You are on page 1of 8

Is there a 'critical age' for language acquisition?

By: Maria Karra

The argument I am supporting in this article is that there is a critical period for second
language acquisition, and that it applies only to accent. To support my argument, I will
examine the case of J.M., a native Spanish speaker who learned English as a second
language. In addition, I will examine whether the conclusions drawn from J.M.’s case are
in agreement with some of the existing literature where viewpoints of linguists are
represented.
J.M. started learning English at the age of eleven in Spain. His teachers gave great
importance to syntax and grammar, and consequently J.M. was very competent in these
areas. However, all of his teachers were non-native speakers of English and their accent
was not native-like; as a result, J.M.’s English sound production was also non-native like.
In the classroom, J.M. was never corrected when he pronounced a word incorrectly. J.M.
was immersed into an English-speaking environment when he came to the United States
at the age of 18. This age is quite beyond the critical age, which ends at puberty,
according to Lenneberg (1967, cited in Grimshaw et al., 1998).

I will examine whether and how J.M.’s accent, vocabulary, grammar and syntax evolved
after his arrival in the United States, and will investigate whether these results agree with
the existing literature. First, let’s examine vocabulary. Contrary to what one might expect,
J.M. had greater difficulty learning new words during childhood than after the age of 18.
This can be easily explained if we consider that during childhood, J.M. was not exposed to
the foreign language continuously, but only in the classroom (1-2 hours per day) and a
couple of hours outside the classroom, when watching English television programs. In
contrast, in the United States he increased his vocabulary with great ease. I believe this
was due to two factors: repetition and need. By repetition I mean that J.M. was now
exposed to the language continuously. Marcotte and Morere (1990) showed that
environmental deprivation leads to atypical brain organization as regards language
functions. Marcotte and Morere’s study concentrated on first language acquisition, and we
could hardly say that J.M. was deprived of learning the language; however, J.M.’s case
agrees with Marcotte and Morere’s results in the following aspect: environmental factors

1
play a very important role in language acquisition. In J.M.’s case, vocabulary repetition
constitutes an environmental factor that is linguistic in nature. The need to communicate is
of great importance for language acquisition; it is a big incentive to the learner. This need
is the pressure that Hurford (1991) refers to when he argues that the capacity to acquire
language “was helped to happen by selective pressure resulting from the enormous
usefulness of language” (p.172), or when he argues that “the critical period effect “just
happened”, and was allowed to happen because of the lack of selective pressure to
acquire (more) language (or to acquire it again) once it has been acquired” (p. 172).
My argument is that there is no critical period for vocabulary, and that vocabulary can be
acquired at advanced ages. In first language acquisition, the reason that the increase in
children’s vocabulary is much greater than that of adults’ vocabulary is that children have a
greater need to learn new words because their vocabulary is not rich enough at an early
age, thus making communication difficult. J.M.’s case is similar to a child acquiring his first
language. Although for J.M. English is his second language, he felt a similar need to
communicate, since his English vocabulary was not rich enough, making communication
difficult in his everyday life in the United States. The above conclusion agrees with that of
Davis and Kelly (1997), whose “experiments examined the vocabulary component of
language and [...] did not find sensitive period effects” (p. 457). They add that although
“one’s ability to learn the phonology and syntax of a new language later in life would be
compromised, vocabulary acquisition is much more open-ended as new words are
encountered throughout life. Thus there is a clear advantage to maintaining the ability to
learn words” (p. 458).

Now let’s examine syntax and grammar. When J.M. arrived in the U.S. at the age of 18,
his level of English syntax and grammar was already quite advanced. Great importance
was placed on these two aspects of the language since he started learning it in Spain; in
addition, due to similarities in syntax and grammar between English and Spanish, he didn’t
have much difficulty reaching an advanced level quickly. This is why when he arrived in
the U.S. there was no significant improvement observed, which we could have used to
investigate whether there are critical age effects on syntax and grammar and whether
syntax and grammar can be acquired after the critical period. However, we should keep in
mind that J.M. started learning English at the age of 11. If there were a critical period for

2
syntax and grammar, J.M. would not have been able to reach a native level. This
conclusion is in agreement with Scovel (1998, cited in Gitterman, 1999), according to
whom there is no support for a critical period for syntax. Nevertheless, this conclusion is
not in agreement with Johnson et al. (1996) who concluded through discussion of
experiments that grammatical performance of adult learners differs from that of native
speakers qualitatively (i.e. not simply quantitatively). However, the results obtained by
Johnson et al. may not be accurate, since, as they state in their study, “adult learners
show a substantial amount of inconsistency in their judgments of grammaticality, and thus
their performance is not well modeled by any simple or entirely deterministic model of
underlying knowledge” (p.349).

So far we have examined vocabulary, syntax and grammar. But what happens when it
comes to accent? Is there a critical period, after which one cannot learn to sound like a
native speaker? Grimsaw et al (1998) examined the case of E.M., a man profoundly deaf
since birth, who was provided with hearing aids at the age of fifteen. The language he
acquired after that age was characterised by severe deficits in both verbal comprehension
and production. This result favors the critical period hypothesis for first language
acquisition. Gitterman (1999) examined Grimshaw et al.’s study and investigated whether
the same conclusion can be drawn for second language acquisition. He concluded that
there is certainly a critical period for phonological skills in second language acquisition.
This conclusion agrees with that of Hakuta (1986, cited in Gitterman, 1999) and Scovel
(1998, cited in Gitterman, 1999); “Scovel asserts that a loss of neuroplasticity in the brain
of adults is the likely reason for the existence of a critical period for speech" (Gitterman,
1999, p. 378). In addition to Gitterman and Hakuta, Munro et al. (1996) also support the
notion that there is a critical period for accent in second language acquisition. Munro et al.
examined the hypothesis that production of English vowels depends on the age at which
the second language is acquired. By examining English vowel production by native Italian
speakers (but who had been living in an English-speaking environment for years), Munro
et al. found that the presence of accent increased with age of arrival at the English-
speaking environment. According to Long (1990) who studied the dependence of second
language learning on age of acquisition, starting after age six appears to make it
impossible for many learners to achieve native-like competence in phonology. Long

3
attributes this lack of ability to achieve native-like competence in phonology to the loss of
brain plasticity which happens with maturation. But what is that critical age, after which
native-like competence in phonology is difficult to achieve? Johnson and Newport (1989)
found that “performance gradually declined from about age seven on, until adulthood” (p.
95) (i.e. the decline begins far before puberty). Claude Hagège (1996), a French linguist,
supports that the critical age is eleven years. Up to that age, he argues, the child can
receive foreign sounds and the mouth can articulate them by imitation. At the age of 11
foreign sounds start being “filtered”, i.e. the child is no longer sensitive to sounds that do
not exist in his native language. Lenneberg (1967, cited in Harley, 1995) suggested that
the critical period ends at puberty.

The majority of studies seem to arrive at the conclusion that there is a critical period for
second language acquisition as regards phonological skills. However, Bialystok (1997)
argues that the evidence for a critical period for second language acquisition is not
convincing. According to Bialystok, young learners acquire a second language with greater
success than adult learners, but he attributes this success not to the existence of a critical
period, but to other factors (such as the time one dedicates to language learning and
motivation). Let’s examine J.M.’s case again, this time from a phonological perspective.
When J.M. arrived in the U.S., he could hardly be understood; his accent was far from
native. However, he had no difficulty at all in extracting (understanding) phonological
information. For example, he pronounced the words “law” and “low” in exactly the same
manner, although he could distinguish between the two when he heard them. Similarly, he
produced / I / and /i/ in the same way (thus making no distinction between “feet” and “fit”),
but had no trouble distinguishing the two when a native English speaker uttered them.
Within a year after his arrival in the US, a significant improvement was observed. Although
J.M. still did not sound like a native speaker, he could now be well understood. This
observation shows that a phonological improvement can be achieved even after the age of
eighteen. But is it possible to achieve native-like phonological competence after that age?
During the following two years (age 18-20), J.M. continued to show phonological
improvement, although this improvement was not significant (compared to the first year
after his immersion into the English-speaking environment). How can we explain this
decrease in phonological improvement? Can we argue then that 18 is the critical period for

4
second language acquisition? At the age of 22, J.M. consulted a speech pathologist and
had three one-hour sessions of “accent modification”. Tongue and lip positions required for
production of various sounds were clearly explained to him. By using a mirror, J.M. could
see whether he followed these mouth movements and compare them to those of the
speech pathologist. In addition, J.M.’s sounds were recorded and replayed, so that he
could observe himself where he needed improvement. After each session, J.M. practiced
the sounds that he had worked on with the speech pathologist. After the treatment was
finished, J.M. was able to produce the sounds he had worked on like a native speaker. For
example, he could now produce the words “law” and “low” in a native-like manner.
Unfortunately, the number of accent-modification sessions was not sufficient for an
improvement in all of J.M.’s phonological difficulties. With more sessions and more
practice, J.M. would likely have reached native-like phonological skills.

The answer to the question I posed earlier, i.e. how can we explain the decrease in J.M.’s
phonological improvement, I believe is straightforward: he stopped trying; he did not
dedicate any time or effort to practicing English sounds after his treatment was over. This
conclusion is in perfect agreement with Bialystok (1997), according to whom adults can
achieve the same success as children if they dedicate time and effort to language
learning. As noted earlier, the decrease in J.M.’s phonological improvement started at the
age of 18. Could this imply that 18 is the critical age? This contradicts most of the studies
conducted on this subject, according to which this age is far beyond the critical period.
However, this finding seems not to be in disagreement with Bialystok, who argues that the
level where language noticeably declines is around the age of 20.

One could argue that J.M.’s case is an exception, and that we should not draw
conclusions about what the critical period is based on this case. Therefore I will not use
the study of J.M. as evidence to show that the critical period extends beyond the age of
18. I will rather use these findings to argue that the critical period is not a value common to
each individual, but that it changes from person to person. Thus, although Claude Hagège
argues that the age of eleven is a threshold for second language acquisition (and other
researchers have suggested other critical ages), I argue that there is a critical period that
differs among individuals, and that the loss of neuroplasticity is one of many factors (and
certainly not the most important) that determine it. Other factors are motivation/need for

5
communication, repetition, encouragement, relation between the native and the second
language (shared features) etc.

Let’s examine how these factors affected J.M.’s phonological progress. The effect of
motivation/need for communication has already been discussed; the need for
communication was a very important factor in J.M.’s progress during the first year after his
arrival in the United States. I believe that repetition also played a very important role during
that first year. J.M. was now able to listen to English sounds repeatedly, and when he had
doubts about the exact pronunciation of a word, he could easily verify it. As far as
encouragement is concerned, it is a necessary factor so that an adult can practice
producing different sounds exactly like a child does, without fear that he could make a
mistake. Claude Hagège (1996) discusses this fear and considers it to be a big obstacle in
an adult’s phonological progress. A child does not have a fear of being laughed at, if he
does not pronounce a word correctly. An adult, on the other hand, is often afraid that he
may produce sounds that will not sound native-like (for example, that he may over-aspirate
a /p/ in the beginning of a word), and consequently he chooses to retain the sounds of his
native language. J.M. certainly had that fear, but he made a great effort to overcome it
when he realized that it was an obstacle to his being understood. J.M.’s speech
pathologist gave him the encouragement he needed, as well as the option of trying to
produce a particular sound many times until he produced it in a native-like manner. As
regards non-shared features between English and Spanish, there are many. This is why
J.M. had to put great effort into learning to produce new sounds. A bigger number of
shared phonological features would facilitate the attainment of native-like pronunciation.
This conclusion is in agreement with Bialystok (1997) who states that “the hypothesis is
that language learners will find it difficult to master a structure that was not a defining
feature of the first language and relatively easy to master a structure shared across the
two languages. These differences may be exacerbated for older learners, but there should
be no age differences in the ability to learn structures that are shared across the two
languages” (p. 126). Although this statement does not refer specifically to accent, it is
evident that it applies to accent, since no extra effort is needed to produce, in a second
language, sounds that already have been mastered in one’s first language.

6
The examination of J.M.’s case in relation to the research conducted by linguists in second
language acquisition, shows that there is no critical period for vocabulary, syntax and
grammar, but that there is a critical period for phonological skills. This period is different
among individuals, and is determined by several factors (and not simply by a loss of
neuroplasticity), such as motivation, repetition, encouragement, and shared phonological
features between one’s first and second language.

7
REFERENCES
Bialystok, E. (1997). The structure of age: in search of barriers to second language
acquisition. Second Language Research, 13, 116-137.
Davis, S. M., & Kelly, M. H. (1997). Knowledge of the English noun-verb stress difference
by native and nonnative speakers. Journal of Memory and Language, 36, 445-460.
Gitterman, M. R. (1999). The critical period: some thoughts on Grimshaw et al. (1998).
Brain and Language, 66, 377-381.
Grimshaw, G. M., Adelstein, A., Bryden, M. P., & MacKinnon, G. E. (1998). First-language
acquisition in adolescence: evidence for a critical period for verbal language development.
Brain and Language, 63, 237-255.
Hagège, C. (1996). L’enfant aux deux langues. Paris, France: Editions Odile Jacob.
Harley, T. (1995). The Psychology of Language: From Data to Theory. Coventry, UK:
Psychology Press.
Hurford, J. R. (1991). The evolution of the critical period for language acquisition.
Cognition, 40, 159-201.
Johnson, J. S., Shenkman, K. D., Newport, E. L. & Medin, D. L. (1996). Indeterminacy in
the grammar of adult language learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 335-352.
Johnson, J. S. & Newport, E. L. (1989). Critical period effects in second language learning:
the influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language.
Cognitive Psychology, 21, 60-69.
Long, M. (1990). Maturational constraints on language development. Studies in second
language acquisition, 12, 251-285.
Marcotte, A. C., & Morere, D.A., (1990). Speech lateralization in deaf populations:
Evidence for a developmental critical period. Brain and Language, 39, 134-152.
Munro, M. J., Flege, J. E., & Mackay, I. R. A. (1996). The effects of age of second
language learning on the production of English vowels. Applied Psycholinguistics, 17, 313-
334.

You might also like