of Medicine,
selection (Hamer & Copeland, 1994) have been suggested as
New York, NY, USA potential mechanisms explaining the gene polymorphism

123
86 Arch Sex Behav (2008) 37:85–99

that is presumed to underlie homosexuality, and mathemat- half of the 20th century, behavioral-endocrinology research
ical models have recently been formulated that should in non-primate mammals demonstrated the profound
facilitate their empirical testing (Gavrilets & Rice, 2006). ‘‘organizational’’ influence of sex hormones during early
However, the variability of homosexual behavior across developmental periods on later mating behavior and sexual
vertebrate species has not led to a consensus on an expla- orientation, with perinatal androgens and estrogens (derived
nation in terms of evolutionary theory (Sommer & Vasey, by aromatization of androgens within brain cells) supporting
2006), and the demonstration of learning mechanisms in the the development of masculine behavior, and estrogens
acquisition of sexual preferences in animal models (Pfaus, supporting the defeminization of behavior, followed by
Kippin, & Coria-Avila, 2003) has further complicated the ‘‘activating’’ effects of sex-specific hormones from puberty
issue. Identification of specific genes has not yet led to on (Arnold, 2002; Wallen & Baum, 2002). The analogous
consistent success (Hamer, Hu, Magnuson, Hu, & Pattatucci, processes in primates may be limited to androgen effects, but
1993; Mustanski et al., 2005; Rice, Anderson, Risch, & the determinants of sexual orientation in primates are not yet
Ebers, 1999), but new findings on extreme skewing of clear (Baum, 2006; Wallen & Baum, 2002). Early attempts to
X-inactivation by DNA-methylation in mothers of gay men identify sex hormone abnormalities in human homosexuality
have added additional genetic possibilities (Bocklandt, were unsuccessful in men (Meyer-Bahlburg, 1977) and only
Horvath, Vilain, & Hamer, 2006). partially successful in women (Meyer-Bahlburg, 1979). In
Bearman and Brückner (2002) provided a strong meth- the absence of systemic hormone abnormalities and of any
odological critique of much of the existing genetic literature signs of somatic intersexuality in human homosexuality,
and presented new data from the AddHealth project that some investigators have suggested that causal endocrine
(indirectly) supported a social-influence hypothesis explain- abnormalities might be limited to the central nervous system
ing same-sex attractions in adolescents. Other non-genetic (‘‘CNS-limited pseudohermaphroditism,’’ Dörner, 1976),
explanations include the progressive immunization hypoth- which is compatible with the recently growing evidence of
esis, which is derived from the well-replicated association of tissue specificity of hormone production and/or metabolism
homosexuality in males with the number of older brothers in and of hormone receptors. During the last two decades,
the sibship and assumes that successive pregnancies with advances in genetics have broadened the focus of research on
male fetuses leads in some mothers to the development of sexual differentiation to include the many genes involved in
male-specific antigens (Blanchard, 2004; Blanchard & the sexual differentiation of the gonads (Fleming & Vilain,
Lippa, 2007). The developmental instability theory explains 2005) and, possibly, of the brain (Arnold, 2002, 2004;
homosexuality as a perturbance of the complex processes of Gatewood et al., 2006). Although research on the specific
prenatal brain development by exogenous influences and genetic mechanisms involved in the brain is still in its early
was originally stimulated by findings of increased non-right stages, the recent use of cellular-biology techniques to
handedness among homosexuals of both sexes (Lalumière, unravel the chain of mechanisms involved in the hormone-
Blanchard, & Zucker, 2000), but attempts at finding an based sexual differentiation of specific sex-dimorphic nuclei
association of homosexuality with fluctuating asymmetry as of the limbic system and the amygdala in the neonatal
a broader index of developmental instability have been (Burks, Wright, & McCarthy, 2007; Todd, Schwarz, &
unsuccessful (Rahman, 2005). McCarthy, 2005; Todd, Schwarz, Mong, & McCarthy, 2007)
The most commonly offered theory places sexual orien- and pubertal (Zehr, Todd, Schulz, McCarthy, & Sisk, 2006)
tation in the context of the sexual differentiation of brain and periods of development is likely to contribute in a major way
behavior in general, with a focus on the role of pre- and to the identification of genes with specific functions in these
perinatal sex hormones in this process (e.g., Ellis & Ames, processes.
1987; Rahman & Wilson, 2003). This approach was pre- Recent quite large-scale behavioral data on humans
sumably prompted by the association of human homosex- continue to support the ‘‘inversion’’ perspective regarding
uality with gender-atypical (non-sexual) behavior and goes homosexuality (e.g., Lippa, 2005), and the attempts to find a
back to the mid-19th century, when scientific embryology hormonal cause continue. Considerable efforts have been
began focusing on the development of the sex-dimorphic made to identify somatic markers of prenatal sex hormone
reproductive tract and its disorders. Such research yielded effects, such as shifts in the second to fourth finger length ratio
medical explanations of somatic hermaphroditism, and (2D:4D) in homosexuals of both sexes (Manning, 2002); the
analogous medical concepts were applied to the explanation findings are suggestive, but far from uniform (Manning,
of homosexuality (Ulrichs, 1862, 1868, as cited in Hirsch- Churchill, & Peters, 2007; Rahman, 2005). Another such
feld, 1906; see also Kennedy, 1988, Ch. 5). In this context, marker may be the reduction of spontaneous otoacoustic
homosexuality was often categorized as an inversion (of emissions in lesbians (McFadden, 2002), which is awaiting
gender roles). With the rapidly advancing techniques of replication by independent teams. Clearly, more direct evi-
measurement and synthesis of sex hormones in the second dence of prenatal sex hormone effects would be desirable.

123
Arch Sex Behav (2008) 37:85–99 87

However, experimental variations of the prenatal sex-hor- of population prevalence figures of bisexual and homosex-
mone milieu solely for behavioral research purposes cannot ual orientation in CAH women from these data.
be ethically justified. Money’s team at Johns Hopkins intro- Even if the majority of the findings listed support an
duced as an alternative the behavioral study of syndromes of association of classical CAH with bisexual or homosexual
intersexuality, which represent naturally occurring extreme orientation in women, a causative interpretation of these
variations of the sex-hormone milieu (Ehrhardt, Evers, & findings in terms of androgen effects on sexual orientation is
Money, 1968). Classical (prenatal-onset) congenital adrenal not as compelling as would be findings from randomized
hyperplasia (CAH) in 46,XX individuals is the most prevalent control trials of androgen treatment. Human studies of this
of the classical intersex syndromes and by far the most thor- kind only use ‘‘quasi-experimental designs’’ with, in the best
oughly investigated in terms of endocrinology and psychol- case, ‘‘patched-up controls’’ (Kazdin, 2002). One could
ogy. About 90% of CAH patients suffer from the deficiency of strengthen the case for the role of androgens by demonstrating
the enzyme, 21-hydroxylase (Grumbach, Hughes, & Conte, a dose–response relationship between the degree of prenatal
2003). As one of several endocrine consequences, 46,XX androgen exposure and the degree of later sexual orientation.
fetuses with CAH are exposed to unusually high levels of Ideally, we would measure prenatal hormone levels repeat-
androgens during fetal development, which variably mascu- edly over the course of fetal development and derive from such
linize the genitalia and presumably also the brain and later measurements an index of the degree of prenatal androgen
behavior. exposure. However, the health risks of even one-time cross-
If sexual orientation is sexually differentiated in a similar sectional determinations of androgen levels in the amniotic
fashion, 46,XX women with classical CAH should show an fluid, for instance, can be justified only if there are compelling
increase in bisexuality and homosexuality. Table 1 presents 18 medical indications for the procedures involved. A relatively
studies on sexual orientation in 46,XX women with classical crude alternative is the demonstration of dose–response rela-
CAH that were published between 1968 and 2007. The data tions on the group level using the clinical-endocrine or
suggest several conclusions. Most women with classical CAH molecular-genetics classification of CAH subtypes that differ
are heterosexual, but where control groups or population in severity, i.e., degree of 21-hydroxylase deficiency and,
data are used for comparisons, bisexual and homosexual thereby, degree of androgen excess. Within classical CAH,
orientation appear increased in CAH women (although four commonly two major subtypes are distinguished, the more
studies did not find such increases: Kühnle, Bullinger, & severe salt-wasting (SW) variant and the simple virilizing
Schwartz, 1995; Lev-Ran, 1974; Müller, Bidlingmaier, Förster, (SV) variant, and several studies have shown that bisexuality
& Knorr, 1982; Slijper et al., 1992). In 11 studies that sepa- and homosexuality are increased more in the SW than the SV
rately assessed both erotic/romantic imagery and the gender variant (Dittmann et al., 1992; Horn, 1997; Mulaikal et al.,
of actual sex partners, increased bisexuality and homosexu- 1987; Zucker et al., 1996) or in CAH women with higher
ality were seen more commonly in imagery (Ehrhardt, 1979; Prader stages of genital masculinization at birth, which are
Ehrhardt et al., 1968; Gastaud et al., 2007; Guth, Witchel, also (moderately) correlated with CAH severity (Gastaud
Witchel, & Lee, 2006; Horn, 1997; May, Boyle, & Grant, et al., 2007); the earlier finding by our team that CAH-SW
1996; Money, Schwartz, & Lewis, 1984; Stikkelbroeck women with gender dysphoria are gynecophilic fits in with the
et al., 2003; Zucker et al., 1996), although the differences other data (Meyer-Bahlburg et al., 1996).
did not always reach statistical significance, and in two studies Our current study had several goals: (1) To replicate the
(Lev-Ran, 1974; Müller et al., 1982), no CAH woman was published findings on sexual orientation in a relatively large
positive for bisexuality or homosexuality on either variable. sample of adult women with classical CAH using a sys-
The methodology of this research is, however, largely tematic assessment of multiple aspects of sexual orientation,
unsatisfactory. Most studies employed relatively small sam- and to establish at which age the women reach the respective
ples, many also lacked a control group, and two (Johannsen romantic/erotic milestones; (2) to extend the dose–response
et al., 2006; Kühnle et al., 1995) even included non-classical approach to the mildest form of CAH, the non-classical (NC)
(NC) women in their CAH sample and did not analyze them variant, which becomes clinically symptomatic (in somatic
separately, although NC women become clinically symp- terms) only after birth, in childhood or adolescence; (3) to
tomatic only years after birth. Many studies limited their examine to what extent sexual orientation and global mea-
data to the gender of actual sex partners or provided only sures of gender behavior other than sexual orientation are
quite limited detail on imagery, in both cases often without correlated; (4) to test whether the prediction of the behavioral
employing systematic assessment methods. In addition, phenotype from the endocrine phenotype can be enhanced
studies differed in sample composition regarding the per- by the molecular-genetics classification; (5) to answer the
centages of mild and severe cases of CAH women and their question how commonly CAH women see themselves as
age distribution. Thus, it is difficult to derive solid estimates men in their romantic/erotic imagery; and (6) to perform a

123
88

Table 1 Sexual orientation in women with CAH: Published data


Reference Country Sample

123
Descriptiona N Age (yrs) Imagery Actual partners Imagery & partners
Range M SD % Hetb % Bi/Hoc No datad % Hetb % Bi/Hoc No datad % Hetb % Bi/ Hoc No datad

Ehrhardt et al. (1968) USA CAH: late-treated 23 19–55 32.3 39.1 43.5 17.4 52.2 17.4 30.4
Lev-Ran (1974) USSR CAH: late-treated 18 13–43 26 77.7 0.0 23.3 38.8 0.0 62.2
Ehrhardt (1979) USA CAH: early-treated 13 11–24 16.8 15.4e 7.7
f
Müller et al. (1982) Germany CAH: 2 SW, (rest SV?) 14 18–30 21 64.3 0.0 35.7 78.6 0.0 21.4
Money et al. (1984) USA CAH: 30 17–26 21g 40.0 37.0 23.0 40.0 13.3 46.7 40.0 36.7 23.3
Controls: 15 AIS, 12 MRKS 27 16–? 66.7 3.7 29.6 92.6 7.4 0.0
Mulaikal, Migeon, USA CAH: 40 SW, 40 SV 80 18–69 33.0 57.5 5.0 37.5
and Rock (1987)
Dittmann, Kappes, Germany CAH: 12 SW, 20 SV 34 11–41 20.0h/
and Kappes (1992) 2 no info 26.5i
Subgroup 9 21–41 22.2 55.6
Controls: Sisters 14 11–31 0.0h/
0.0i
Slijper et al. (1992) Netherlands CAH: 10 16–33 20.8 20.0 0.0 80.0 100.0 0.0 0.0
Kühnle et al. (1995) Germany CAH: 20 SW, 17 SV, 8 NC 45 27.0 6.6 4.4k
Controls: Hospital 46 26.3 5.4 2.2k
staff & families
May et al. (1996) UK CAH: 19 18–37 26.1 5.6 26.5e,l 10.5
Controls: Diabetics 17 18–34 27.0 5.3 0.0m
Zucker et al. (1996)n Canada CAH: 19 SW, 12 SV 31 24.4 6.7 66.7 26.7 6.7 80.0 3.3 16.7
Controls: Sisters, fem. cousins 15 25.6 5.5 100.0 0.0 0.0 100.0 0.0 0.0
Horn (1997) Germany CAH: 17 SW, 12 SV 29 17–36 23.3 5.1 27.6 3 27.6
Stikkelbroeck et al. (2003) Netherlands CAH: All SW 8o 18–29 22.8 50.0 0.0
Hines, Brook, UK CAH: 14 SW, 2 no info 16 23.6 6.7 31p
and Conway (2004) Controls: Sisters, fem. cousins 15 22.7 3.4 0.0q
Morgan, Murphy, Lacey, UK CAH: 18 18–36 22.2r
and Conway (2005)
Johannsen, Ripa, Mortensen, Denmark CAH: 21 SW, 33 17–51 30.3 12.1s
and Main (2006) 6 SV, 5 NC, 1 no info
Controls: Civil Registry 33 17–51 0.0s
Guth et al. (2006) USA CAH: 5 23–34 27 80.0 20.0 0.0 80.0 0.0 20.0
Arch Sex Behav (2008) 37:85–99
Table 1 continued
Reference Country Sample
Descriptiona N Age (yrs) Imagery Actual partners Imagery & partners
Range M SD % Hetb % Bi/Hoc No datad % Hetb % Bi/Hoc No datad % Hetb % Bi/ Hoc No datad

Gastaud et al. (2007) France CAHt: 4 P-I, 6 P-II, 11 P-III, 35 18–43 29.5 20.0 5.8
11 P-IV, 3 P-V
Controls: Healthy 69 19–45 30.0 5.7
hospital-staff families
Arch Sex Behav (2008) 37:85–99

a
‘‘CAH’’ refers to classical CAH (SW, SV, excluding NC) unless otherwise specified
b
‘‘% Het’’ percentage of women with exclusively heterosexual imagery/experience
c
‘‘%Bi/Ho’’ percentage of women with bisexual or exclusively homosexual imagery/experience
d
‘‘No data’’ combines ‘‘no sexual imagery/partner experience’’ and ‘‘missing information’’
e
‘‘Including the women with actual same-sex partner experience
f
Median
g
According to Zucker and Bradley (1995), p. 143
h
‘‘Had or wishes to have long-term/steady relationship with F partner’’
i
At least one positive item on a 10-item HOM scale indicating homosexual fantasies, wishes, or experiences
k
‘‘Being lesbian and living with a female partner’’
l
‘‘Strong sense of sexual appreciation of females’’
m
‘‘Sexual interest in other women’’
n
Lifetime data
o
Sexual orientation is based on only 6 of the 8 women
p
For the past 12 months
q
‘‘None rated themselves as bisexual or homosexual; all indicated their behavior had been exclusively or mainly heterosexual’’
r
Self-categorization as homosexual or bisexual; partner experience was not specified
s
‘‘Present relationships’’
t
Prader stage of genital development at birth
89

123
90 Arch Sex Behav (2008) 37:85–99

methodological study of the interrelationship of the sexual Measures and Procedure


orientation variables.
All women underwent an 8–10 h protocol (often spread out
over several days) of standard self-report questionnaires,
psychometric tests, physical examinations, and systematic
Method interviews. Sexual orientation was assessed as part of the
Sexual Behavior Assessment Schedule (SEBAS-A, 1983
Participants ed.; H. F. L. Meyer-Bahlburg & A. A. Ehrhardt, Privately
printed), a comprehensive sexual-history interview schedule
The current study is part of a comprehensive long-term follow- that covers psychosexual milestones, sexual orientation,
up project of women with CAH. Selection and recruitment of sexual activity level, and sexual dysfunctions. Its adminis-
participants were described in detail elsewhere (Meyer- tration takes approximately 1 h. The SEBAS-A was placed
Bahlburg, Dolezal, Baker, Ehrhardt, & New, 2006) and will be late in the overall protocol in order to facilitate rapport
only briefly summarized here. During an initial pilot phase of development between interviewer and interviewee and,
this project, a small number of women with CAH was thereby, increase disclosure of sensitive information, and
recruited from two pediatric endocrine clinics in New York the SEBAS-A instructions to the interviewee emphasized
City. Subsequent data collection for the main study was lim- the importance of accuracy to enhance the participants’
ited to the senior endocrine author’s (M.I.N.) clinic, but data motivation. All SEBAS-A interviews (as well as most other
from both study phases were combined for the final analysis. study interviews) with women were conducted by female
Eligible were all adult women with CAH due to 21-hydrox- interviewers in order to facilitate self-disclosure. Inter-
ylase deficiency for whom the molecular genetics of the 21- viewers were clinical psychologists who were specifically
hydroxylase gene had been determined and who spoke Eng- trained for sexual research interviewing. Procedures were
lish. Geographically, the participating women were spread introduced to keep the interviewers from identifying the
over the entire United States and other continents. Transpor- group membership of the study participants along with
tation reimbursement was provided for women within the instructions for the women against the disclosure of their
continental US. medical histories to the interviewers. All interviews were
The total analysis sample for CAH women in this report audiotaped to permit monitoring of interviewer perfor-
included 40 SW women, 21 SV women, and 82 NC women. mance. Excellent interrater reliability of the SEBAS-A has
Almost all CAH women were on glucocorticoid replace- been demonstrated (Meyer-Bahlburg et al., 1995).
ment treatment at the time of the study. A total of 24 non- SEBAS-A variables pertinent to sexual orientation covered
CAH control women (labeled COS) consisted of sisters and masturbation fantasies, masturbation erotica, romantic/erotic
female cousins of participating CAH women. Ages ranged fantasies during sexual relations with a partner, romantic/sexual
from 18–61 years (subgroup means, 28.8–34.7 years). Not daydreams, romantic/sexual nightdreams, sexual attractions,
included in this count are two SW women: one patient ‘‘Total Imagery,’’ actual sex partners (‘‘Actual Partners’’), and
because he had changed to living as a man and was therefore overall sexual responsiveness (‘‘Overall Kinsey’’). The first six
not administered questionnaires and interviews designed for variables addressed ‘‘current’’ sexual orientation, with ‘‘cur-
women, the other because of cognitive limitations which rent’’ defined as the 12 months prior to interview, and each was
interfered with the standard administration of assessment preceded by a question concerning its frequency (e.g., ‘‘How
instruments; the latter was also seriously considering gender often did you have romantic or sexual nightdreams during the
change to male at the time of the examination. past 12 months?’’). The remaining three aspects were rated
In addition to the control group of sisters and female separately for the past 12 months and for lifelong (‘‘Lifetime’’)
cousins, we included for selected comparisons and illus- patterns (thus, yielding six variables), with lifelong defined as
trations two control groups (labeled COD) from our ‘‘since puberty’’ (for ‘‘Total Imagery’’ and overall sexual res-
preceding project on the long-term behavioral after effects ponsiveness), or as ‘‘since becoming sexually active, excluding
of prenatal diethylstilbestrol (DES) exposure (Meyer- prepubertal sexual activities’’ (for sexual relations); both defi-
Bahlburg et al., 1995; Pillard et al., 1993), which had used nitions of lifelong included the past 12 months.
the same assessment instrument for sexual behavior and For each sexual-orientation variable, interviewers’ ratings
sexual orientation: 67 DES-unexposed female controls and used the Kinsey Rating Scale (Kinsey, Pomeroy, Martin,
60 DES-unexposed male controls, of comparable age ranges & Gebhard, 1953) with the following formulations: 0 =
and subgroup means. entirely heterosexual; 1 = largely heterosexual but inci-
Study procedures were approved by the appropriate dentally homosexual; 2 = largely heterosexual but also
institutional review boards, and all participants gave written distinctly homosexual; 3 = equally heterosexual and homo-
informed consent. sexual; 4 = largely homosexual but also distinctly hetero-

123
Arch Sex Behav (2008) 37:85–99 91

sexual; 5 = largely homosexual but incidentally heterosex- equality of variances, weighted least square (WLS) regres-
ual; and 6 = entirely homosexual. Since the Kinsey team sions were performed for analyses requiring demographic
had not defined ‘‘distinct,’’ ‘‘a distinct’’ homosexual history controls; otherwise, t-tests with equal variances not assumed.
(Kinsey score 2 or ‘‘K2’’) was rated when the woman had In addition, we also followed the wide-spread practice of
experiences such as homosexual dreams or fantasies over a dichotomizing the Kinsey-format scales for statistical tests
period of at least 1-year recurring with some regularity (not so as to contrast the number of K0-1 women (exclusively or
less than ‘‘about once a month’’). Whenever a subscale was almost exclusively heterosexual) with the number of K2-6
rated ‘‘K2,’’ the corresponding global score could not be women. Four-group and two-group comparisons of these
rated less than ‘‘K2.’’ dichotomized variables were performed by way of Fisher’s
The variables on actual sex partners were based on Exact test and its extension to multiple groups. In view of the
detailed structured interview sections concerning diverse modest sample sizes of women with the rare syndrome of
romantic and sexual activities, separately for male and CAH, both conventionally (p < .05) and marginally (p <
female partners. The definition of sexual relations as used .10) significant results were listed. Effect sizes (Cohen’s d) of
here for actual sex partners required genital contact including differences between CAH groups and controls were calcu-
but not limited to penile–vaginal intercourse; it did not lated using the variance of the controls, because of the
require orgasm. Total Imagery was a global rating encom- commonly increased variance of CAH samples on outcome
passing the preceding six variables on imagery and attrac- variables. All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS
tions and taking into consideration the frequencies of the for Windows Release 13.01 (December 12, 2004) or
respective experiences as reported by the interviewee. StatXact, 4.0.1 (2000).
Overall sexual responsiveness was a global rating based on
Total Imagery and Actual Partners.
Results

Data Analysis Psychosexual Milestones

The study groups were compared on all sexual-behavior and Table 2 shows findings on selected items from the SEBAS-
sexual-orientation variables by overall standard parametric A section, Psychosexual Milestones. Substantial minorities
procedures (ANOVA), and then, for exploratory purposes, of women in all groups experienced same-sex crushes, while
pairwise by independent-samples t-test. If, for a given fewer women experienced same-sex love and same-sex
comparison, any of the demographic variables (age; ethnic- genital sex. CAH women were increased above control
ity; and mean parental education as an index of socioeco- women in all three categories, and SW women were highest
nomic status) significantly differed between the study groups (the differences reached significance in several pair com-
and was correlated with the outcome variable in the control parisons [data not shown]). The NC group was higher than
group, the potentially confounding influence of the demo- the COS group on all 3 variables (significantly so for genital
graphic variable was controlled for by including it in a sex and marginally significantly for love [data not shown]).
regression analysis. If study groups differed significantly in The ages at first occurrence of these states or events
variability of the outcome variables on Levene’s test for the appeared to be relatively late. However, as Table 3 shows,

Table 2 Homosexual milestones


COS (n = 22) NC (n = 81) SV (n = 21) SW (n = 39) pa

Number (%) of participants with the experience


Crush 3 (14%) 13 (16%) 6 (29%) 15 (38%) .033
Love (yes or maybe versus no) 0 (0%) 4 (5%) 1 (5%) 7 (18%) .048
Genital sex 0 (0%) 9 (11%) 1 (5%) 6 (15%) n.s.
Mean age (SD) in years
First crush 15.5 (–b) 15.0 (4.9) 21.8 (7.6) 13.6 (5.6) (.057)
First love – (–) 18.8 (2.1) 18.5 (–b) 19.1 (3.8) n.s.
First genital sex – (–) 23.2 (4.7) 13.5 (–b) 19.8 (7.5) n.s.
a
Unordered Row by Column Table Test (Stat Xact) for the top three variables; ANOVA (NC, SV, SW) for age at first crush; t-test (NC, SW) for the
remaining two variables
b
No SD (n = 1)

123
92 Arch Sex Behav (2008) 37:85–99

this does not indicate a general delay of psychosexual with both heterosexual and homosexual experiences were
milestones (heterosexual and homosexual combined) in too few for statistical analysis.)
CAH. Rather, among those women with a history of both
heterosexual and homosexual experiences, the first experi-
ence tended to be heterosexual. For instance, the number of Sexual Orientation: Lifetime
such women who first had a heterosexual crush as compared
to those who first had a homosexual crush breaks down as As can be seen on Table 4, four-group comparisons on the
follows: COS 1:0; NC 8:1; SV 6:0; and SW 9:4. The same three lifetime Kinsey variables are highly significant. The
holds true of the female COD group (DES-unexposed con- data show a rather consistent progression of Kinsey score
trols) with 5:3. (For the other milestone categories, women means (and also SDs), except for the SV group on Actual

Table 3 Psychosexual milestones (homo- and heterosexual combined): Mean age (SD) in years
COS NC SV SW p (ANOVA)

First crush 10.6 (2.3) 9.4 (2.9) 10.3 (3.0) 10.8 (2.9) (.093)
Menarche 13.4 (1.1) 13.1 (2.1) 14.1 (2.4) 14.1 (2.9) n.s.
First masturbation 13.9 (6.0) 13.5 (4.4) 14.2 (4.3) 15.4 (5.8) n.s.
First genital sexa 17.6 (2.7) 17.6 (4.3) 17.3 (3.8) 18.3 (3.8) n.s
First orgasmb 18.4 (4.5) 17.3 (5.2) 18.9 (4.2) 18.3 (6.5) n.s.
First love 19.4 (5.3) 18.0 (3.6) 17.9 (3.6) 20.2 (4.3) (.094)
a
With a partner, any mode
b
With or without a partner, any mode

Table 4 Lifetime Kinsey-scale scores by subgroup: Means (SDs) and distributions


COS NC SV SW p
M (SD) n M (SD) n M (SD) n M (SD) n

Total Imagery 0.2 (0.5) 21 0.7 (1.1) 79 1.1 (1.8) 21 1.8 (2.2) 38 <.001a
K0–1 20 60 14 20
K2–6 1 19 7 18
No imagery 1 1 0 1
Total 22 80 21 39
K2–6/(K0-1 + K2-6) 5% 24% 33% 47% .003b
Actual Partners 0.0 (0.0) 18 0.2 (0.6) 77 0.1 (0.4) 20 1.1 (2.3) 28 .001a
K0-1 18 74 19 22
K2-6 0 3 1 6
No partners 4 4 1 11
Total 22 81 21 39
K2-6/(K0-1 + K2-6) 0% 4% 5% 21% .017b
Overall Kinsey 0.2 (0.5) 22 0.7 (0.9) 80 0.6 (1.0) 21 1.7 (2.1) 39 <.001a
K0-1 21 60 15 23
K2-6 1 20 6 16
No sexual responsiveness 0 0 0 0
Total 22 80 21 39
K2-6/(K0-1 + K2-6) 5% 25% 29% 41% .014b
a
ANOVA
b
Unordered Row by Column Table Test (StatXact) for K2-6 versus KO-1 across 4 subgroups

123
Arch Sex Behav (2008) 37:85–99 93

Partners and Overall Kinsey, and of K-2-6% for all three


variables with increasing degree of androgenization in terms
6 of 21-OHD severity, although of the 18 respective two-group
comparisons of Kinsey scores only 12 were conventionally
5 and 1 marginally significant, and of the 18 two-group com-
Overall Kinsey, Lifetime

parisons of the dichotomized Kinsey scales only 7 were


4 conventionally and 3 marginally significant (data not shown).
Figure 1 shows the Overall Kinsey scores for lifetime sexual
3 orientation, which combines both imagery and actual partner
experience (using reverse scoring for the male COD group to
2 facilitate comparisons). Effect sizes d for the Kinsey-score
differences between the COS and the CAH subgroups were
1 1.0 for NC, .9 for SV, 3.0 for SW, and 2.3 for women with
classical CAH (SV and SW combined). The effect size for the
0
difference between the male and female DES-unexposed
control groups (COD-F and COD-M) from our preceding
DES study was 6.7 with the combined SD used as reference
N= 63 22 80 21 39 60 (8.1 with the women’s SD used as reference).
COD-F COS NC SV SW COD-M The K2-6% for women with classical CAH (SV and SW
combined) was 42% for Total Imagery, 15% for Actual
Fig. 1 Overall sexual responsiveness, lifetime, as a function of CAH
severity. To facilitate comparisons of the CAH effects with the usual Partners, and 37% for the Overall Kinsey rating. The NC
sex difference, DES-unexposed female controls (COD-F) from our group was significantly higher than the COS group on all
preceding DES project have been added on the left and DES- three Kinsey scores (p B .003, B .006, and B .002, respec-
unexposed male controls (COD-M; reverse-scored) on the right. Both
tively). The table also shows that the Kinsey scores for
individual values (graphed with jitter-function software) and means
and SDs are shown lifetime Total Imagery were higher than for lifetime Actual

Table 5 Homosexual partner experience, lifetime: Mean (SD) frequency category and frequency distributions
COS (n = 22) NC (n = 80) SV (n = 21) SW (n = 38) p (ANOVA)

Same-sex sex partners 0.0 (0.0) 0.2 (0.45) 0.1 (0.2) 0.5 (1.5) .037
None 22 71 20 32
1 6 1 1
2–3 3 2
4–6 1
7–10
11–19 1
20–29
30+ 1
% Any 0% 11% 5% 16%
Same-sex sex occasions 0.0 (0.0) 0.4 (1.3) 0.4 (1.7) 1.1 (2.6) (.086)
None 22 71 20 32
1 3
2–3 3
4–6 1 1
7–10
11–19
20–29 1
30–49
50+ 2 1 4

123
94 Arch Sex Behav (2008) 37:85–99

Partners. According to Table 5, the number of women with Sexual Orientation and Non-sexual Gender-related
any actual same-sex partner experience was relatively small, Behavior
and the number of those with considerable experience in
terms of same-sex partner numbers or same-sex occasions The global lifetime and 12 months Kinsey scores were
was even smaller. In interpreting these data, one has to take correlated with selected global variables of gender-related
into consideration that women with classical CAH, and behavior (not including sexual orientation), as listed in
especially those with the SW variant, had significantly lower Meyer-Bahlburg et al. (2006). The expected correlations
lifetime actual-partner numbers and lower total lifetime sex were significant and in the predicted direction, but of modest
occasions (heterosexual and homosexual combined in both size, and stronger for childhood measures than adulthood
variables) than the other groups (data not shown). measures. For instance, the Overall Kinsey score for lifetime
correlated r = -.40 with the Gender scale of the Recalled
Childhood Gender Questionnaire-Revised (RCGQ-R), but
Sexual Orientation: Current (Past 12 Months) the Overall Kinsey score for the past 12 months correlated
only r = -.27 with the (adult) Hobby Preferences Scale
The current data on sexual orientation also show Kinsey (the high end on both gender scales is feminine).
scores for all three CAH subgroups in an apparent dose–
response fashion (Table 6). The gradual increase in Kinsey
scores from non-CAH controls to the most severe SW variant Cross-gender Imagery
applied to all variables, and was highly significant for all
categories except for fantasies during partner sex. Again, the At the end of the imagery section, the participant was asked
NC group had higher Kinsey scores than the COS group on all about the frequency with which they saw themselves as ‘‘a
9 variables (5 differences were conventionally significant and person of the opposite sex’’ in their erotic imagery, sepa-
1 marginally so), the SW group was significantly higher than rately for the past 12 months and lifetime (since puberty,
the two other CAH variants (versus SV with conventional excluding the past 12 months). As Table 7 shows, all groups
significance on 1 variable and marginal significance on 2; except SV included some women with such experience. The
versus NC with conventional significance on 8 variables), but percent of women with such experience and the frequency of
the difference between SV and NC women reached con- having that experience, especially for lifetime, was signifi-
ventional and marginal significance on only 1 variable each. cantly increased in the SW subgroup above all other groups.

Table 6 Mean Kinsey scores (SD) for the past 12 months by subgroup
COS NC SV SW p (ANOVA)

Masturbation erotica 0.8 (1.5) 0.9 (1.3) 3.0 (2.4) 3.4 (2.3) .004
Masturbation fantasies 0.3 (0.8) 0.9 (1.3) 1.7 (1.9) 2.5 (2.4) .000
Fantasies during partner sex 0.3 (0.7) 0.8 (1.3) 1.0 (2.0) 2.4 (3.0) (.069)
Daydreams 0.0 (0.0) 0.6 (1.3) 1.0 (1.8) 2.4 (2.8) .000
Nightdreams 0.1 (0.3) 0.5 (1.1) 1.2 (2.1) 3.2 (2.9) .000
Attractions 0.1 (0.3) 0.3 (0.9) 0.9 (1.3) 1.6 (2.2) .000
Total Imagery 0.2 (0.5) 0.7 (1.1) 1.1 (1.6) 1.9 (2.3) .000
Actual Partners 0.0 (0.0) 0.1 (0.7) 0.05 (0.2) 1.4 (2.6) .000
Overall Kinsey score 0.2 (0.5) 0.5 (0.8) 0.9 (1.3) 1.8 (2.3) .000
Note that the n’s vary somewhat between items because not all women have experience with all of them

Table 7 Seeing self as man during romantic/erotic imagery, by subgroup


COS NC SV SW p (ANOVA or
Exact Test)

Mean frequency (categorized), lifetime 0.1 (0.3) 0.1 (0.5) 0.0 (0.0) 0.4 (0.8) .024
Mean frequency (categorized), past 12 months 0.1 (0.4) 0.1 (0.4) 0.0 (0.0) 0.3 (0.8) (.074)
% Women, lifetime 10% 6% 0% 21% .030
% Women, past 12 months 5% 6% 0% 16% n.s.

123
Arch Sex Behav (2008) 37:85–99 95

Predicting Sexual Orientation from Prenatal Table 9 Prediction of overall Kinsey score (past 12 months) from
Androgenization and Gender Variables endocrinea and geneticb classification of CAH, using stepwise hierar-
chical regression
Using hierarchical stepwise regression, we tried to predict the R2 change p
Overall Kinsey Score for the past 12 months successively
Genetic entered first, clinical second
from the degree of prenatal androgenization in terms of the
Genetic .100 .000
four CAH-severity groups, childhood gender-related behav-
Clinical .007 .356
ior (the Gender scale of the RCGQ-R), adult gender-related
Clinical entered first, genetic second
behavior (the sum score of the Hobby Preferences Scale), and
Clinical .102 .000
Seeing Self as Man during Romantic/Erotic Imagery, Fre-
Genetic .004 .453
quency: Lifetime (Excluding the Past 12 Months). Prenatal
a
androgenization and RCGQ-R Gender contributed signifi- Endocrine classification coded SW = 1, SV = 2, NC = 3
b
cantly to the variance accounted for and were significant in Genetic classification coded by molecular genotypes: V281L or
the final model, but Hobby Preferences and Seeing Self as P30L = 1; I172N = 2, I2 splice = 3, Gene deleted = 4
Man was not (Table 8). I172N (1+% 21-OH; 10% of patients are SW); (3) I2 splice
(0+% 21-OH; most patients are SW); (4) ‘‘Null’’/gene
deleted (0% 21-OH; all patients are SW). In compound
Role of the Molecular Genotype heterozygotes the classification is based on the allele with
the milder mutation. We had severity-classifiable genotypes
Finally, we wanted to test whether the CAH subtypes as for 28 SW women, 17 SV women, and 79 NC women. Using
defined by endocrine criteria or their classification by stepwise hierarchical regression, we either entered the mole-
molecular genotype is more closely associated with sexual cular genotype classification first, and the clinical endocrine
orientation. We employed the widely used severity classi- classification second, or vice versa. As Table 9 shows, nei-
fication of molecular genotypes of the 21-OH gene (White & ther procedure indicated that the variable entered second
Speiser, 2000), which is based on the in-vitro synthesis of added significant predictive variance to sexual orientation
21-OH expressed as % of synthesis by the intact genotype. It outcome. Thus, the two classifications did not differ in their
yields four groups, listed here in ascending severity and with association with the Kinsey score. In this particular sub-
the major molecular genotypes: (1) Genotypes V281L or sample, the endocrine and genetic classifications correlated
P30L (20–50% or 30–60% 21-OH; all patients are NC); (2) Pearson r = .90 with each other, i.e., there was little room
for divergence between molecular genotype and endocrine
Table 8 Prediction of overall Kinsey score (past 12 months) from
phenotype.
degree of prenatal androgenization and gender variables, using step-
wise hierarchical regression
R2 change p Intercorrelations among Sexual Orientation Variables
a,b
Degree of prenatal androgenization .130 .000
For methodological purposes, we constructed an intercor-
RCGQ-R Gender scaleb .062 .002
b relation matrix of all 12-month Kinsey variables. All of the
Hobby Preferences scale .015 .115
Kinsey variables were quite highly intercorrelated, with
Seeing Self as Man During Romantic/Erotic
Pearson r ranging from .50 to .92 (median = .78). A prin-
Imagery, lifelong (excluding past 12 months)c .014 .135
cipal components analysis of all these variables (except for
Final model Masturbation Erotica which had too few women with the
b p
relevant experience and excluding the two global variables,
Total imagery and Overall Kinsey score) yielded one gen-
a,b
Degree of prenatal androgenization -.219 .028 eral factor accounting for 77.2% of the variance. The factor
b
RCGQ-R Gender scale -.386 .001 loadings for the individual variables were: Daydreams .96,
Hobby Preferences scaleb .167 .144 Attractions .93, Masturbation Fantasies .92, Fantasies Dur-
Seeing Self as Man During Romantic/ .118 .135 ing Partner Sex .88, Night Dreams .81, and Actual Partners
Erotic Imagery, lifelong .76. On the basis of the principal components analysis, we
(excluding the past 12 months)c
constructed a 6-item unit-weighted scale of sexual orienta-
a
Degree of prenatal androgenization coded SW = 1, SV = 2, tion. This scale was correlated with the interviewer rating of
NC = 3, COS = 4 overall sexual responsiveness during the past 12 months (at
b
High score = feminine the end of the sexual-orientation section) with Pearson
c
High score = masculine r = .94 (p < .001).

123
96 Arch Sex Behav (2008) 37:85–99

Discussion which had been shown by others (e.g., Hines et al., 2004).
Such findings are also in line with an understanding of sexual
Our data clearly showed increased sexual orientation orientation in the context of sexual differentiation. Given the
towards females (i.e., bisexuality and homosexuality) in many differences in sexual orientation and associated vari-
women with classical CAH (SV and SW combined) com- ables between men and women, the question arises, whether
pared to non-CAH controls. Note that our data constitute the increased sexual orientation towards females associated
underestimates: The two 46,XX individuals with SW-CAH with CAH severity in 46,XX individuals is a model of the
who were excluded from analysis for technical reasons were role of androgens in sexual-orientation development in
both living with female partners at the time of the evaluation males rather than lesbian women. Early developmental
and had a history of sexual responsiveness to females. Thus, research on non-sexual sex-dimorphic behavior in animals
our study corroborates earlier findings on sexual orientation sought to explain sex differences in behavior (e.g., Beatty,
in CAH. We also clearly replicate earlier demonstrations of 1979), and experimental manipulations of pre- and perinatal
increased bisexual/homosexual orientation in SW women androgen levels served to show to what extent one could
compared to SV women, and earlier reports indicating that create male-typical behavior in females so treated. Later,
these shifts are more strongly accentuated in romantic/erotic such findings were also used to explain interindividual
imagery than actual sex-partner experiences. variations of gender-related behavior among females (e.g.,
More surprising is the finding of increased bisexual/ Hines, 2004), and there is some supportive evidence for this
homosexual orientation in NC women above controls in the approach in humans. That this may apply also to sexual
diverse variables evaluated here. This finding is in line with orientation in at least a subgroup of women is suggested by
our earlier report on mild, but significant shifts of the same the fact that earlier research has repeatedly shown that about
NC women towards masculinized gender-related behavior one third of homosexual women have (modestly) increased
other than sexual orientation (Meyer-Bahlburg et al., 2006). levels of androgens (Meyer-Bahlburg, 1979).
In the endocrine literature, NC is usually described as a One of the major limitations of the interpretation of
syndrome characterized by onset of clinical (somatic, phy- findings in classical CAH in relation to animal studies is the
siological) symptoms of androgen excess after birth, in hormone treatment that females with classical CAH typi-
childhood or later. Since there is consensus that the mas- cally receive throughout postnatal life. The classical animal
culinization of gender-related behavior in classical CAH is study of sexual differentiation of brain and behavior exposes
due to the effect of prenatal androgens on the developing the female fetus to androgen treatment during the known
brain, these behavioral shifts in NC women were not early hormone-sensitive period of sexual differentiation of
expected. The finding raises the question whether the mild the brain and then again around the time of puberty/young
androgen excess that is likely to be present in NC fetuses adulthood. That combination tends to maximize the devel-
from the first trimester on, but is insufficient to noticeably opment of cross-gender sexual behaviors, especially when
affect the sexual differentiation of the genitalia, is never- ovariectomy after puberty preceded the treatment with tes-
theless sufficient to slightly affect the sexual differentiation tosterone propionate, as shown by Dörner (1976, p. 164) in
of the brain. Alternatively, the data suggest an unexpected the rat. By contrast, human 46,XX individuals with CAH are
postnatal effect of mild but persistent androgen excess on initially exposed to excess, endogenous androgens from
brain and gender-related behaviors. Our study does not their adrenals during the presumed hormone-sensitive pre-
provide data that would help us to argue in favor of one or natal period of sexual differentiation of the brain, and in the
the other explanation. severe variants at even higher levels than normal males and
In conjunction with the other CAH subgroups and the more chronically so (Carson et al., 1982; Pang, Levine,
control women, our NC data further strengthen the notion of Chow, Faiman, & New, 1979; Pang et al., 1980, 1985;
a dose–response relationships of androgens with sexual Wudy, Dörr, Solleder, Djalali, & Homoki, 1999); however,
orientation, at least on the subgroup level, given that the from birth on, the excess androgen levels are suppressed by
other hormone abnormalities seen in the CAH syndrome glucocorticoid replacement therapy (sometimes to levels
(e.g., deficiency of cortisol and aldosterone, excess of ACTH even lower than is normal for healthy females), the ovaries
and 17-hydroxyprogesterone) are not known to be associ- are left in place, and female puberty is induced by an
ated with masculinization of gender-related behavior in endogenous, largely normal female hormonal milieu.
animals or humans (although specific studies of this kind are Unfortunately, there are no behavioral studies of non-human
yet to be conducted). As we had found analogous relation- primates that mimic the androgen history of 46,XX humans
ships with CAH severity for non-sexual gender-related with CAH, so that we cannot be sure if we should expect to
behavior (Meyer-Bahlburg et al., 2006), we also could see much more bisexuality and homosexuality in 46,XX
confirm significant, but modest-sized correlations of sexual individuals without postnatal androgen suppression. The
orientation scores with non-sexual gender-related behaviors, early study by Ehrhardt et al. (1968) on late-treated women

123
Arch Sex Behav (2008) 37:85–99 97

with CAH who did not have glucocorticoid treatment Our data show that current sexual orientation is not only
available before later adolescence or adulthood does not predicted from the degree of prenatal androgen exposure as
show massive increases in bisexuality or homosexuality indicated by the CAH-severity classification, but in addi-
compared to our data, but most of those early studied women tion also from the degree of masculinization of gender-
must have had milder (non-SW) variants of CAH implying related behavior during childhood. The latter variable
relatively less prenatal androgen excess, or they would not could reflect variable brain responsiveness to prenatal
have survived. Also the fact that the effect size for sexual androgens as well as postnatal psychosocial influences,
orientation in SW as the most severe CAH variant does not provided retrospective reporting bias can be ruled out. This
reach even half the effect size for the sex difference despite study does not provide an opportunity to decide between
chronically high prenatal androgen levels may be related to these options.
the other male-atypical features of 46,XX CAH develop- The association of CAH severity with outcome appears in
ment and/or reflect the absence of Y-based genes in the all aspects of sexual orientation assessed in our protocol and
46,XX brain, which are suspected of participating in the plausibly explains the strong general factor that emerged
sexual differentiation of the brain in other vertebrates from the principal-component analysis. This finding, along
(Arnold, 2002; Gatewood et al., 2006). with the high correlation of the resulting summary scale
Another potentially confounding factor is the fact that with the global interviewer rating of overall sexual
insufficient glucocorticoid replacement or treatment inter- responsiveness, suggests the feasibility of a simplified self-
ruptions lead to virilization, i.e., somatic symptoms of and- report version of a multi-item sexual orientation assessment.
rogen excess, and, if occurring early enough during childhood The current data set on sexual orientation suffers from the
development, also to stunting of growth, while overtreatment same overall limitations as the previous one on gender
brings about variable degrees of obesity, all of which may development (Meyer-Bahlburg et al., 2006), namely, small
reduce attractivity to men and romantic approaches by men, sample size, questionable representativeness, and cross-
and to related body-image concerns of the CAH women. This sectional design. However, our findings on a dose–response
raises the question whether associated inhibition of romantic relationship of androgens and sexual orientation appear
practice in adolescence, perhaps also supported by less-ster- even stronger than for non-sexual gender-related behavior
eotypic feminine leisure time activities and, at least for some and make a persuasive case for the extension of this asso-
CAH women, relative isolation in the peer group, might ciation to women with NC CAH. Overall, our findings
increase the chance of bisexual/homosexual development support a sexual-differentiation perspective involving pre-
rather than exclusively a direct effect of androgens on brain natal androgens on the development of sexual orientation.
circuits that regulate sexual behavior. If so, this might be an
example of the interaction of social and biological factors in Acknowledgments The project described was supported in part by
USPHS Grants HD-38409, RR06020 (GCRC), and by Grant Number
the development of bisexuality/homosexuality, for which U54 RR01-9484 from the National Center for Research Resources
Bearman and Brückner (2002) argued. (NCRR), a component of the National Institutes of Health (NIH). Its
In our data set, the clinical-endocrine classification of contents are solely the responsibilities of the authors and do not nec-
CAH was highly correlated with the classification based on essarily represent the official views of NCRR or NIH. We thank the
study women for their participation in this research. Ms. Rhoda Gruen
molecular genetics, and knowledge of one did not add to the served as interviewer trainer. Ms. Patricia Connolly assisted in word
predictive power of the other in terms of gender outcome. processing.
Thus, there is no suggestion that the molecular-genetic
defect influences sexual-orientation outcome through any
other physiological route than the hormonal abnormalities
caused by the degree of enzyme deficiency. References
It is noteworthy that our data suggest an influence of CAH
Arnold, A. P. (2002). Concepts of genetic and hormonal induction of
severity on cross-gender identity in sexual situations. Our vertebrate sexual differentiation in the twentieth century, with
data here again represent an underestimate because of the special reference to the brain. In D. W. Pfaff, A. P. Arnold, A. M.
exclusion of two SW patients from the current data analysis, Etgen, S. E. Fahrbach, & R. T. Rubin (Eds.), Hormones, brain,
one having undergone gender change to male, the other and behavior (Vol. 4, pp. 105–135). Amsterdam: Academic Press
(Elsevier Science).
considering it (see above). Perhaps, gender-atypicality in Arnold, A. P. (2004). Sex chromosomes and brain gender. Nature
non-sexual behavior along with later emerging atypical Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 701–708.
sexual orientation facilitate in some CAH women an iden- Baum, M. J. (2006). Mammalian animal models of psychosexual
tification with selected aspects of the male role, which may differentiation: When is ‘translation’ to the human situation
possible? Hormones and Behavior, 50, 579–588.
subsequently broaden and thereby lead to late overall gender Bearman, P. S., & Brückner, H. (2002). Opposite-sex twins and
identity change, as it has been documented to occur in some adolescent same-sex attraction. American Journal of Sociology,
46,XX individuals with CAH (Meyer-Bahlburg et al., 1996). 107, 1179–1205.

123
98 Arch Sex Behav (2008) 37:85–99

Beatty, W. W. (1979). Gonadal hormones and sex differences in Hines, M. (2004). Brain gender. New York: Oxford University
nonreproductive behaviors in rodents: Organizational and acti- Press.
vational influences. Hormones & Behavior, 12, 112–163. Hines, M., Brook, C., & Conway, G. S. (2004). Androgen and
Blanchard, R. (2004). Quantitative and theoretical analyses of the psychosexual development: Core gender identity, sexual orienta-
relation between older brothers and homosexuality in men. tion, and recalled childhood gender role behavior in women and
Journal of Theoretical Biology, 230, 173–187. men with congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH). Journal of Sex
Blanchard, R., & Lippa, R. A. (2007). Birth order, sibling sex ratio, Research, 41, 75–81.
handedness, and sexual orientation of male and female partici- Hirschfeld, M. (1906). Vom Wesen der Liebe. Zugleich ein Beitrag zur
pants in a BBC internet research project. Archives of Sexual Lösung der Frage der Bisexualität. Jahrbuch für Sexuelle
Behavior, 36, 163–176. Zwischenstufen, 8, 1–284.
Bocklandt, S., Horvath, S., Vilain, E., & Hamer, D. H. (2006). Extreme Horn, P. (1997). Sexualverhalten, psychosexuelle Orientierung und
skewing of X chromosome inactivation in mothers of homosexual Orgasmuserleben junger Frauen mit kongenitalem adrenogeni-
men. Human Genetics, 118, 691–694. talem Syndrome. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Universität
Burks, S. R., Wright, C. L., & McCarthy, M. M. (2007). Exploration of Leipzig, Leipzig.
prostanoid receptor subtype regulating estradiol and prostaglan- Johannsen, T. H., Ripa, C. P. L., Mortensen, E. L., & Main, K. M.
din E2 induction of spinophilin in developing preoptic area (2006). Quality of life in 70 women with disorders of sex
neurons. Neuroscience, 146, 1117–1127. development. European Journal of Endocrinology, 155, 877–885.
Carson, D. J., Okuno, A., Lee, P. A., Stetten, G., Didolkar, S. M., & Kazdin, A. E. (2002). Research design in clinical psychology (4th ed.).
Migeon, C. J. (1982). Amniotic fluid steroid levels. Fetuses with Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
adrenal hyperplasia, 46,XXY fetuses, and normal fetuses. Amer- Kennedy, H. (1988). Ulrichs: The life and works of Karl Heinrich
ican Journal of Diseases of Children, 136, 218–222. Ulrichs, pioneer of the modern gay movement. Boston: Alyson
Dittmann, R. W., Kappes, M. E., & Kappes, M. H. (1992). Sexual Publications.
behavior in adolescent and adult females with congenital adrenal Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., Martin, C. E., & Gebhard, P. H. (1953).
hyperplasia. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 17, 153–170. Sexual behavior in the human female. Philadelphia: Saunders.
Dörner, G. (1976). Hormones and brain differentiation. Amsterdam: Kühnle, U., Bullinger, M., & Schwarz, H. P. (1995). The quality of life
Elsevier/North-Holland Biomedical Press. in adult female patients with congenital adrenal hyperplasia: A
Ehrhardt, A. A. (1979). Psychosocial adjustment in adolescence in comprehensive study of the impact of genital malformation and
patients with congenital abnormalities of their sex organs. In H. L. chronic disease on female patients’ life. European Journal of
Vallet & I. H. Porter (Eds.), Genetic mechanisms of sexual Pediatrics, 154, 708–716.
development (pp. 473–483). New York: Academic Press. Lalumière, M. L., Blanchard, R., & Zucker, K. J. (2000). Sexual
Ehrhardt, A. A., Evers, K., & Money, J. (1968). Influence of androgen orientation and handedness in men and women: A meta-analysis.
and some aspects of sexually dimorphic behavior in women with Psychological Bulletin, 126, 575–592.
the late-treated adrenogenital syndrome. Johns Hopkins Medical Lev-Ran, A. (1974). Sexuality and educational levels of women with
Journal, 123, 115–122. the late-treated adrenogenital syndrome. Archives of Sexual
Ellis, L., & Ames, M. A. (1987). Neurohormonal functioning and Behavior, 3, 27–32.
sexual orientation: A theory of homosexuality–heterosexuality. Lippa, R. A. (2005). Sexual orientation and personality. Annual Review
Psychological Bulletin, 101, 233–258. of Sex Research, 16, 119–153.
Fleming, A., & Vilain, E. (2005). The endless quest for sex Manning, J. T. (2002). Digit ratio: A pointer to fertility, behavior and
determination genes. Clinical Genetics, 67, 15–25. health. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Gastaud, F., Bouvattier, C., Duranteau, L., Brauner, R., Thibaud, E., Manning, J. T., Churchill, A. J. G, & Peters, M. (2007). The effects of
Kutten, F., et al. (2007). Impaired sexual and reproductive sex, ethnicity, and sexual orientation on self-measured digit ratio
outcomes in women with classical forms of congenital adrenal (2D:4D). Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36, 223–233.
hyperplasia. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, May, B., Boyle, M., & Grant, D. (1996). A comparative study of
92, 1391–1396. sexual experiences: Women with diabetes and women with
Gatewood, J. D., Wills, A., Shetty, S., Xu, J., Arnold, A. P., Burgoyne, congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Journal of Health Psychology,
P. S., et al. (2006). Sex chromosome complement and gonadal sex 1, 479–492.
influence aggressive and parental behaviors in mice. Journal of McFadden, D. (2002). Masculinization effects in the auditory systems.
Neuroscience, 26, 2335–2342. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 31, 99–111.
Gavrilets, S., & Rice, W. R. (2006). Genetic models of homosexuality: Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L. (1977). Sex hormones and male homosex-
Generating testable predictions. Proceedings of the Royal Society uality in comparative perspective. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 6,
B, 273, 3031–3038. 297–325.
Grumbach, M. M., Hughes, I. A., & Conte, F. A. (2003). Disorders of Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L. (1979). Sex hormones and female homo-
sex differentiation. In P. R. Larsen, H. M. Kronenberg, S. sexuality: A critical examination. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 8,
Melmed, & K. S. Polonsky (Eds.), Williams textbook of endocri- 101–119.
nology (10th ed., pp. 842–1002). Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders. Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L., Dolezal, C., Baker, S. W., Ehrhardt, A. A.,
Guth, L. J., Witchel, R. I., Witchel, S. F., & Lee, P. A. (2006). & New, M. I. (2006). Gender development in women with
Relationships, sexuality, gender identity, gender roles, and self- congenital adrenal hyperplasia as a function of disorder severity.
concept of individuals who have congenital adrenal hyperplasia: Archives of Sexual Behavior, 35, 667–684.
A qualitative investigation. Journal of Gay & Lesbian Psycho- Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L., Ehrhardt, A. A., Rosen, L. R., Gruen, R. S.,
therapy, 10, 57–75. Veridiano, N. P., Vann, F. H., et al. (1995). Prenatal estrogens and
Hamer, D., & Copeland, P. (1994). The science of desire: The search the development of homosexual orientation. Developmental
for the gay gene and biology of behavior. New York: Simon & Psychology, 31, 12–21.
Schuster. Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L., Gruen, R. S., New, M. I., Bell, J. J.,
Hamer, D. H., Hu, S., Magnuson, V. L., Hu, N., & Pattatucci, A. M. L. Morishima, A., Shimshi, M., et al. (1996). Gender change from
(1993). A linkage between DNA markers on the X-chromosome female to male in classical congenital adrenal hyperplasia.
and male sexual orientation. Science, 261, 321–327. Hormones and Behavior, 30, 319–332.

123
Arch Sex Behav (2008) 37:85–99 99

Miller, E. M. (2000). Homosexuality, birth order, and evolution: Rice, G., Anderson, C. A., Risch, N., & Ebers, G. (1999). Male
Toward an equilibrium reproductive economics of homosexual- homosexuality: Absence of linkage to microsatellite markers at
ity. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 29, 1–34. Xq28. Science, 284, 665–667.
Money, J., Schwartz, M., & Lewis, V. G. (1984). Adult erotosexual Slijper, F. M. E., van der Kamp H. J., Brandenburg, H., de Muinck
status and fetal hormonal masculinization and demasculinization: Keizer-Schrama S. M. P. F., Drop, S. L. S., & Molenaar, J. C.
46,XX congenital virilizing adrenal hyperplasia and 46,XY (1992). Evaluation of psychosexual development of young
androgen-insensitivity syndrome compared. Psychoneuroendo- women with CAH: A pilot study. In W. Bezemer, P. Cohen-
crinology, 9, 405–414. Kettenis, K. Slob, & N. van Son-Schoones (Eds.), Sex matters (pp.
Morgan, J. F., Murphy, H., Lacey, J. H., & Conway, G. (2005). Long 47–50). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers B.V.
term psychological outcome for women with congenital adrenal Sommer V., & Vasey P. L. (Eds.). (2006). Homosexual behavior in
hyperplasia: Cross sectional survey. British Medical Journal, 330, animals. An evolutionary perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge
340–341. University Press.
Mulaikal, R. M., Migeon, C. J., & Rock, J. A. (1987). Fertility rates in Stikkelbroeck, N. M. M. L., Beerendonk, C. C. M., Willemsen, W. N.
female patients with congenital adrenal hyperplasia due to 21- P., Schreuders-Bais, C. A., Feitz, W. F. J., Rieu, P. N. M. A., et al.
hydroxylase deficiency. New England Journal of Medicine, 316, (2003). The long term outcome of feminizing genital surgery for
178–182. congenital adrenal hyperplasia: Anatomical, functional and
Müller, M., Bidlingmaier, F., Förster, C., & Knorr, D. (1982). cosmetic outcomes, psychosexual development, and satisfaction
Psychosexuelles Verhalten von Frauen mit adrenogenitalem in adult female patients. Journal of Pediatric and Adolescent
Syndrom. Helvetica Paediatrica Acta, 37, 571–580. Gynecology, 16, 289–296.
Mustanski, B. S., DuPree, M. G., Nievergelt, C. M., Bocklandt, S., Todd, B. J., Schwarz, J. M., & McCarthy, M. M. (2005). Prostaglandin-
Schork, N. J., & Hamer, D. H. (2005). A genomewide scan of E2: A point of divergence in estradiol-mediated sexual differen-
male sexual orientation. Human Genetics, 116, 272–278. tiation. Hormones and Behavior, 48, 512–521.
Pang, S., Levine, L. S., Cederqvist, L. L., Fuentes, M., Riccardi, V. M., Todd, B. J., Schwarz, J. M., Mong, J. A., & McCarthy, M. M. (2007).
Holcombe J. H., et al. (1980). Amniotic fluid concentrations of Glutamate AMPA/kainate receptors, not GABAA receptors,
delta 5 and delta 4 steroids in fetuses with congenital adrenal mediate estradiol-induced sex differences in the hypothalamus.
hyperplasia due to 21-hydroxylase deficiency and in anencephalic Developmental Neurobiology, 67, 304–315.
fetuses. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 51, Wallen, K., & Baum, M. J. (2002). Masculinization and defeminization
223–229. in altricial and precocial mammals: Comparative aspects of
Pang, S., Levine, L. S., Chow, D. M., Faiman, C., & New, M. I. (1979). steroid hormone action. In D. W. Pfaff, A. P. Arnold, A. M. Etgen,
Serum androgen concentrations in neonates and young infants S. E. Fahrbach, & R. T. Rubin (Eds.), Hormones, brain, and
with congenital adrenal hyperplasia due to 21-hydroxylase behavior (Vol. 4. pp. 385–423). Amsterdam: Academic Press
deficiency. Clinical Endocrinology, 11, 575–584. (Elsevier Science).
Pang, S., Pollack, M. S., Loo, M., Green, O., Nussbaum, R., Clayton, White, P. C., & Speiser, P. W. (2000). Congenital adrenal hyperplasia
G., et al. (1985). Pitfalls of prenatal diagnosis of 21-hydroxylase due to 21-hydroxylase deficiency. Endocrine Reviews, 21, 245–
deficiency congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Journal of Clinical 291.
Endocrinology and Metabolism, 61, 89–97. Wudy, S. A., Dörr, H. G., Solleder, C., Djalali, M., & Homoki, J.
Pfaus, J. G., Kippin, T. E., & Coria-Avila, G. (2003). What can animal (1999). Profiling steroid hormones in amniotic fluid of midpre-
models tell us about human sexual response? Annual Review of gnancy by routine stable isotope dilution/gas chromatography-
Sex Research, 14, 1–63. mass spectrometry: Reference values and concentrations in
Pillard, R. C., & Bailey, J. M. (1998). Human sexual orientation has a fetuses at risk for 21-hydroxylase deficiency. Journal of Clinical
heritable component. Human Biology, 70, 347–365. Endocrinology & Metabolism, 84, 2724–2728.
Pillard, R. C., Rosen, L. R., Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L., Weinrich, J. M., Zehr, J. L., Todd, B. J., Schulz, K. M., McCarthy, M. M., & Sisk, C. L.
Feldman, J. F., Gruen, R. S., & Ehrhardt, A. A. (1993). (2006). Dendritic pruning of the medial amygdala during pubertal
Psychopathology and social functioning in men prenatally development of the male Syrian hamster. Journal of Neurobiol-
exposed to diethylstilbestrol (DES). Psychosomatic Medicine, ogy, 66, 578–590.
55, 485–491. Zucker, K. J., & Bradley, S. J. (1995). Gender identity disorder and
Rahman, Q. (2005). Fluctuating asymmetry, second to fourth finger psychosexual problems in children and adolescents. New York:
length ratios and human sexual orientation. Psychoneuroendo- Guilford Press.
crinology, 30, 382–391. Zucker, K. J., Bradley, S. J., Oliver, G., Blake, J., Fleming, S., & Hood,
Rahman, Q., & Wilson, G. D. (2003). Born gay? The psychobiology of J. (1996). Psychosexual development of women with congenital
human sexual orientation. Personality and Individual Differences, adrenal hyperplasia. Hormones and Behavior, 30, 300–318.
34, 1337–1382.

123
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

You might also like